Hostname: page-component-8448b6f56d-tj2md Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-04-23T20:56:40.758Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

The processing of dialectal variants: Further insight from French

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  14 November 2018

SOPHIE DUFOUR*
Affiliation:
Aix-Marseille Université
YU-YING CHUANG
Affiliation:
University of Tübingen
NOËL NGUYEN
Affiliation:
Aix-Marseille Université
*
ADDRESS FOR CORRESPONDENCE Sophie Dufour, Laboratoire Parole et Langage, Aix-Marseille Université, CNRS, 5 avenue Pasteur 13604 Aix-en-Provence, France. E-mail: sophie.dufour@lpl-aix.fr

Abstract

In two semantic priming experiments, this study examined how southern French speakers process the standard French [o] variant in closed syllables in comparison to their own variant [ɔ]. In Experiment 1, southern French speakers showed facilitation in the processing of the associated target word VIOLET whether the word prime mauve was pronounced by a standard French speaker ([mov]) or a southern French speaker ([mɔv]). More importantly, Experiment 1 has also revealed that words of type mauve, which are subject to dialectal variation, behave exactly in the same way as words of type gomme, which are pronounced with [ɔ] by both southern and standard French speakers, and for which we also found no modulation in the magnitude of the priming effect as a function of the dialect of the speaker. Experiment 2 replicated the priming effect found with the standard French variant [mov], and failed to show a priming effect with nonwords such as [mœv] that also differ from the southern French variant [mɔv] by only one phonetic feature. Our study thus provides further evidence for efficient processing of dialectal variants during spoken word recognition, even if these variants are not part of the speaker’s own productions.

Type
Original Article
Copyright
© Cambridge University Press 2018 

Access options

Get access to the full version of this content by using one of the access options below. (Log in options will check for institutional or personal access. Content may require purchase if you do not have access.)

References

REFERENCES

Baayen, R. H., Davidson, D. J., & Bates, D. M. (2008). Mixed-effects modeling with crossed random effects for subjects and items. Journal of Memory and Language, 59, 390412.Google Scholar
Baayen, R. H., & Milin, P. (2010). Analyzing reaction times. International Journal of Psychological Research, 3, 1228.Google Scholar
Barr, D. J., Levy, R., Scheepers, C., & Tily, H. J. (2013). Random effects structure for confirmatory hypothesis testing: Keep it maximal. Journal of Memory and Language, 68, 255278.Google Scholar
Bates, D. M., & Sarkar, D. (2007). lme4: Linear mixed-effects models using S4 classes. R package version 2.6. Retrieved November 20, 2017, from http://lme4.r-forge.r-project.org/Google Scholar
Best, C. T., McRoberts, G. W., & Sithole, N. M. (1988). Examination of perceptual reorganization for nonnative speech contrasts: Zulu click discrimination by English-speaking adults and infants. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 14, 345360.Google Scholar
Connine, C. M. (2004). It’s not what you hear, but how often you hear it: On the neglected role of phonological variant frequency in auditory word recognition. Psychonomic Bulletin & Review, 11, 10841089.Google Scholar
Connine, C. M., Blasko, D. G., & Titone, D. (1993). Do the beginnings of spoken words have a special status in auditory word recognition? Journal of Memory and Language, 32, 193210.Google Scholar
Connine, C. M., Ranbom, L. J., & Patterson, D. J. (2008). Processing variant forms in spoken word recognition: The role of variant frequency. Perception & Psychophysics, 70, 403411.Google Scholar
Conrey, B., Potts, G., & Niedzielski, N. (2005). Effects of dialect on merger perception: ERP and behavioral correlates. Brain and Language, 95, 435449.Google Scholar
Dahan, D., Drucker, S. J., & Scarborough, R. A. (2008). Talker adaptation in speech perception: Adjusting the signal or the representations? Cognition, 108, 710718.Google Scholar
Dufour, S., Brunellière, A., & Nguyen, N. (2013). To what extent do we hear phonemic contrasts in a non-native regional variety? Tracking the dynamics of perceptual processing with EEG. Journal of Psycholinguistic Research, 42, 161173.Google Scholar
Dufour, S., Nguyen, N., & Frauenfelder, U. H. (2007). The perception of phonemic contrasts in a non-native dialect. Journal of Acoustical Society of America, 121, EL131EL136.Google Scholar
Dufour, S., Nguyen, N., & Frauenfelder, U. H. (2010). Does training on a phonemic contrast absent in the listener’s dialect influence word recognition? Journal of the Acoustical Society of America, 128, EL43EL48.Google Scholar
Dufour, S., Nguyen, N., Pattamadilok, C., & Frauenfelder, U. H. (2016). Does orthographic training on a phonemic contrast absent in the listener’s dialect influence word recognition? Journal of the Acoustical Society of America, 140, 18711877.Google Scholar
Dufour, S., Peereman, R., Pallier, C., & Radeau, M. (2002). VoCoLex: A lexical database on phonological similarity between French words. L’Année Psychologique, 102, 725746.Google Scholar
Ferrand, L., & Alario, F.-X. (1998). Normes d’association verbales pour 366 d’objets concrets. L’Année Psychologique, 98, 659709.Google Scholar
Frauenfelder, U. H., Scholten, M., & Content, A. (2001). Bottom-up inhibition in lexical selection: Phonological mismatch effects in spoken word recognition. Language and Cognitive Processes, 16, 583607.Google Scholar
Gaskell, M. G., & Marslen-Wilson, W. D. (1996). Phonological variation and inference in lexical access. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 22, 144158.Google Scholar
Goldinger, S. D. (1998). Echoes of echoes? An episodic theory of lexical access. Psychological Review, 105, 251279.Google Scholar
Hintzman, D. L. (1986). “Schema abstraction” in a multiple-trace memory model. Psychological Review, 93, 411428.Google Scholar
Hintzman, D. L. (1988). Judgments of frequency and recognition memory in a multiple-trace memory model. Psychological Review, 95, 528551.Google Scholar
Ingram, J. C. L., & Park, S. G. (1997). Cross-language vowel perception and production by Japanese and Korean learners of English. Journal of Phonetics, 25, 437470.Google Scholar
Janson, T., & Schulman, R. (1983). Non-distinctive features and their use. Journal of Linguistics, 19, 321336.Google Scholar
Labov, W., Karan, M., & Miller, C. (1991). Near-mergers and the suspension of phonemic contrast. Language Variation and Change, 3, 3374.Google Scholar
Larraza, S., Samuel, A. G., & Oñederra, M. L. (2016). Listening to accented speech in a second language: First language and age of acquisition effects. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 42, 17741797.Google Scholar
Larraza, S., Samuel, A. G., & Oñederra, M. L. (2017). Where do dialectal effects on speech processing come from? Evidence from a cross-dialect investigation. Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 70, 92108.Google Scholar
Marslen-Wilson, W. D., Moss, H. E., & van Halen, S. (1996). Perceptual distance and competition in lexical access. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 22, 13761392.Google Scholar
Marslen-Wilson, W. D., & Warren, P. (1994). Levels of perceptual representation and process in lexical access: Words, phonemes, and features. Psychological Review, 101, 653675.Google Scholar
Marslen-Wilson, W. D., & Welsh, A. (1978). Processing interaction and lexical access during word recognition in continuous speech. Cognitive Psychology, 10, 2963.Google Scholar
Maye, J., Aslin, R. N., & Tanenhaus, M. K. (2008). The weckud wetch of the wast: Lexical adaptation to a novel accent. Cognitive Science, 32, 543562.Google Scholar
McClelland, J. L., & Elman, J. L. (1986). The TRACE model of speech perception. Cognitive Psychology, 18, 186.Google Scholar
Miyawaki, K., Strange, W., Verbrugge, R., Liberman, A. M., Jenkins, J. J., & Fujimura, O. (1975). An effect of linguistic experience: The discrimination of /r/ and /l/ by native listeners of Japanese and English. Perception and Psychophysics, 18, 331340.Google Scholar
Näätänen, R., Lehtokoski, A., Lennes, M., Cheour, M., Huotilainen, M., Iivonen, A., … Alho, K. (1997). Language-specific phoneme representations revealed by electric and magnetic brain responses. Nature, 358, 432434.Google Scholar
Norris, D. (1994). Shortlist: A connectionist model of continuous speech recognition. Cognition, 52, 189234.Google Scholar
Norris, D., McQueen, J. M., & Cutler, A. (2003). Perceptual learning in speech. Cognitive Psychology, 47, 204238.Google Scholar
Pierrehumbert, J. (2001). Why phonological constraints are so coarse-grained. Language & Cognitive Processes, 16, 691698.Google Scholar
Racine, I., Bürki, A., & Spinelli, E. (2014). The implication of spelling and frequency in the recognition of phonological variants: Evidence from pre-readers and readers. Language, Cognition and Neuroscience, 29, 893898.Google Scholar
Ranbom, L. J., & Connine, C. M. (2007). Lexical representation of phonological variation in spoken word recognition. Journal of Memory and Language, 57, 273298.Google Scholar
R Development Core Team. (2007). R: A language and environment for statistical computing. Vienna, Austria: R Foundation for Statistical Computing. Retrieved from http://www.R-project.orgGoogle Scholar
Snoeren, N., Segui, J., & Hallé, P. (2008). On the role of regular phonological variation in lexical access: Evidence from voice assimilation in French. Cognition, 108, B512B521.Google Scholar
Sumner, M., & Samuel, A. G. (2009). The effect of experience on the perception and representation of dialect variants. Journal of Memory and Language, 60, 487501.Google Scholar
Trehub, S. (1976). The discrimination of foreign speech contrasts by infants and adults. Child Development, 47, 466472.Google Scholar
Trude, A. M., & Brown-Schmidt, S. (2012) Talker-specific perceptual adaptation during on-line speech perception. Language and Cognitive Processes, 27, 9791001.Google Scholar
Werker, J. F., Gilbert, J. H. V., Humphrey, G. K., & Tees, R. C. (1981). Developmental aspects of cross-language speech perception. Child Development, 52, 349355.Google Scholar
Werker, J. F., & Tees, R. C. (1984). Cross-language speech perception: Evidence for perceptual reorganization during the first year of life. Infant Behavior and Development, 7, 4963.Google Scholar
Zhang, X., Samuel, A. G., & Liu, S. (2012). The perception and representation of segmental and prosodic Mandarin contrasts in native speakers of Cantonese. Journal of Memory and Language, 66, 438457.Google Scholar