Hostname: page-component-8448b6f56d-c4f8m Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-04-23T06:26:13.885Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

Sex differences in language competence of 4-year-old children: Female advantages are mediated by phonological short-term memory

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  16 August 2021

Benjamin P. Lange*
Affiliation:
Department of Social Sciences, IU International University of Applied Sciences, Berlin, Germany Department of Media Psychology, Julius Maximilian University of Wuerzburg, Wuerzburg, Germany
Eugen Zaretsky
Affiliation:
Department of Phoniatrics and Paediatric Audiology, Marburg University Hospital, Marburg, Germany
*
*Corresponding author. Email: benjamin.lange@iu.org

Abstract

For some time now, psycholinguistic research has involved the study of sex differences in language development. Overall, girls seem to have an early advantage over boys, mainly in regard to vocabulary, which appears to decrease and, eventually, vanish with age. While there are numerous studies on sex differences in the acquisition of vocabulary as well as grammar, early sex differences in phonological short-term memory (PSTM) have been mostly neglected, or if research was conducted, it resulted in null findings, for the most part. In the present study, we examined sex differences in language competence (in a wide array of linguistic domains) of German children 4 years of age. Several tests were administered to assess articulation, vocabulary, grammar, speech comprehension, and, most importantly, PSTM (by means of the repetition of non-words and sentences). Girls performed better than boys in all domains, although some effect sizes were small. Most importantly, we found evidence for a female advantage in PSTM performance. Furthermore, mediation analyses revealed that the obtained sex differences in articulation, vocabulary, grammar, and comprehension were partially or fully mediated by (sex differences in) PSTM.

Type
Original Article
Copyright
© The Author(s), 2021. Published by Cambridge University Press

Access options

Get access to the full version of this content by using one of the access options below. (Log in options will check for institutional or personal access. Content may require purchase if you do not have access.)

References

Abdi, H. (2007). Bonferroni and Sidak corrections for multiple comparisons. In Salkind, N. J. (Ed.), Encyclopedia of measurement and statistics (pp. 103107). Sage.Google Scholar
Adams, K. A., Marchman, V. A., Loi, E. C., Ashland, M. D., Fernald, A., & Feldman, H. M. (2018). Caregiver talk and medical risk as predictors of language outcomes in full term and preterm toddlers. Child Development, 89(5), 16741690.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Adani, S., & Cepanec, M. (2019). Sex differences in early communication development: behavioral and neurobiological indicators of more vulnerable communication system development in boys. Croatian Medical Journal, 60(2), 141149. https://doi.org/10.3325/cmj.2019.60.141 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Adlof, S. M., & Patten, H. (2017). Nonword repetition and vocabulary knowledge as predictors of children’s phonological and semantic word learning. Journal of Speech, Language, and Hearing Research, 60(3), 682693.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Andrade, J., & Baddeley, A. (2011). The contribution of phonological short-term memory to artificial grammar learning. The Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 64(5), 960974.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Ardila, A., Rosselli, M., Matute, E., & Inozemtseva, O. (2011). Gender differences in cognitive development. Developmental Psychology, 47(4), 984990. https://doi.org/10.1037/a002381.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Ash, A. C., Redmond, S. M., Timler, G. R., & Kean, J. (2017). The influence of scale structure and sex on parental reports of children’s social (pragmatic) communication symptoms. Clinical Linguistics & Phonetics, 31(4), 293312. https://doi.org/10.1080/02699206.2016.1257655 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Baddeley, A. D. (2003). Working memory and language: an overview. Journal of Communication Disorders, 36, 189208. https://doi.org/10.1016/S0021-9924(03)00019-4 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Baddeley, A. D., Gathercole, S., & Papagno, C. (1998). The phonological loop as a language learning device. Psychological Review, 105(1), 158–73. https://doi.org/10.1037/0033-295X.105.1.158 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Baddeley, A. D., & Hitch, G. (1974). Working Memory. Psychology of Learning and Motivation: Advances in Research and Theory, 8, 4789.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Bauer, D. J., Goldfield, B. A., & Reznick, J. S. (2002). Alternative approaches to analyzing individual differences in the rate of early vocabulary development. Applied Psycholinguistics, 23(3), 313335. https://doi.org/10.1017.S0142716402003016 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Befi-Lopes, D. M., Pereira, A. C., & Bento, A. C. (2010). Phonological representation of children with Specific Language Impairment (SLI). Pró-Fono Revista de Atualização Científica, 22(3), 305310. https://doi.org/10.1590/S0104-56872010000300025 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Beltz, A. M., Blakemore, J. E. O., & Berenbaum, S. A. (2013). Sex differences in brain and behavioural development. In Tager-Flusberg, H., Rakic, P., & Rubenstein, J. (Eds.), Comprehensive developmental neuroscience: Vol. 3. Neural circuit development and function in the healthy and diseased brain (pp. 467499). Oxford, UK: Elsevier.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Berglund, E., Eriksson, M., & Westerlund, M. (2005). Communicative skills in relation to gender, birth order, childcare and socioeconomic status in 18-month-old children. Scandinavian Journal of Psychology, 46, 485491. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1467-9450.2005.00480.x CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Bishop, D. V. M., North, T., & Donlan, C. (1996). Nonword repetition as a behavioural marker for inherited language impairment: Evidence from a twin study. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 37(4), 391403.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Bleses, D., Vach, W., Slott, M., Wehberg, S., Thomsen, P., Madsen, T. O., & Basbøll, H. (2008). The Danish Communicative Developmental Inventories: Validity and main developmental trends. Journal of Child Language, 35, 651669. https://doi.org/10.1017/S0305000907008574 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Bornstein, M. H., Hahn, C.-S., & Haynes, O. M. (2004). Specific and general language performance across early childhood: Stability and gender considerations. First Language, 24, 267304. https://doi.org/10.1177/0142723704045681 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Bouchard, C., Trudeau, N., Sutton, A., Boudreault, M.-C., & Deneault, J. (2009). Gender differences in language development in French Canadian children between 8 and 30 months of age. Applied Psycholinguistics, 30(4), 685707. https://doi.org/10.1017/S0142716409990075 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Burt, L., Holm, A., & Dodd, B. (1999). Phonological awareness skills of 4-year-old British children: An assessment and developmental data. International Journal of Language and Communication Disorders, 34(3), 311335. https://doi.org/10.1080/136828299247432 Google ScholarPubMed
Casserly, E. D., & Pisoni, D.B. (2013). Nonword repetition as a predictor of long-term speech and language skills in children with cochlear implants. Otology & Neurotology, 34(3), 460470.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Chaney, C. (1992). Language development, metalinguistic skills, and print awareness in 3-year-old children. Applied Psycholinguistics, 13(4), 485514.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Chiat, S., & Roy, P. (2007). The preschool repetition test: An evaluation of performance in typically developing and clinically referred children. Journal of Speech, Language, and Hearing Research, 50, 429443.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Chiat, S., & Roy, P. (2013). Early predictors of language and social communication impairments at ages 9-11 years: A follow-up study of early-referred children. Journal of Speech, Language, and Hearing Research, 56(6), 18241836.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Coady, J. A., & Evans, J. L. (2008). Uses and interpretations of non-word repetition tasks in children with and without specific language impairments (SLI). International Journal of Language & Communication Disorders, 43(1), 140. https://doi.org/10.1080/13682820601116485 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Cohen, J. (1988). Statistical power analysis for the behavioral sciences (2nd ed.). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.Google Scholar
Collette, F., Van der Linden, M., & Poncelet, M. (2000). Working memory, long-term memory and language processing: Issues and future directions. Brain and Language, 71, 4651.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Conti-Ramsden, G., & Durkin, K. (2012). Language development and assessment in the preschool period. Neuropsychology Review, 22, 384401. https://doi.org/10.1007/s11065-012-9208-z CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Dickinson, D. K., & Porche, M. V. (2011). Relation between language experiences in preschool classrooms and children’s kindergarten and fourth-grade language and reading abilities. Child Development, 82(3), 870886.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
D’Odorico, L., Assanelli, A., Franco, F., & Jacob, V. (2007). A follow-up study on Italian late talkers: Development of language, short-term memory, phonological awareness, impulsiveness, and attention. Applied Psycholinguistics, 28(1), 157169. https://doi.org/10.1017.S0142716406070081 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Eriksson, M., Marschik, P. B., Tulviste, T., Almgren, M., Pérez Pereira, M., Wehberg, S., Marjanovič-Umek, L., Gayraud, F., Kovacevic, M., & Gallego, C. (2012). Differences between girls and boys in emerging language skills: Evidence from 10 language communities. British Journal of Developmental Psychology, 30, 326343. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.2044-835X.2011.02042.x CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Estes, K., Evans, J. L., & Else-Quest, N. M. (2007). Differences in the nonword repetition performance of children with and without specific language impairment: A meta-analysis. Journal of Speech, Language, and Hearing Research, 50, 177195.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Euler, H. A., Holler-Zittlau, I., van Minnen, S., Sick, U., Dux, W., Zaretsky, Y., & Neumann, K., (2012). Psychometrische Gütekriterien eines Kurztests zur Erfassung des Sprachstands 4-jähriger Kinder [Validity criteria of a short test to assess speech and language competence in 4-year-olds]. HNO, 58(11), 11161123.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Farmani, H., Sayyahi, F., Soleymani, Z., Zadeh Labbaf, F., Talebi, E., & Shourvazi, Z. (2018). Normalization of the non-word repetition test in Farsi-speaking children. Journal of Modern Rehabilitation, 12(4), 217224. https://doi.org/10.32598/JMR.V12.N4.217 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Feldman, H. M., Dollaghan, C. A., Campbell, T. F., Kurs-Lasky, M., Janosky, J. E., & Paradise, J. L. (2000). Measurement properties of the MacArthur communicative development inventories at ages one and two years. Child Development, 71, 310322.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Fenson, L., Dale, P. S., Reznick, J. S., Bates, E., Thal, D. J., Pethick, S. J., Tomasello, M. , Mervis, C. B., & Stiles, J. (1994). Variability in early communicative development. Monographs of the Society for Research in Child Development, 59, i, iii–v, 1185. https://doi.org/10.2307/1166093 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Fivush, R., Brotman, M. A., Buckner, J. P., & Goodman, S. H. (2000). Gender differences in parent–child emotion narratives. Sex Roles, 42(3/4), 233253.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Galsworthy, M. J., Dionne, G., Dale, P. S., & Plomin, R. (2000). Sex differences in early verbal and non-verbal cognitive development. Developmental Science, 3, 206215. https://doi.org/10.1111/1467-7687.00114 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Gathercole, S. E., & Baddeley, A. D. (1990a). The role of phonological memory in vocabulary acquisition: A study of young children learning new names. British Journal of Psychology, 81(4), 439454.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Gathercole, S. E., & Baddeley, A. D. (1990b). Phonological memory deficits in language disordered children: Is there a causal connection? Journal of Memory and Language, 29(3), 336360.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Gathercole, S. E., Dunning, D. L., Holmes, J., & Norris, D. (2019). Working memory training involves learning new skills. Journal of Memory and Language, 105, 1942.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Girbau, D., & Schwartz, R. G. (2008). Phonological working memory in Spanish-English bilingual children with and without specific language impairment. Journal of Communication Disorders, 41(2), 124145.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Gray, S. (2003). Diagnostic accuracy and test-retest reliability of nonword repetition and digit span tasks administered to preschool children with specific language impairment. Journal of Communication Disorders, 36, 129151. https://doi.org/ 10.1016/s0021-9924(03)00003-0 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Grimm, H. (2001). SETK 3-5. Sprachentwicklungstest für drei- bis fünfjährige Kinder. Hogrefe.Google Scholar
Hayes, A. F. (2018). Introduction to Mediation, Moderation, and Conditional Process Analysis, Second Edition (Methodology in the Social Sciences) (2nd ed.). Guilford Press.Google Scholar
Hayiou-Thomas, M. E., Dale, P. S., & Plomin, R. (2012). The etiology of variation in language skills changes with development: A longitudinal twin study of language from 2 to 12 years. Developmental Science, 15, 233249. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1467-7687.2011.01119.x CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Hoff, E. (2006). How social contexts support and shape language development. Developmental Review, 26, 5588.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Holler-Zittlau, I., Euler, H. A., & Neumann, K. (2011). Kindersprachscreening (KiSS) – das hessische Verfahren zur Sprachstandserfassung [Kindersprachscreening (KiSS) – the Hessian language test]. Sprachheilarbeit, 5(6), 263268.Google Scholar
Hurtado, N., Marchman, V. A., & Fernald, A. (2008). Does input influence uptake? Links between maternal talk, processing speed and vocabulary size in Spanish-learning children. Developmental Science, 11(6), F31F39. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1467-7687.2008.00768.x CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Huttenlocher, J., Vasilyeva, M., Cymerman, E., & Levine, S. (2002). Language input and child syntax. Cognitive Psychology, 45, 337374.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Jackson, E., Leitao, S., & Claessen, M. (2016). The relationship between phonological short-term memory, receptive vocabulary, and fast mapping in children with specific language impairment. International Journal of Language & Communication Disorders, 51(1), 6173.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Jones, G. (2016). The influence of children’s exposure to language from two to six years: The case of nonword repetition. Cognition, 153, 7988. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.cognition.2016.04.017 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Kapalková, S., Polišenská, K., & Vicenová, Z. (2013). Non-word repetition performance in Slovak-speaking children with and without SLI: novel scoring methods. International Journal of Language & Communication Disorders, 48(1), 7889. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1460-6984.2012.00189.x CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Karousou, A., & Nerantzaki, T. (in press). Phonological memory training and its effect on second language vocabulary development. Second Language Research. https://doi.org/10.1177/0267658319898514 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Kazemi, Y., & Saeednia, S. (2017). The clinical examination of non-word repetition tasks in identifying Persian-speaking children with primary language impairment. International Journal of Pediatric Otorhinolaryngology, 93, 712. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.ijporl.2016.11.028 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Kiese-Himmel, C., & Risse, T. (2009). Normen für den Mottier-Test bei 4- bis 6-jährigen Kindern [Norms for the Mottier Test for 4- to 6-year-old children]. HNO, 57, 943948.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Kovas, Y., Hayiou-Thomas, M. E., Oliver, B., Dale, P. S., Bishop, D. V. M., & Plomin, R. (2005). Genetic influences in different aspects of language development: The etiology of language skills in 4.5-year-old twins. Child Development, 76(3), 632651.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Krishnan, S., Alcock, K. J., Carey, D., Bergström, L., Karmiloff-Smith, A., & Dick, F. (2017). Fractionating nonword repetition: The contributions of short-term memory and oromotor praxis are different. PLoS One, 12(7), e0178356. https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0178356 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Lange, B. P. (2015). Digit ratio as a predictor of language development and media preferences in kindergarten children. Acta Linguistica, 9(2), 7083.Google Scholar
Lange, B. P., Euler, H. A., & Zaretsky, E. (2016). Sex differences in language competence of three- to six-year old children. Applied Psycholinguistics, 37(6), 14171438. https://doi.org/10.1017/S0142716415000624 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Lange, B. P., Zaretsky, E., Schwarz, S., & Euler, H. A. (2014). Words won’t fail: Experimental evidence on the role of verbal proficiency in mate choice. Journal of Language and Social Psychology, 33(5), 482499. https://doi.org/10.1177/0261927x13515886 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Leaper, C., Anderson, K. J., & Sanders, P. (1998). Moderators of gender effects on parents’ talk to their children: a meta-analysis. Developmental Psychology, 34(1), 327.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Letts, C., Edwards, S., Sinka, I., Schaefer, B., & Gibbons, W. (2013). Socio-economic status and language acquisition: Children’s performance on the new Reynell Developmental Language Scales. International Journal of Language & Communication Disorders, 48(2), 131143.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Lutchmaya, S., Baron-Cohen, S., & Raggatt, P. (2001). Foetal testosterone and vocabulary size in 18- and 24-month-old infants. Infant Behavior and Development, 24(4), 418424.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Martin, R. C., & Slevc, L. R. (2014). Language production and working memory. In Goldrick, M., Ferreira, V. & Miozzo, M. (Eds.), The Oxford handbook of language production (pp. 437450). Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Martinelli, V. (2013). Sex differences and variability in phonological sensitivity among primary school children. Malta Review of Educational Research, 7(1), 126.Google Scholar
Mealey, L. (2000). Sex differences: Development and evolutionary strategies. Academic Press.Google Scholar
Melby-Lervåg, M., & Hulme, C. (2013). Is working memory training effective? A meta-analytic review. Developmental Psychology, 49(2), 270291.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Montgomery, J. W., Magimairaj, B. M., & Finney, M. C. (2010). Working memory and specific language impairment: An update on the relation and perspectives on assessment and treatment. American Journal of Speech-Language Pathology, 19(1), 7894. https://doi.org/10.1044/1058-0360(2009/09-0028)CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Mottier, G. (1951). Über Untersuchungen der Sprache lesegestörter Kinder [On language tests with dyslexic children]. Folia Phoniatrica, 3, 170177.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Neugebauer, U., & Becker-Mrotzek, M. (2013). Die Qualität von Sprachstandsverfahren im Elementarbereich. Eine Analyse und Bewertung [The Quality of Language Tests for Pre-schoolers. Analysis and Evaluation. The Mercator Institute for Literacy and Language Education.Google Scholar
Newbury, J., Klee, T., Stokes, S. F., & Moran, C. (2015). Exploring expressive vocabulary variability in two-year-olds: The role of working memory. Journal of Speech, Language, and Hearing Research, 58, 17611772.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Nicolay, A. C., & Poncelet, M. (2013). Cognitive abilities underlying second-language vocabulary acquisition in an early second-language immersion education context: A longitudinal study. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 115(4), 655671.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Noel, M., Peterson, C., & Jesso, B. (2008). The relationship of parenting stress and child temperament to language development among economically disadvantaged preschoolers. Journal of Child Language, 35, 823843.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Pan, B. A., Rowe, M. L., Singer, J. D., & Snow, C. E. (2005). Maternal correlates of growth in toddler vocabulary production in low-income families. Child Development, 76(4), 763782.Google ScholarPubMed
Pancsofar, N., & Vernon-Feagans, L. (2006). Mother and father language input to young children: Contributions to later language development. Journal of Applied Developmental Psychology, 27(6), 571587.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Pelczarski, K. M., & Yaruss, J. S. (2016). Phonological memory in young children who stutter. Journal of Communication Disorders, 62, 5466. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jcomdis.2016.05.006 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Radeborg, K., Barthelom, E., Sjöberg, M. & Sahlén, B. (2006). A Swedish non-word repetition test for preschool children. Scandinavian Journal of Psychology, 47, 187192.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Ramachandra, V., Hewitt, L. E., & Brackenbury, T. (2011). The relationship between phonological memory, phonological sensitivity, and incidental word learning. Journal of Psycholinguistic Research, 40, 93109.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Raven, J.C. (2009). Coloured Progressive Matrices. Pearson Assessment and Information GmbH.Google Scholar
Reichenbach, K., Bastian, L., Rohrbach, S., Gross, M., & Sarrar, L. (2016). Cognitive functions in preschool children with specific language impairment. International Journal of Pediatric Otorhinolaryngology, 86, 2226.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Rice, M. L., & Hoffman, L. (2015). Predicting vocabulary growth in children with and without specific language impairment: A longitudinal study from 2;6 to 21 years of age. Journal of Speech, Language, and Hearing Research, 58, 345359.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Rowe, M. L. (2008). Child-directed speech: relation to socioeconomic status, knowledge of child development and child vocabulary skill. Journal of Child Language, 35(1), 185205. https://doi.org/10.1017/S0305000907008343 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Rowe, M. L. (2012). A longitudinal investigation of the role of quantity and quality of child-directed speech in vocabulary development. Child Development, 83(5), 17621774. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1467-8624.2012.01805.x CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Roy, P., & Chiat, S. (2004). A prosodically controlled word and nonword repetition task for 2- to 4-year-olds: Evidence from typically developing children. Journal of Speech, Language, and Hearing Research, 47, 223234.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Rujas, I., Mariscal, S., Casla, M., Lázaro, M., & Murillo, E. (2017). Word and nonword repetition abilities in Spanish language: Longitudinal evidence from typically developing and late talking children. The Spanish Journal of Psychology, 20, E72.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Schöler, H., & Brunner, M. (2008). Heidelberger auditives Screening in der Einschulungsuntersuchung (HASE) (2. überarbeitete und erweiterte Auflage) [Heidelberg Auditory Screening in the School-Enrolment Examination (HASE) (2nd revised and extended edition]. Westra.Google Scholar
Szmalec, A., Brysbaert, M., & Duyck, W. (2013). Working memory and (second) language processing. In Altarriba, J. & Isurin, L. (Eds.), Memory, language, and bilingualism: Theoretical and applied approaches (pp. 7494). Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
Stokes, S. F., & Klee, T. (2009). Factors that influence vocabulary development in two-year-old children. The Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 50(4), 498505. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1469-7610.2008.01991.x CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Stromswold, K. (2001). The heritability of language: A review and meta-analysis of twin, adoption, and linkage studies. Language, 77(4), 647723.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Tager-Flusberg, H. (2005). Designing studies to investigate the relationships between genes, environments, and developmental language disorders. Applied Psycholinguistics, 26, 2939.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Taylor, C. L., Christensen, D., Lawrence, D., Mitrou, F., & Zubrick, S. R. (2013). Risk factors for children’s receptive vocabulary development from four to eight years in the longitudinal study of Australian children. Plos One, 8(9), e73046.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Thomas, M. S. C., Forrester, N. A., & Ronald, A. (2013). Modeling socioeconomic status effects on language development. Developmental Psychology, 49(12), 23252343.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Toivainen, T., Papageorgiou, K. A., Tosto, M. G., & Kovas, Y. (2017). Sex differences in non-verbal and verbal abilities in childhood and adolescence. Intelligence, 64, 8188. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.intell.2017.07.007 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Topbaş, S., Kaçar-Kütükçü, D., & Kopkalli-Yavuz, H. (2014). Performance of children on the Turkish Nonword Repetition Test: Effect of word similarity, word length, and scoring. Clinical Linguistics & Phonetics, 28(7–8), 602616. https://doi.org/10.3109/02699206.2014.927003 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Torrington Eaton, C., Newman, R. S., Ratner, N. B., Rowe, M. L. (2015). Non-word repetition in 2-year-olds: Replication of an adapted paradigm and a useful methodological extension. Clinical Linguistics & Phonetics, 29(7), 523535. https://doi.org/10.3109/02699206.2015.1029594 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Truong, D. T., Shriberg, L. D., Smith, S. D., Chapman, K. L., Scheer-Cohen, A. R., DeMille, M. M., Adams, A. K., Nato, A. Q., Wijsman, E. M., Eicher, J. D., & Gruen, J. R. (2016). Multipoint genome-wide linkage scan for nonword repetition in a multigenerational family further supports chromosome 13q as a locus for verbal trait disorders. Human Genetics, 135(12), 13291341.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Tunmer, W. E., Bowey, J. A., & Grieve, R. (1983). The development of young children’s awareness of the word as a unit of spoken language. Journal of Psycholinguistic Research, 12(6), 567594.Google Scholar
Van Dyke, J. A. (2012). The role of memory in language and communication. In Peach, R. & Shapiro, L. (Eds.), Cognition and acquired language disorders: A process-oriented approach (pp. 94120). Mosby Elsevier.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Van Hulle, C. A., Goldsmith, H. H., & Lemery, K. S. (2004). Genetic, environmental, and gender effects on individual differences in toddler expressive language. Journal of Speech, Language, and Hearing Research, 47, 904912.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Von Bastian, C. C., & Oberauer (2013). Distinct transfer effects of training different facets of working memory capacity. Journal of Memory and Language, 69, 3658.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Wadsworth, S. J., DeFries, J. C., Fulker, D. W., Olson, R. K., & Pennington, B. F. (1995). Reading performance and verbal short-term memory: A twin study of reciprocal causation. Intelligence, 20, 145167.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Wallentin, M. (2009). Putative sex differences in verbal abilities and language cortex: A critical review. Brain & Language, 108, 175183. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.bandl.2008.07.001 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Weisleder, A., & Fernald, A. (2013). Talking to children matters: Early language experience strengthens processing and builds vocabulary. Psychological Science, 24(11) 21432152. https://doi.org/10.1177/0956797613488145 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Willinger, U., Schmoeger, M., Deckert, M., Eisenwort, B., Loader, B., Hofmair, A., & Auff, E. (2017). Screening for specific language impairment in preschool children: Evaluating a screening procedure including the token test. Journal of Psycholinguistic Research, 46(5), 12371247. https://doi.org/10.1007/s10936-017-9493-z CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Zaretsky, E., Euler, H. A., Neumann, K., & Lange, B. P. (2014). Sociolinguistic predictors of language deficits in pre-school children with and without immigrant background. Talk at the Asian Conference on Language Learning, Osaka, Japan, April 17–20, 2014.Google Scholar
Zaretsky, E., & Lange, B. P. (2017). Sociolinguistic factors associated with the subjectively and objectively measured language development in German preschoolers in three follow-up studies. Linguistics, 55(1), 3965. https://doi.org/10.1515/ling-2016-0036.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Zaretsky, E., Lange, B. P., Euler, H. A., Robinson, F., & Neumann, K. (2017). Pre-schoolers who stutter score lower in verbal skills than their non-stuttering peers. Buckingham Journal of Language and Linguistics, 10, 96115. https://doi.org/10.5750/bjll.v10i0.1171 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Zell, E., Krizan, Z., & Teeter, S. R. (2015). Evaluating gender similarities and differences using metasynthesis. American Psychologist, 70, 1020. https://doi.org/10.1037/a0038208 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed