Hostname: page-component-7c8c6479df-xxrs7 Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-03-28T14:00:29.178Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

The case of case: Children's knowledge and use of upper- and lowercase letters

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  23 June 2004

REBECCA TREIMAN
Affiliation:
Washington University in St. Louis
BRETT KESSLER
Affiliation:
Washington University in St. Louis

Abstract

Research on children's spelling has focused on its phonological bases. In the present study, we examined a type of nonphonological knowledge that even young children may possess—knowledge about the distinction between upper- and lowercase letters. In Study 1, we analyzed the capitalization patterns used by children in kindergarten through second grade on words that did not contain a capital letter in their conventional spellings. The younger children, especially, often wrote with capital letters. They did so in a nonrandom way, being more likely to capitalize word-initial letters than later letters. When children inserted an uppercase letter in a noninitial position of a spelling, it tended to be a letter whose uppercase form was especially familiar to the child, the initial letter of the child's first name. In Study 2, which examined kindergartners' knowledge of the names of upper- and lowercase letters, we found further evidence that children's names influence their knowledge about letters and that some of this knowledge is case specific. Together, the results show that early spelling involves more than phonology.

Type
Articles
Copyright
© 2004 Cambridge University Press

Access options

Get access to the full version of this content by using one of the access options below. (Log in options will check for institutional or personal access. Content may require purchase if you do not have access.)

References

Bissex G. L. 1980. Gnys at wrk. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press.
Bloodgood J. W. 1999 What's in a name? Children's name writing and literacy acquisition. Reading Research Quarterly, 34, 342367.Google Scholar
Cassar M. & Treiman R. 1997 The beginnings of orthographic knowledge: Children's knowledge of double letters in words. Journal of Educational Psychology, 89, 631644.Google Scholar
Evans M. A., Shaw D., Moretti S., & Page J. 2003. Letter names, letter sounds and phonological awareness: An examination of kindergarten children across letters and of letters across children. Manuscript submitted for publication.
Ferreiro E. & Teberosky A. 1982. Literacy before schooling. New York: Heinemann.
Frith U. 1985. Beneath the surface of developmental dyslexia. In K. E. Patterson, J. C. Marshall, & M. Coltheart (Eds.), Surface dyslexia: Neuropsychological and cognitive studies of phonological reading (pp. 301330). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.
Gentry J. R. 1982 An analysis of developmental spelling in GNYS AT WRK. Reading Teacher, 36, 192200.Google Scholar
Gombert J. E. & Fayol M. 1992 Writing in preliterate children. Learning and Instruction, 2, 2341.Google Scholar
Levin I., & Aram D. (in press). Children's names contribute to early literacy: A linguistic and a social perspective. In D. Ravid & H. Bat–Zeev Shyldkrot (Eds.), Perspectives on language and language development. Dordrecht: Kluwer.
Lombardino L. J., Morris D., Mercado L., DeFillipo F., Sarisky C., & Montgomery A. 1999 The Early Reading Screening Instrument: A method for identifying kindergarteners at risk for learning to read. International Journal of Language & Communication Disorders, 34, 135150.Google Scholar
Read C. 1971 Pre-school children's knowledge of English phonology. Harvard Educational Review, 41, 134.Google Scholar
Read C. 1975. Children's categorization of speech sounds in English (NCTE Research Report No. 17). Urbana, IL: National Council of Teachers of English.
Read C. 1986. Children's creative spelling. London: Routledge & Kegan Paul.
Reece C. & Treiman R. 2001 Children's spelling of syllabic /r/ and of letter-name vowels: Broadening the study of spelling development. Applied Psycholinguistics, 22, 139165.Google Scholar
Torrey J. W. 1993. Learning to read without a teacher: A case study. In F. Smith (Ed.), Psycholinguistics and reading (pp. 147157). New York: Holt, Rinehart & Winston.
Treiman R. 1993. Beginning to spell: A study of first-grade children. New York: Oxford University Press.
Treiman R., Berch D., Tincoff R., & Weatherston S. 1993 Phonology and spelling: The case of syllabic consonants. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 56, 267290.Google Scholar
Treiman R., Berch D. & Weatherston S. 1993 Children's use of phoneme–grapheme correspondences in spelling: Roles of position and stress. Journal of Educational Psychology, 85, 112.Google Scholar
Treiman R. & Bourassa D. 2000 Children's written and oral spelling. Applied Psycholinguistics, 21, 183204.Google Scholar
Treiman R. & Broderick V. 1998 What's in a name: Children's knowledge about the letters in their own names. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 70, 97116.Google Scholar
Treiman R., Cassar M., & Zukowski A. 1994 What types of linguistic information do children use in spelling? The case of flaps. Child Development, 65, 13101329.Google Scholar
Treiman R. & Kessler B. 2003. The role of letter names in the acquisition of literacy. In R. Kail (Ed.), Advances in Child Development and Behavior (Vol. 31, pp. 105135). San Diego, CA: Academic Press.
Treiman R., Kessler B., & Bourassa D. 2001 Children's own names influence their spelling. Applied Psycholinguistics, 22, 555570.Google Scholar