Hostname: page-component-7bb8b95d7b-dtkg6 Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-10-06T16:55:59.830Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

Antarctica as a ‘natural laboratory’ for the critical assessment of the archaeological validity of early stone tool sites

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  31 January 2023

Metin I. Eren*
Affiliation:
Department of Anthropology, Kent State University, Kent, USA Department of Archaeology, Cleveland Museum of Natural History, USA
Michelle R. Bebber
Affiliation:
Department of Anthropology, Kent State University, Kent, USA
Briggs Buchanan
Affiliation:
Department of Anthropology, University of Tulsa, USA
Anne Grunow
Affiliation:
Polar Rock Repository, Byrd Polar and Climate Research Center, Ohio State University, Columbus, USA
Alastair Key*
Affiliation:
Department of Archaeology, University of Cambridge, UK
Stephen J. Lycett
Affiliation:
Department of Anthropology, University at Buffalo, USA
Erica Maletic
Affiliation:
Polar Rock Repository, Byrd Polar and Climate Research Center, Ohio State University, Columbus, USA
Teal R. Riley
Affiliation:
British Antarctic Survey, Cambridge, UK
*
*Authors for correspondence ✉ meren@kent.edu & ak2389@cam.ac.uk
*Authors for correspondence ✉ meren@kent.edu & ak2389@cam.ac.uk
Rights & Permissions [Opens in a new window]

Abstract

Lithic technologies dominate understanding of early humans, yet natural processes can fracture rock in ways that resemble artefacts made by Homo sapiens and other primates. Differentiating between fractures made by natural processes and primates is important for assessing the validity of early and controversial archaeological sites. Rather than depend on expert authority or intuition, the authors propose a null model of conchoidally fractured Antarctic rocks. As no primates have ever occupied the continent, Antarctica offers a laboratory for generating samples that could only have been naturally fractured. Examples that resemble artefacts produced by primates illustrate the potential of ‘archaeological’ research in Antarctica for the evaluation of hominin sites worldwide.

Type
Research Article
Creative Commons
Creative Common License - CCCreative Common License - BY
This is an Open Access article, distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution licence (https://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/4.0/), which permits unrestricted re-use, distribution, and reproduction in any medium, provided the original work is properly cited.
Copyright
Copyright © The Author(s), 2023. Published by Cambridge University Press on behalf of Antiquity Publications Ltd

Introduction

For at least three million years, extinct species of hominins and Homo sapiens made tools using various types of rock that fracture conchoidally, for example, flint, obsidian and basalt (Harmand et al. Reference Harmand2015). There is also evidence to suggest that both living and past non-human primates can exhibit behaviours that might lead to rock fracture (Mercader et al. Reference Mercader, Panger and Boesch2002; Proffitt et al. Reference Proffitt2016; Falótico et al. Reference Falótico2019). Natural processes, however, can also fracture or alter rock and have been doing so for far longer than hominins and primates (Warren Reference Warren1914; Barnes Reference Barnes1939; Hiscock Reference Hiscock1985; Pevny Reference Pevny, Carr, Bradbury and Price2012; Andrefsky, Jr. Reference Andrefsky, Graf, Ketron and Waters2013). Such processes include fluvial and glacial actions, falls and landslides, temperature extremes, animal trampling and sediment consolidation (Eren et al. Reference Eren2010; Andrefsky, Jr. Reference Andrefsky, Graf, Ketron and Waters2013). The comparison of primate-made stone tools and naturally fractured rocks demonstrates potential similarities or overlap in some morphological and technological elements (Figure 1). This is because certain elements associated with primate-made stone tools can also occur in naturally fractured rocks. These include: flake morphology; percussion bulbs; distal termination types; platform types; platform angles; sharp edges; regularised or continuous retouch; ‘patterned’ or ‘intentional’ flaking; and size, shape and spatial patterning (Manninen Reference Manninen2007; Eren et al. Reference Eren2011; Andrefsky Reference Andrefsky, Graf, Ketron and Waters2013; Borrazzo Reference Borrazzo2016, Reference Borrazzo, Martínez, Rojas and Cabrera2020; Borrero Reference Borrero2016). Moreover, elements associated with naturally fractured rocks, such as natural cleavage planes, frost-fracturing, physical and chemical weathering, post-depositional damage and natural transport processes, can also characterise or affect primate-made stone tools or assemblages (Borrazzo Reference Borrazzo2016). Such overlap can become even more challenging to differentiate when knappers take advantage of features such as natural platforms or naturally formed acute angles to initiate intentional fracture, or, alternatively, where natural processes modify a primate-made stone tool assemblage (Manninen Reference Manninen2007: 77; Andrefsky, Jr. Reference Andrefsky, Graf, Ketron and Waters2013).

Figure 1. There is overlap in technological and morphological elements between primate-made and naturally fractured rocks, but how much overlap is currently poorly understood (figure produced by M.I. Eren and S.J. Lycett).

This overlap in the morphological and technological elements of hominin-induced and natural conchoidal fracture creates an identification problem. Differentiation is especially challenging when seeking to identify the earliest occupations of regions by stone tool-using hominins, because such stone artefacts may be low in frequency, crude in form, found in equivocal contexts, or lack other associated artefactual data, leading to potential ambiguity as to hominin agency (Meltzer Reference Meltzer, Bonnichsen and Steele1994; Dennell & Hurcombe Reference Dennell and Hurcombe1995; Bar-Yosef & Belfer-Cohen Reference Bar-Yosef and Belfer-Cohen2001). This issue is critical, since such early occurrences are inevitably chronologically ‘anomalous’ with respect to other regional data. At best, this creates potential controversy (e.g. Dennell & Hurcombe Reference Dennell and Hurcombe1995; Driver Reference Driver2001a; Gillespie et al. Reference Gillespie, Tupakka and Cluney2004; Gao et al. Reference Gao, Wei, Shen and Keates2005; Surovell et al. Reference Surovell2022) and, at worst, could lead to a Type I error (i.e. falsely accepting a result as positive when it is actually negative), resulting in the construction of false knowledge within the field.

To distinguish between naturally occurring and hominin-induced conchoidal fractures, archaeologists often rely on expert authority, experience or intuition to determine whether or not a stone object is an artefact (Driver Reference Driver2001b; Gillespie et al. Reference Gillespie, Tupakka and Cluney2004; O'Connor Reference O'Connor2007; Meltzer Reference Meltzer2015; Borrero Reference Borrero2016; Boehm & Anderson Reference Boehm, Anderson, Andrews, Meltzer and Stiger2021). In some cases, the archaeological validity of an artefact or assemblage of artefacts is determined by the consensus of several experts. One of the reasons that we must currently rely on authority, experience, intuition and consensus to determine archaeological validity is that, in some cases, we do not have a realistic understanding of how many elements are shared between primate-made and naturally fractured assemblages. There have been numerous experiments that illustrate how natural processes can produce specimens that appear to be primate-made (e.g. Warren Reference Warren1914; McPherron et al. Reference McPherron2014; Borrazzo Reference Borrazzo2016, Reference Borrazzo, Martínez, Rojas and Cabrera2020), and these serve as an important reservoir of interpretative cautionary tales. No experiment, however, can replicate reality with exact precision, and an experiment's relationship to the parameters of direct interest (i.e. the natural world) requires specific assumptions and inferences (Lycett & Eren Reference Lycett and Eren2013: 526).

In particular, there are three unavoidable drawbacks to lithic experiments in terms of their application to our understanding of how many elements are shared between primate-made and naturally fractured assemblages. First, while an experiment can demonstrate how a specific natural process can create specimens that appear to be primate-made, it cannot demonstrate how frequently such an event occurred in the past. Consider, for example, animal trampling. Numerous experiments have demonstrated that trampling can produce ‘knapped’ flakes with sharp edges (e.g. Warren Reference Warren1914; Lopinot & Ray Reference Lopinot and Ray2007; Domínguez-Solera et al. Reference Domínguez-Solera, Maíllo-Fernández, Baquedano and Domínguez-Rodrigo2021), ‘retouched’ tools (e.g. McBrearty et al. Reference McBrearty1998; Pargeter & Bradfield Reference Pargeter and Bradfield2012), and ‘bend-break’ fractures (e.g. Warren Reference Warren1914; Eren et al. Reference Eren2011; Jennings Reference Jennings2011; Andrefsky, Jr. Reference Andrefsky, Graf, Ketron and Waters2013). But how often, in reality, did animals walk over blocks of stone and create a lithic scatter? This question is not one an experiment can answer, because all experiments are, by their nature, somewhat contrived (Lycett & Eren Reference Lycett and Eren2013: 527). A second unavoidable drawback to lithic experiments is that some natural processes may be difficult, or even impossible, to replicate. For instance, it is currently unclear how an experiment could convincingly replicate the effect of glacial activity on rocks that possess conchoidal fracture properties. The third drawback is the short time duration of lithic experiments relative to singular, or multiple, natural processes that may occur over decades, centuries, millennia, or even longer.

These three drawbacks highlight the need for a sustained field research programme that complements experimental efforts (e.g. Eren & Bebber Reference Eren and Bebber2019; Magnani Reference Magnani2019a, Reference Magnani2019b; Borrazzo Reference Borrazzo, Martínez, Rojas and Cabrera2020) by investigating what natural processes do to conchoidally fracturing rocks outside of the laboratory. In other words, archaeological research would benefit tremendously from the development of a null model of conchoidally fractured rocks that developed entirely from natural processes, against which potential archaeological samples could be compared. Such a model should not only include quantitative and qualitative information on morphological (e.g. size and shape), technological (e.g. flake scar counts and patterning) and raw material (e.g. chert, obsidian, limestone) attributes of conchoidally fractured specimens, but also specimen frequency and density at particular geographical locales, the context of specimens (e.g. cave, coastline) and their distribution across broader landscapes.

Unfortunately, the global distribution of primates means that archaeologists cannot exclude the possibility that what they believe to be naturally fractured rocks were, in fact, produced by living or extinct primates. As such, in most regions of the world, the construction of a null model based on a long-term field research programme would depend on the authority, experience and intuition of lithic experts to determine which specimens or locales to include or exclude as ‘natural’. Given that the sole purpose of the proposed null model is to eliminate authority, experience and intuition in such determinations, the inherent circularity of this situation should be readily apparent. Consequently, archaeologists need a primate-free ‘natural laboratory’ from which a null model of naturally fractured rocks can be constructed. Here, we propose that Antarctica can act as such a natural laboratory, because no hominin or non-human primate has ever occupied the continent. As proof of concept, we present a series of Antarctic rock specimens that exhibit conchoidal fracture and, which if found anywhere beyond ‘the ice’, could easily be mistaken for stone artefacts produced by hominins—even Homo sapiens.

Conchoidally fractured rocks from Antarctica

The Polar Rock Repository (https://prr.osu.edu/) in Columbus, Ohio, forms part of the Byrd Polar and Climate Research Center of The Ohio State University. As of March 2022, it curates nearly 59 000 rock samples from Antarctica, the southern oceans, and South America, as well as small collections from Africa and Australia. Using the Center's searchable online database, we requested samples of raw materials, such as chert, basalt and obsidian, commonly used by primates to make stone tools. Upon visual inspection of the samples at the Polar Rock Repository, we quickly identified several dozen that could easily be miscategorised as primate-produced, of which we present 14 here (see Figure 2 and Table 1; see also the online supplementary material (OSM)). We limit our presentation to these 14 out of an abundance of caution; unlike some other specimens we examined, the selected samples show no recent marks, such as those that could be produced by a modern geological hammer. The set of 14 conchoidally fractured rocks, collected from numerous locations across Antarctica (Figure 3), comprises a variety of forms, including those that could be mistaken for flakes, cores and even bifaces. The lithologies include chert, quartzite, hornfels, basalt and obsidian (Table 1).

Figure 2. Examples of Antarctic rock samples that bear resemblance to proposed human- or non-human primate-made stone tools: a) PRR-37153, ‘large flake’; b) PRR-17243, ‘discoid core’; c) PRR-37115, ‘core’; d) PRR-23389, ‘biface’; e) PRR-56439, ‘bipolar core’; f) PRR-34869, ‘chopper’. For more specimens and images, see the online supplementary material (OSM) (figure produced by M.I. Eren and M.R. Bebber).

Figure 3. The geographic locations where the presented specimens were collected. For specimen identification, see Table 1. Source of base map: Polar Rock Repository (https://prr.osu.edu/) (figure produced by M.I. Eren).

Table 1. Rock specimens possessing natural conchoidal fracture from Antarctica.

Going forward

There are numerous instances in which the archaeological validity of early stone tool sites and lithic artefacts is either contentious or unknown and which could therefore be strengthened or weakened by comparison to a null model of naturally fractured rocks. Targeted field research could assess analogous contexts and raw materials in Antarctica for comparison with proposed archaeological stone tool assemblages from: caves (e.g. Ardelean et al. Reference Ardelean2020, Reference Ardelean2022; Chatters et al. Reference Chatters2022); rockshelters (e.g. Meltzer et al. Reference Meltzer, Adovasio and Dillehay1994; Boëda et al. Reference Boëda2021, Reference Boëda, Pérez-Balarezo and Ramos2022; Coutouly Reference Coutouly2022); open-air sites (e.g. Domínguez-Rodrigo & Alcalá Reference Domínguez-Rodrigo and Alcalá2016, Reference Domínguez-Rodrigo and Alcalá2019; Zhu et al. Reference Zhu2018; Harmand et al. Reference Harmand2019); deglaciated landscapes (e.g. Overstreet & Kolb Reference Overstreet and Kolb2003; Joyce Reference Joyce2006, Reference Joyce, Graf, Ketron and Waters2013); coasts or ancient river courses (e.g. Parfitt et al. Reference Parfitt2005, Reference Parfitt2010); mountainous or desert regions (e.g. Rowe et al. Reference Rowe2022); or under water (e.g. O'Shea Reference O'Shea2014; Lemke Reference Lemke2021; White Reference White2021). Once a null model is created from the specific context(s) in question, quantitative and qualitative morphological, technological, frequency and density comparisons could be made between the null model(s) and the proposed archaeological dataset(s), such that objective and probabilistic statements of archaeological validity can be made. The documentation of the geological and other natural processes in Antarctica will be fundamental for this research, in order to identify the conchoidal fracturing—or lack thereof—of different types of rock. Detailed contextual documentation is especially important, given that current polar rock databases do not present or report such information for key raw materials such as cherts, basalts and obsidian.

Readers of this proposal may disagree with the conclusion that the 14 specimens presented here could be mistaken for tools produced by primates. They are entitled to that view, but if that is all the reader takes away, then they have missed our point entirely. Two main facts underpin the proposal that Antarctica would make an excellent natural laboratory for generating null models of conchoidally fractured rock: 1) its variety of natural processes; and 2) the specimens presented here are made from rocks with properties that support conchoidal fracture. It is entirely beside the point for the purpose of generating null models whether some of these specimens appear to be from the Lower, Middle or Upper Palaeolithic, or whether they appear to be formal cores or expediently made. That some specimens do appear to resemble those that are made by hominins does suggest, however, that future Antarctic null models have the potential to substantially weaken the validity of some controversial archaeological sites. Conversely, the comparison of material from a controversial site to an Antarctic null model could potentially strengthen the validity of the archaeological interpretation of the site.

While not all 14 specimens presented here are necessarily highly convincing examples, in the sense that they might be misidentified as artefacts made by primates, they do provide strong examples relative to proposed artefacts from some early and/or controversial archaeological sites. Consider, for example, the ‘discoid core’ from Chiquihuite Cave (Ardelean et al. Reference Ardelean2020: 91) or the ‘cultural lithics’ from the Hebior and Schaefer sites (Joyce Reference Joyce, Graf, Ketron and Waters2013: 475). These proposed tools are directly comparable to the specimens from Antarctica presented here. Given the absence of other strong evidence to support the archaeological validity of these sites (e.g. Grayson & Meltzer Reference Grayson and Meltzer2015; Chatters et al. Reference Chatters2022), the similarities between the ‘artefacts’ from Chiquihuite or Hebior and Schaefer and the Antarctic specimens presented here suggests that they cannot be automatically taken as evidence of primate manufacture. Moreover, if more ‘complex’ specimens, such as proposed discoid cores or bifaces, possess Antarctic ‘doppelgängers’, then bashed or split cobbles, flakes and microflakes should certainly be compared with specimens from Antarctic contexts (e.g. Parfitt et al. Reference Parfitt2005, Reference Parfitt2010; Lemke Reference Lemke2021; Rowe et al. Reference Rowe2022).

Generating the Antarctic null datasets proposed above will be neither quick nor easy. It will take years, possibly decades, and will require multidisciplinary collaboration and detailed field research. Documenting each Antarctic rock dataset and context will, however, complement the plethora of existing lithic experiments by contributing to archaeologists’ broader understanding of the extent of overlap that exists between primate-produced and naturally fractured rocks, thereby reducing dependency on authority, experience and intuition in the assessment of the archaeological validity of proposed early sites around the world.

Acknowledgements

We are appreciative to Steve Emslie, John Goodge, C. Owen Lovejoy, Richard Meindl, David J. Meltzer and Mary Ann Raghanti, who provided useful discussions or feedback on this manuscript and/or its presentation at the 2022 Annual Meeting of the Society for American Archaeology (SAA) in Chicago. This material is based on services provided by the Polar Rock Repository, with support from the National Science Foundation, under Cooperative Agreement OPP-1643713.

Funding statement

This research received no specific grant from any funding agency or from commercial and not-for-profit sectors.

Data statement

All data generated or analysed during this study are included in this article (and its online supplementary material).

Supplementary material

To view supplementary material for this article, please visit https://doi.org/10.15184/aqy.2023.4.

References

Andrefsky, W. Jr. 2013. Fingerprinting flake production and damage processes: toward identifying human artifact characteristics, in Graf, K., Ketron, V. & Waters, M. (ed.) Paleoamerican odyssey: 415–28. College Station: Texas A&M University Press.Google Scholar
Ardelean, C. et al. 2020. Evidence of human occupation in Mexico around the Last Glacial Maximum. Nature 584: 8792. https://doi.org/10.1038/s41586-020-2509-0CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Ardelean, C. et al. 2022. Chiquihuite Cave and America's hidden limestone industries: a reply to Chatters et al. PaleoAmerica 8: 1728. https://doi.org/10.1080/20555563.2021.1985063
Barnes, A. 1939. The differences between natural and human flaking on prehistoric flint implements. American Anthropologist 41: 99112. https://doi.org/10.1525/aa.1939.41.1.02a00080CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Bar-Yosef, O. & Belfer-Cohen, A.. 2001. From Africa to Eurasia: early dispersals. Quaternary International 75: 1928. https://doi.org/10.1016/S1040-6182(00)00074-4CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Boëda, E., Pérez-Balarezo, A. & Ramos, M.. 2022. Another “critique,” same old song: a brief rebuttal to Gómez Coutouly. PaleoAmerica 8: 5361. https://doi.org/10.1080/20555563.2021.1985051CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Boëda, E. et al. 2021. 24.0 kyr cal BP stone artefact from Vale da Pedra Furada, Piauí, Brazil: techno-functional analysis. PLoS ONE 16: e0247965. https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0247965CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Boehm, A. & Anderson, R.. 2021. Blocks X and Y, in Andrews, B., Meltzer, D. & Stiger, M. (ed.) The Mountaineer site: a Folsom winter camp in the Rockies: 94109. Louisville: University Press of Colorado. https://doi.org/10.5876/9781646421404.c005CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Borrazzo, K. 2016. Lithic taphonomy in desert environments: contributions from Fuego-Patagonia (southern South America). Quaternary International 422: 1928. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.quaint.2015.12.012CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Borrazzo, K. 2020. Expanding the scope of actualistic taphonomy in archaeological research, in Martínez, S., Rojas, A. & Cabrera, F. (ed.) Actualistic taphonomy in South America: 221–42. Cham: Springer.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Borrero, L.A. 2016. Ambiguity and debates on the early peopling of South America. PaleoAmerica 2: 1121. https://doi.org/10.1080/20555563.2015.1136498CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Chatters, J. et al. 2022. Evaluating claims of early human occupation at Chiquihuite Cave, Mexico. PaleoAmerica 8: 116. https://doi.org/10.1080/20555563.2021.1940441CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Coutouly, Y. 2022. Questioning the anthropic nature of Pedra Furada and the Piauí Sites. PaleoAmerica 8: 2952. https://doi.org/10.1080/20555563.2021.1943181CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Dennell, R. & Hurcombe, L.. 1995. Comment on Pedra Furada. Antiquity 69: 604. https://doi.org/10.1017/S0003598X0008203XCrossRefGoogle Scholar
Domínguez-Rodrigo, M. & Alcalá, L.. 2016. 3.3-million-year-old stone tools and butchery traces? More evidence needed. Paleoanthropology: 4653. https://doi.org/10.4207/PA.2016.ART99Google Scholar
Domínguez-Rodrigo, M. & Alcalá, L.. 2019. Pliocene archaeology at Lomekwi 3? New evidence fuels more skepticism. Journal of African Archaeology 17: 173–76. https://doi.org/10.1163/21915784-20190006CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Domínguez-Solera, S., Maíllo-Fernández, J., Baquedano, E. & Domínguez-Rodrigo, M.. 2021. Equids can also make stone artefacts. Journal of Archaeological Science: Reports 40: 103260. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jasrep.2021.103260Google Scholar
Driver, J. 2001a. A comment on methods for identifying quartzite cobble artifacts. Canadian Journal of Archaeology 25: 127–31.Google Scholar
Driver, J. 2001b. Preglacial archaeological evidence at Grimshaw, the Peace River area, Alberta: discussion. Canadian Journal of Earth Science 38: 871–74. https://doi.org/10.1139/e00-124CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Eren, M. & Bebber, M.. 2019. The Cerutti Mastodon site and experimental archaeology's quiet coming of age. Antiquity 93: 796–97. https://doi.org/10.15184/aqy.2019.50CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Eren, M. et al. 2010. Experimental examination of animal trampling effects on artifact movement in dry and water saturated substrates: a test case from south India. Journal of Archaeological Science 37: 3010–21. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jas.2010.06.024CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Eren, M. et al. 2011. Flaked stone taphonomy: a controlled experimental study of the effects of sediment consolidation on flake edge morphology. Journal of Taphonomy 9: 201–17.Google Scholar
Falótico, T. et al. 2019. Three thousand years of wild capuchin stone tool use. Nature Ecology and Evolution 3: 1034–38. https://doi.org/10.1038/s41559-019-0904-4CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Gao, X., Wei, Q., Shen, C. & Keates, S.. 2005. New light on the earliest hominid occupation in East Asia. Current Anthropology 46: S115–20. https://doi.org/10.1086/497666CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Gillespie, J., Tupakka, S. & Cluney, C.. 2004. Distinguishing between naturally and culturally flaked cobbles: a test case from Alberta, Canada. Geoarchaeology 19: 615–33. https://doi.org/10.1002/gea.20015CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Grayson, D. & Meltzer, D.. 2015. Revisiting Paleoindian exploitation of extinct North American mammals. Journal of Archaeological Science 56:177–93. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jas.2015.02.009CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Harmand, S. et al. 2015. 3.3-million-year-old stone tools from Lomekwi 3, West Turkana, Kenya. Nature 521: 310–15. https://doi.org/10.1038/nature14464CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Harmand, S. et al. 2019. Reply to Domínguez-Rodrigo and Alcalá: interpretation without accurate evidence is fantasy. Journal of African Archaeology 17: 177–81. https://doi.org/10.1163/21915784-20190007CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Hiscock, P. 1985. The need for a taphonomic perspective in stone artefact analysis. Queensland Archaeological Research 2: 8297. https://doi.org/10.25120/qar.2.1985.197CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Jennings, T. 2011. Experimental production of bending and radial flake fractures and implications for lithic technologies. Journal of Archaeological Science 38: 3644–51. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jas.2011.08.035CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Joyce, D. 2006. Chronology and new research on the Schaefer mammoth (?Mammuthus primigenius) site, Kenosha County, Wisconsin, USA. Quaternary International 142: 4457. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.quaint.2005.03.004CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Joyce, D. 2013. Pre-Clovis megafauna butchery sites in the western Great Lakes region, USA, in Graf, K., Ketron, V. & Waters, M. (ed.) Paleoamerican odyssey: 467–83. College Station: Texas A&M University Press.Google Scholar
Lemke, A. 2021. The day the armchair broke: a reply to White. PaleoAmerica 7: 305308. https://doi.org/10.1080/20555563.2021.1979309CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Lopinot, N.H. & Ray, J.H.. 2007. Trampling experiments in the search for the earliest Americans. American Antiquity 72: 771–82. https://doi.org/10.2307/25470445CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Lycett, S. & Eren, M.. 2013. Levallois lessons: the challenge of integrating mathematical models, quantitative experiments and the archaeological record. World Archaeology 45: 519–38. https://doi.org/10.1080/00438243.2013.821670CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Manninen, M.A. 2007. Non-flint pseudo-lithics: some considerations. Fennoscandia Archaeologica 24: 7697.Google Scholar
Magnani, M. et al. 2019a. Evaluating claims for an early peopling of the Americas: experimental design and the Cerutti Mastodon site. Antiquity 93: 789–95. https://doi.org/10.15184/aqy.2019.14CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Magnani, M. et al. 2019b. Experimental futures in archaeology. Antiquity 93: 808–10. https://doi.org/10.15184/aqy.2019.76CrossRefGoogle Scholar
McBrearty, S. et al. 1998. Tools underfoot: human trampling as an agent of lithic artifact edge modification. American Antiquity 63: 108–29. https://doi.org/10.2307/2694779CrossRefGoogle Scholar
McPherron, S. et al. 2014. An experimental assessment of the influences on edge damage to lithic artifacts: a consideration of edge angle, substrate grain size, raw material properties, and exposed face. Journal of Archaeological Science 49: 7082. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jas.2014.04.003CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Meltzer, D. 1994. The discovery of deep time: a history of views on the peopling of the Americas, in Bonnichsen, R. & Steele, D. (ed.) Method and theory for investigating the peopling of the Americas: 726. Corvallis (OR): Center for the Study of the First Americans.Google Scholar
Meltzer, D. 2015. The great Paleolithic war. Chicago (IL): University of Chicago Press. https://doi.org/10.7208/chicago/9780226293363.001.0001CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Meltzer, D., Adovasio, J. & Dillehay, T.. 1994. On a Pleistocene human occupation at Pedra Furada, Brazil. Antiquity 68: 695714. https://doi.org/10.1017/S0003598X00047414CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Mercader, J., Panger, M. & Boesch, C.. 2002. Excavation of a chimpanzee stone tool site in the African rainforest. Science 296: 1452–55. https://doi.org/10.1126/science.1070268CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
O'Connor, A. 2007. Finding time for the old stone age: a history of Palaeolithic archaeology and quaternary geology in Britain, 1860–1960. Oxford: Oxford University Press. https://doi.org/10.1093/oso/9780199215478.001.0001CrossRefGoogle Scholar
O'Shea, J. et al. 2014. A 9000-year-old caribou hunting structure beneath Lake Huron. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the USA 111: 6911–15. https://doi.org/10.1073/pnas.1404404111CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Overstreet, D. & Kolb, M.. 2003. Geoarchaeological contexts for Late Pleistocene archaeological sites with human-modified woolly mammoth remains in southeastern Wisconsin, USA. Geoarchaeology 18: 91114. https://doi.org/10.1002/gea.10052CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Parfitt, S. et al. 2005. The earliest record of human activity in Northern Europe. Nature 438: 1008–12. https://doi.org/10.1038/nature04227CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Parfitt, S. et al. 2010. Early Pleistocene human occupation at the edge of the boreal zone in northwest Europe. Nature 466: 229–33. https://doi.org/10.1038/nature09117CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Pargeter, J. & Bradfield, J.. 2012. The effects of class I and II sized bovids on macrofracture formation and tool displacement: results of a trampling experiment in a southern African Stone Age context. Journal of Field Archaeology 37: 238–51. https://doi.org/10.1179/0093469012Z.00000000022CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Pevny, C. 2012. Distinguishing taphonomic processes from stone tool use at the Gault site, Texas, in Carr, P.J., Bradbury, A.P. & Price, S.E. (ed.) Contemporary lithic analysis in the southeast: problems, solutions, and interpretations: 5578. Tuscaloosa: University of Alabama Press.Google Scholar
Proffitt, T. et al. 2016. Wild monkeys flake stone tools. Nature 539: 8588. https://doi.org/10.1038/nature20112CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Rowe, T. et al. 2022. Human occupation of the North American Colorado Plateau ~37 000 years ago. Frontiers in Ecology and Evolution: 534. https://doi.org/10.3389/fevo.2022.903795Google Scholar
Surovell, T. et al. 2022. Late date of human arrival to North America: continental scale differences in stratigraphic integrity of pre-13 000 BP archaeological sites. PLoS ONE 17: e0264092. https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0264092CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Warren, S. 1914. The experimental investigation of flint fracture and its application to problems of human implements. Journal of the Royal Anthropological Institute 44: 412–50.Google Scholar
White, A. 2021. A critique of the case for Paleoindian caribou hunting on the submerged Alpena-Amberley Ridge. PaleoAmerica 7: 287304. https://doi.org/10.1080/20555563.2021.1942651CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Zhu, Zhaoyu et al. 2018. Hominin occupation of the Chinese Loess Plateau since about 2.1 million years ago. Nature 559: 608–12. https://doi.org/10.1038/s41586-018-0299-4CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Figure 0

Figure 1. There is overlap in technological and morphological elements between primate-made and naturally fractured rocks, but how much overlap is currently poorly understood (figure produced by M.I. Eren and S.J. Lycett).

Figure 1

Figure 2. Examples of Antarctic rock samples that bear resemblance to proposed human- or non-human primate-made stone tools: a) PRR-37153, ‘large flake’; b) PRR-17243, ‘discoid core’; c) PRR-37115, ‘core’; d) PRR-23389, ‘biface’; e) PRR-56439, ‘bipolar core’; f) PRR-34869, ‘chopper’. For more specimens and images, see the online supplementary material (OSM) (figure produced by M.I. Eren and M.R. Bebber).

Figure 2

Figure 3. The geographic locations where the presented specimens were collected. For specimen identification, see Table 1. Source of base map: Polar Rock Repository (https://prr.osu.edu/) (figure produced by M.I. Eren).

Figure 3

Table 1. Rock specimens possessing natural conchoidal fracture from Antarctica.

Supplementary material: PDF

Eren et al. supplementary material

Eren et al. supplementary material

Download Eren et al. supplementary material(PDF)
PDF 1.9 MB