Hostname: page-component-77c89778f8-gq7q9 Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-07-17T02:32:05.331Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

Prediction differs at sentence and discourse level: An event-related potential study

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  29 June 2020

Ruohan Chang
Affiliation:
Beijing Sport University Key Laboratory of Behavioral Science, Institute of Psychology, Chinese Academy of Sciences University of Chinese Academy of Sciences
Xiaohong Yang*
Affiliation:
Key Laboratory of Behavioral Science, Institute of Psychology, Chinese Academy of Sciences University of Chinese Academy of Sciences
Yufang Yang*
Affiliation:
Key Laboratory of Behavioral Science, Institute of Psychology, Chinese Academy of Sciences University of Chinese Academy of Sciences
*
*Corresponding author: E-mails: yangyf@psych.ac.cn, yangxh@psych.ac.cn
*Corresponding author: E-mails: yangyf@psych.ac.cn, yangxh@psych.ac.cn

Abstract

This study used event-related potentials (ERPs) to investigate how predicting upcoming words differ when contextual information used to generate the prediction is from the immediately preceding sentence context versus an earlier discourse context. Four-sentence discourses were presented to participants, with the critical words in the last sentences, either predictable or unpredictable based on sentence- or discourse-level contextual information. At the sentence level, the crucial contextual information for prediction was provided by the last sentence, where the critical word was embedded (e.g., Xiaoyu came to the living room. She made a cup of lemon tea. Then she sat down in a chair. She opened a box/an album to look at the pictures.), and at the discourse level by the first sentence (e.g., Xiaoyu took out a box/an album. She made a cup of lemon tea. Then she sat down in a chair. She leisurely looked at the pictures.). Results showed reduced N400 for predictable words compared to unpredictable counterparts at sentence and discourse levels and also a post-N400 positivity effect of predictability at sentence level. This suggests that both sentence- and discourse-level semantic information help readers predict upcoming words, but supportive sentence context more than discourse context.

Type
Original Article
Copyright
© The Author(s), 2020. Published by Cambridge University Press

Access options

Get access to the full version of this content by using one of the access options below. (Log in options will check for institutional or personal access. Content may require purchase if you do not have access.)

References

Albrecht, J. E., & Myers, J. L. (1998). Accessing distant text information during reading: Effects of contextual cues. Discourse Processes, 26, 87107. doi: 10.1080/01638539809545040 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Altmann, G. T. M., & Mirković, J. (2009). Incrementality and prediction in human sentence processing. Cognitive Science, 33, 583609. doi: 10.1111/j.1551-6709.2009.01022.x CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Boudewyn, M. A., Gordon, P. C., Long, D., Polse, L., & Swaab, T. Y. (2012). Does discourse congruence influence spoken language comprehension before lexical association? Evidence from event-related potentials. Language and Cognitive Processes, 27, 698733. doi: 10.1080/01690965.2011.577980 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Boudewyn, M. A., Long, D. L., & Swaab, T. Y. (2013). Effects of working memory span on processing of lexical associations and congruence in spoken discourse. Frontiers in Psychology, 4, 60. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2013.00060 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Boudewyn, M. A., Long, D. L., & Swaab, T. Y. (2015). Graded expectations: predictive processing and the adjustment of expectations during spoken language comprehension. Cognitive, Affective, & Behavioral Neuroscience, 15, 607624. doi: 10.3758/s13415-015-0340-0 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Brothers, T., Swaab, T. Y., & Traxler, M. J. (2015). Effects of prediction and contextual support on lexical processing: Prediction takes precedence. Cognition, 136, 135149. doi: 10.1016/j.cognition.2014.10.017 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Brouwer, H., Fitz, H., & Hoeks, J. (2012). Getting real about semantic illusions: Rethinking the functional role of the P600 in language comprehension. Brain Research, 1446, 127143. doi: 10.1016/j.brainres.2012.01.055 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Burkhardt, P. (2007). The P600 reflects cost of new information in discourse memory. NeuroReport, 18, 18511854. doi: 10.1097/WNR.0b013e3282f1a999 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Camblin, C. C., Gordon, P. C., & Swaab, T. Y. (2007). The interplay of discourse congruence and lexical association during sentence processing: Evidence from ERPs and eye tracking. Journal of Memory & Language, 56, 103. doi: 10.1016/j.jml.2006.07.005 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Coulson, S., & Van Petten, C. (2007). A special role for the right hemisphere in metaphor comprehension? ERP evidence from hemifield presentation. Brain Research, 1146, 128145. doi: 10.1016/j.brainres.2007.03.008 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Delong, K. A., Chan, Wen-hsuan, & Kutas, M. (2019). Similar time courses for word form and meaning preactivation during sentence comprehension. Psychophysiology, 56, e13312. doi: 10.1111/psyp.13312 Google ScholarPubMed
Delong, K. A., Quante, L., & Kutas, M. (2014). Predictability, plausibility, and two late ERP positivities during written sentence comprehension. Neuropsychologia, 61, 150162. doi: 10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2014.06.016 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Delong, K. A., Urbach, T. P., Groppe, D. M., & Kutas, M. (2011). Overlapping dual ERP responses to low cloze probability sentence continuations. Psychophysiology, 48, 12031207. doi: 10.1111/j.1469-8986.2011.01199.x CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Delong, K. A., Urbach, T. P., & Kutas, M. (2005). Probabilistic word pre-activation during language comprehension inferred from electrical brain activity. Nature Neuroscience, 8, 11171121. doi: 10.1038/nn1504 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Diaz, M. T., Swaab, T. Y. (2007). Electrophysiological differentiation of phonological and semantic integration in word and sentence contexts. Brain Research, 1146, 85100. doi: 10.1016/j.brainres.2006.07.034 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Federmeier, K. D. (2007). Thinking ahead: the role and roots of prediction in language comprehension. Psychophysiology, 44, 491505. doi: 10.1111/j.1469-8986.2007.00531.x CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Federmeier, K. D., Wlotko, E. W., Ochoa-Dewald, E. D., & Kutas, M. (2007). Multiple effects of sentential constraint on word processing. Brain Research, 1146, 7584. doi: 10.1016/j.brainres.2006.06.101 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Freunberger, D., & Nieuwland, M. S. (2016). Incremental comprehension of spoken quantifier sentences: Evidence from brain potentials. Brain Research, 1646, 475481. doi: 10.1016/j.brainres.2016.06.035 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Freunberger, D., & Roehm, D. (2016). Semantic prediction in language comprehension: Evidence from brain potentials. Language, Cognition & Neuroscience, 31, 11931205. doi: 10.1080/23273798.2016.1205202 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Guerra, E., & Knoeferle, P. (2014). Spatial distance effects on incremental semantic interpretation of abstract sentences: Evidence from eye tracking. Cognition, 133, 535552. doi: 10.1016/j.cognition.2014.07.007 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Hasson, U., Chen, J., & Honey, C. J. (2015). Hierarchical process memory: Memory as an integral component of information processing. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 19, 304313. doi: 10.1016/j.tics.2015.04.006 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Ito, A., Corley, M., Pickering, M. J., Martin, A. E., & Nieuwland, M. (2016). Predicting form and meaning: Evidence from brain potentials. Journal of Memory Language & Cognitive Processes, 86, 157171. doi: 10.1016/j.jml.2015.10.007 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Kintsch, W. (1988). The role of knowledge in discourse comprehension: A construction-integration model. Psychological Review, 95, 163182. doi: 10.1037/0033-295X.95.2.163 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Kong, F., & Zhou, G. (2014). Chinese comma disambiguation on K-best parse trees. In Zong, C. et al. (Eds.) Natural Language Processing and Chinese Computing: Communications in Computer and Information Science 496 (pp. 1322). Springer-Verlag Berlin Heidelberg.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Kuperberg, G. R., & Jaeger, F. T. (2016). What do we mean by prediction in language comprehension? Language, Cognition & Neuroscience, 31, 3259. doi: 10.1080/23273798.2015.1102299 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Kutas, M. (1993). In the company of other words: Electrophysiological evidence for single-word and sentence context effects. Language and Cognitive Processes, 8, 533572. doi: 10.1080/01690969308407587 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Kutas, M., & Federmeier, K. D. (2011). Thirty years and counting: Finding meaning in the N400 component of the event related brain potential (ERP). Annual Review of Psychology, 62, 621647. doi: 10.1146/annurev.psych.093008.131123 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Kutas, M., & Hillyard, S. A. (1980). Reading senseless sentences: Brain potentials reflect semantic incongruity. Science, 207, 203205. doi: 10.1126/science.7350657 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Kutas, M., & Hillyard, S. A. (1984). Brain potentials during reading reflect word expectancy and semantic association. Nature, 307, 161163. doi: 10.1038/307161a0 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Kutas, M., Neville, H. J., & Holcomb, P. J. (1987). A preliminary comparison of the N400 response to semantic anomalies during reading, listening and signing. Electroencephalography and Clinical Neurophysiology. Supplement, 39, 325330.Google ScholarPubMed
Laszlo, S., & Federmeier, K. D. (2009). A beautiful day in the neighborhood: An event-related potential study of lexical relationships and prediction in context. Journal of Memory & Language, 61, 326338. doi: 10.1016/j.jml.2009.06.004 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Ledoux, K., Camblin, C. C., Swaab, T. Y., & Gordon, P. C. (2006). Reading words in discourse: The modulation of lexical priming effects by message-level context. Cognitive, Affective, & Behavioral Neuroscience, 5, 107. doi: 10.1177/1534582306289573 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Li, X., Zhang, Y., Xia, J., & Swaab, T. Y. (2017). Internal mechanisms underlying anticipatory language processing: Evidence from event-related-potentials and neural oscillations. Neuropsychologia, 102, 7081. doi: 10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2017.05.017 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
McKoon, G., & Ratcliff, R. (1998). Memory-based language processing: psycholinguistic research in the 1990s. Annual Review of Psychology, 49, 2542. doi: 10.1146/annurev.psych.49.1.25 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Myers, J. L., Cook, A. E., Kambe, G., Mason, R. A., & O’Brien, E. J. (2000). Semantic and episodic effects on bridging inferences. Discourse Processes, 29, 179199. doi: 10.1207/S15326950dp2903_1 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Myers, J. L., & O’Brien, E. J. (1998). Accessing the discourse representation during reading. Discourse Processes, 26, 131157. doi: 10.1080/01638539809545042 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Nieuwland, M. S. (2019). Do “early” brain responses reveal word form prediction during language comprehension? A critical review. Neuroscience & Biobehavioral Reviews, 96, 367400. doi: 10.1016/j.neubiorev.2018.11.019 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Nieuwland, M. S., Barr, D. J., Bartolozzi, F., Busch-Moreno, S., Darley, E., Donaldson, D. I., … Von Grebmer Zu Wolfsthurn, S. (2019). Dissociable effects of prediction and integration during language comprehension: Evidence from a large-scale study using brain potentials. Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society. doi: 10.1101/267815 Google ScholarPubMed
Nieuwland, M. S., Politzer-Ahles, S., Heyselaar, E., Segaert, K., Darley, E., Kazanina, N., … Huettig, F. (2018). Large-scale replication study reveals a limit on probabilistic prediction in language comprehension. eLife, 7, e33468. doi: 10.7554/eLife.33468 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
O’Brien, E. J., Plewes, P. S., & Albrecht, J. E. (1990). Antecedent retrieval processes. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 16, 241249. doi: 10.1037/0278-7393.16.2.241 Google ScholarPubMed
Otten, M., & Van Berkum, J. J. A. (2007). What makes a discourse constraining? Comparing the effects of discourse message and scenario fit on the discourse-dependent N400 effect. Brain Research, 1153, 166177. doi: 10.1016/j.brainres.2007.03.058 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Otten, M., & Van Berkum, J. J. A. (2008). Discourse-based word anticipation during language processing: Prediction or priming? Discourse Processes, 45, 464496. doi: 10.1080/01638530802356463 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Pickering, M. J., & Garrod, S. (2007). Do people use language production to make predictions during comprehension? Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 11, 105110. doi: 10.1016/j.tics.2006.12.002 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Pickering, M. J., & Garrod, S. (2013). An integrated theory of language production and comprehension. The Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 36, 329347. doi: 10.1017/S0140525X12001495 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Pijnacker, J., Geurts, B., Van Lambalgen, M., Buitelaar, J., & Hagoort, P. (2010). Exceptions and anomalies: An ERP study on context sensitivity in autism. Neuropsychologia, 48, 29402951. doi: 10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2010.06.003 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Sassenhagen, J., & Bornkessel-Schlesewsky, I. (2015). The P600 as a correlate of ventral attention network reorientation. Cortex, 66, A3A20. doi: 10.1016/j.cortex.2014.12.019 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Sassenhagen, J., Schlesewsky, M., & Bornkessel-Schlesewsky, I. (2014). The P600-as-P3 hypothesis revisited: Single-trial analyses reveal that the late EEG positivity following linguistically deviant material is reaction time aligned. Brain and Language, 137, 2939. doi: 10.1016/j.bandl.2014.07.010 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Thornhill, D. E., & Van Petten, C. (2012). Lexical versus conceptual anticipation during sentence processing: Frontal positivity and N400 ERP components. International Journal of Psychophysiology, 83, 382392. doi: 10.1016/j.ijpsycho.2011.12.007 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Van Berkum, J. J. A., Brown, C. M., Zwitserlood, P., Kooijman, V., & Hagoort, P. (2005). Anticipating upcoming words in discourse: Evidence from ERPs and reading times. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 31, 443467. doi: 10.1037/0278-7393.31.3.443 Google ScholarPubMed
Van Den Broek, P., Rapp, D. N., & Kendeou, P. (2005). Integrating memory-based and constructionist processes in accounts of reading comprehension. Discourse Processes, 39, 299316. doi: 10.1080/0163853X.2005.9651685 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Van Petten, C., Coulson, S., Rubin, S., Plante, E., & Parks, M. (1999). Time course of word identification and semantic integration in spoken language. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory and Cognition, 25, 394417. doi: 10.1037/0278-7393.25.2.394 Google ScholarPubMed
Van Petten, C., & Luka, B. J. (2012). Prediction during language comprehension: benefits, costs, and ERP components. International Journal of Psychophysiology, 83, 176190. doi: 10.1016/j.ijpsycho.2011.09.015 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Wang, L., Hagoort, P., & Jensen, O. (2017). Language prediction is supported by coupling between frontal gamma and posterior alpha oscillations. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 30, 432447. doi: 10.1162/jocn_a_01190 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Yang, X., Chen, S., Chen, X., & Yang, Y. (2015). How distance affects semantic integration in discourse: Evidence from event-related potentials. PLoS One, 10, e0142967. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0142967 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Yang, X., Zhang, X., Yang, Y., & Lin, N. (2018). How context features modulate the involvement of the working memory system during discourse comprehension. Neuropsychologia, 111, 36. doi: 10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2018.01.010 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Zhou, Y., & Xue, N. (2012, July). PDTB-style discourse annotation of Chinese text. In Proceedings of the 50th Annual Meeting of the Association for Computational Linguistics (Vol. 1: Long Papers, pp. 69–77). Jeju, Korea.Google Scholar
Zhou, Y., & Xue, N. (2015). The Chinese discourse treebank: A Chinese corpus annotated with discourse relations. Language Resources and Evaluation, 49, 397431. doi: 10.1007/s10579-014-9290-3 CrossRefGoogle Scholar