Hostname: page-component-cd9895bd7-q99xh Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-12-26T22:53:44.707Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

Motivation to dust-bathe of laying hens housed in cages and in aviaries

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  01 March 2007

S. Colson
Affiliation:
UEBEAC, AFSSA-LERAP, Zoopôle Beaucemaine BP 53, 22440 Ploufragan, France
C. Arnould
Affiliation:
Station de Recherches Avicoles, INRA – Centre de Tours, 37380 Nouzilly, France
V. Michel*
Affiliation:
UEBEAC, AFSSA-LERAP, Zoopôle Beaucemaine BP 53, 22440 Ploufragan, France

Abstract

New housing systems for commercial egg production, furnished cages and non-cage systems, should improve the welfare of laying hens. In particular, thanks to the presence of a litter area, these new housing systems are thought to satisfy the dust-bathing motivation of hens more than in conventional cages, in which no litter area is present. However, although apparently obvious, there is no concrete evidence that non-cage systems, particularly aviaries, satisfy hens' motivation to dust-bathe and thus improve hens' welfare in terms of dust-bathing behaviour. The aim of this study was to compare hens' dust-bathing motivation when housed for a long time under similar conditions to commercial conditions in laying aviaries (with litter) and in conventional cages (without litter). Three treatments were compared: hens reared in floor pens then housed in conventional cages, hens reared in furnished floor pens then housed in a laying aviary, and hens reared in rearing aviaries then housed in a laying aviary. All three treatments provided access to litter during the rearing period. After transfer to the laying systems, access to litter was maintained for the aviary hens but stopped for the cage hens. Twelve groups of four hens per treatment were tested 36 to 43 weeks after transfer. The hens were placed in sawdust-filled testing arenas, and latency to dust-bathe, duration and number of dust baths, and number of hens dust-bathing were recorded. Latency to dust-bathe was shorter, dust baths were longer and more numerous and more hens dust-bathed among cage hens than among aviary hens. Our results indicate that hens' motivation to dust-bathe was more satisfied in laying aviaries than in conventional cages. Thus, laying aviaries improve hens' welfare in term of dust-bathing behaviour compared with conventional cages.

Type
Research Paper
Copyright
Copyright © The Animal Consortium 2007

Introduction

In many countries, laying hens are mainly housed in conventional cages during the laying period (for example, in 2002, in France, about 83% of the hens were housed in conventional cages; Chalimbaud (Reference Chalimbaud2003)). Under this system, space is restricted and the only furnishings are nipples and feed troughs. In Europe, Council Directive 1999/74/EC is intended to improve laying hens' welfare by imposing more spacious housing systems with more furnishings: furnished cages and non-cage systems (aviaries and floor systems, with or without access to outdoor areas). These imposed systems are expected to diversify the behavioural repertoire of hens, e.g. dust-bathing in a suitable substrate.

According to the European Food Safety Authority report (EFSA, 2005), dust-bathing in a dry and friable substrate is a behavioural priority that maintains hens' feathers in good condition (Van Liere and Bokma, Reference Van Liere and Bokma1987; Van Liere, Reference Van Liere1992). Various techniques have been used to study motivation to dust-bathe, in particular after a deprivation period (Vestergaard, Reference Vestergaard1982; Hogan et al., Reference Hogan, Honrado and Vestergaard1991; Vestergaard et al., Reference Vestergaard, Damm, Abbott and Bildsoe1999). The reports have shown that when hens are deprived of litter, their motivation to dust-bathe increases: latency to dust-bathe decreases, and the intensity (number of elements composing the dust baths), number and length of dust baths all increase when hens are given access to a litter after a deprivation period. Studies have been performed on young chicks (Hogan et al., Reference Hogan, Honrado and Vestergaard1991; Vestergaard et al., Reference Vestergaard, Damm, Abbott and Bildsoe1999) and on adult hens (Vestergaard, Reference Vestergaard1982) under research conditions with groups of two birds (Hogan et al., Reference Hogan, Honrado and Vestergaard1991; Vestergaard et al., Reference Vestergaard, Damm, Abbott and Bildsoe1999) or 50 birds (Vestergaard, Reference Vestergaard1982) and with short deprivation periods (less than 5 days).

There is no evidence that the laying systems imposed by Directive 1999/74/EC provide sufficient access to litter to enhance hens' welfare in terms of dust-bathing behaviour. To the best of our knowledge, no studies have compared adult hens' motivation to dust-bathe in conventional cages v. aviaries. Different studies compared the effect of different designs of furnished cages (Appleby et al., Reference Appleby, Smith and Hughes1993) and different flooring materials (Merrill et al., Reference Merrill, Cooper, Albentosa and Nicol2006) on dust-bathing behaviour, but Guesdon (Reference Guesdon2004) performed the only study, under conditions close to commercial conditions, comparing motivation to dust-bathe in hens housed in conventional cages v. furnished cages. None of the aviary studies published has shown that it was possible for every hen to perform dust-bathing. As it has been observed that hens present uneven spatial distribution (Hansen, Reference Hansen1994; Carmichael et al., Reference Carmichael, Walker and Hughes1999; Channing et al., Reference Channing, Hughes and Walker2001; Odén et al., Reference Odén, Keeling and Algers2002), there may be inadequate access to litter, and the litter provided in aviaries may be of poor quality in the second part of the laying period.

The aim of this experiment was to compare the motivation of adult hens to dust-bathe in a suitable substrate when they were housed for a long time under conditions close to commercial conditions in laying aviaries (with litter) and in conventional cages (without litter). This experiment is a part of a study comparing welfare of laying hens (housed in both of these housing systems) and adaptation to laying aviaries of hens previously reared in various conditions (Colson et al., Reference Colson, Arnould, Guémené and Michel2005a, Reference Colson, Arnould, Huonnic, Boilletot and Michelb and Reference Colson, Arnould, Huonnic and Michelc and Reference Colson, Michel and Arnould2006; Michel et al., Reference Michel, Huonnic, Colson and Maurice2005). In the present study, two rearing systems for pullets assigned to laying aviaries were used: rearing aviaries and furnished floor pens.

Material and methods

Animals and housing

The experiment was performed on ISA Brown hens. They arrived at 1 day of age, were beak-trimmed at 9 days, transferred from rearing to laying systems at 17 weeks, and slaughtered at 69 weeks of age.

Three treatments were compared: one involving cage hens (5 060 “C hens”) and two involving aviary hens (2 560 “A1 hens” and 2 560 “A2 hens”). C hens were reared in seven floor pens each measuring 69 m2 and covered with litter (847 cm2 of litter area per pullet). After transfer, the hens were allotted to four three-tier batteries of conventional cages (five hens per cage: 582 cm2 of usable area per hen) for the laying period. The cages (Big Dutchman, Germany) were 60 cm wide, 48.5 cm deep and 40 cm high (at 2/3 depth) with two water nipples at the rear and an automatic feeding-chain at the front. A1 hens were reared in three furnished floor pens each measuring 69 m2, covered with litter, and containing perches and two slatted platforms (742 cm2 of litter area per pullet). After transfer, the hens were housed in a laying aviary (Natura Nova, Big Dutchman, Germany; Figure 1). A2 hens were reared in four rearing aviaries (Natura Rearing type, Big Dutchman, Germany) each measuring 24 m2 and containing litter areas (208 cm2 of litter area per pullet). They were identical to laying aviaries but adapted to pullets' size and without nest boxes. After transfer, they were housed in a second laying aviary. The laying aviaries were 7.5 m wide and 30 m deep, giving 1 149 cm2 of usable area per hen.

Figure 1 Cross-sectional view of the laying aviary (Natura Nova, Big Dutchman, Germany) housing the A2 hens (hens previously reared in rearing aviaries). The laying aviary where the A1 hens were housed (hens previously reared in floor pens furnished with perches and slatted platforms) was the mirror image of this one.

Conventional cages and laying aviaries were installed in two barns in which all conditions (lighting, temperature, etc.) were standardised and similarly controlled. During the laying period, hens were given 16 h of light.

In all three treatments, the hens had access to litter for 16 weeks before they were transferred to the laying systems. After transfer, litter access was maintained for A1 and A2 hens whereas C hens were deprived of litter.

Test arenas and procedure

The test arenas were 100 cm wide, 100 cm deep and 80 cm high. They were composed of a wire lid, a bare wooden floor, two contiguous wooden walls and two contiguous wire walls, similar to those used by Guesdon (Reference Guesdon2004). All test arenas contained one tray feeder, one flow drinker, and a piece of Astroturf® for laying (40 × 40 cm) designed to be a compromise between nest boxes in aviaries and no nests in cages.

Twelve groups of four hens per treatment were tested from 36 to 43 weeks after transfer, i.e. 36 to 43 weeks of litter deprivation for C hens. All hens were tested only once. Each group of four C hens was housed in the same cage. The cages sampled were distributed across all the batteries. Each group of four A1 and four A2 hens was constituted of hens close together. For each aviary, the 12 groups were sampled from 12 areas distributed on the length and the height of aviary. About 2 h before the light was turned off, the hens were carried in a closed plastic box to a test arena. They were individually identified, placed in the test arena, and left undisturbed. Eight hours after the light was turned on, when the time of laying had passed and the hens' motivation to dust-bathe should be maximum (Vestergaard, Reference Vestergaard1982; Vestergaard et al., Reference Vestergaard, Hogan and Kruijt1990), the piece of Astroturf® was removed, sawdust was added (a layer of about 10 cm high, covering all the floor of the arena) and the hens were then observed for 5 h. As there was no test location available outside of barns, the hens were tested in their own setting, i.e. in a test arena located in the cage barn for C hens, and in another test arena located in the aviary barn for A1 and A2 hens. Test arenas were visually isolated from the rest of the barn.

Focal sampling on video recording was used to record latency to dust-bathe, duration and number of dust baths per hen, and the number of hens dust-bathing. Based on current knowledge, typical dust-bath bouts can be divided into two phases (Borchelt, Reference Borchelt1974; Van Liere, Reference Van Liere1992; Duncan et al., Reference Duncan, Widowski, Malleau, Lindberg and Petherick1998). During the first phase, the hens stand and scratch the substrate. During the second phase, hens squat or lay down, and perform the following elements: vertical wing shaking, bill-raking, head or side-rubbing, side-lying (sometimes with leg-wing stretching), and scratching. Only the second phase, which can be defined more precisely, was recorded in our tests. This phase can be interrupted several times in the course of the same dust bath. When the interruption was longer than 10 min, it was considered that a new dust-bath bout had begun (Vestergaard et al., Reference Vestergaard, Hogan and Kruijt1990; Hogan and Van Boxel, Reference Hogan and Van Boxel1993; Duncan et al., Reference Duncan, Widowski, Malleau, Lindberg and Petherick1998).

Statistical analyses

Dust baths recorded as lasting less than 5 min did not meet the full definition of a dust-bath as they did not contain all the elements of a typical bout. They were considered as remnants or forerunners of typical bouts, and were discarded.

The statistical unit was the group of four hens. For the latency to dust-bathe and the number of dust baths per hen, means were calculated per group on the four hens. For duration of dust baths, means were calculated per group on all dust baths performed. As data had a non-normal distribution and samples were of small size, variables were analysed using non-parametric tests, i.e. Kruskal-Wallis one-way analysis of variance for overall comparison of the three treatments (d.f. = 2), followed by pair comparisons using the Mann-Whitney U-test (d.f. = 1) when significant. The level of significance was set at P < 0.05. Analyses were performed using the Systat 9.0 software package (Systat Software Inc., Richmond, CA, USA).

Results

The treatment effects were significant for all variables (KW = 21, P < 0.001 in all cases). Latency to dust-bathe was shorter (Figure 2a), dust baths were longer (Figure 2b) and more numerous (Figure 2c), and more hens dust-bathed (Figure 2d) among C hens than among A1 and A2 hens.

Figure 2 Means ± standard deviations for latency to dust-bathe (a), length of dust baths (b), number of dust baths (c) and number of hens dust-bathing (d) measured during the five hours of observation, for each treatment: C hens reared in floor pens and then housed in conventional cages - A1 hens reared in furnished-floor pens (with perches and slatted platforms) and then housed in a laying aviary - A2 hens reared in rearing aviaries and then housed in a laying aviary. (*** P < 0.001).

Discussion

Our results show that the motivation to dust-bathe was higher in cage hens (deprived of litter) than in aviary hens (with litter). In our laying aviaries, access to litter seemed to be sufficient to satisfy the dust-bathing motivation of the hens. Moreover, even though the percentage of hens on litter was lower in hens from rearing aviaries than in hens from furnished floor pens (Colson et al., Reference Colson, Arnould, Huonnic and Michel2005c), this difference in terms of use of space had no effect on motivation to dust-bathe.

We cannot exclude that the observed differences in hens' motivation to dust-bathe arose from the different experiences lived not only in the laying environments but also in the rearing environments. Indeed, the rearing environments may have differentially satisfied the motivation to dust-bathe or have influenced the hens' development in unknown ways. However, we focussed on the impact of the full production system (rearing and laying) on motivation to dust-bathe during the tests. Hens housed in the two aviary production systems tested had a similar motivation to dust-bathe, in spite of a great difference in the surface of litter available per pullet between rearing aviaries (200 cm2) and furnished floor pens (742 cm2), whereas the size of the litter area available per pullet was much more similar between furnished floor pens and floor pens (847 cm2). Thus, it is probable that the difference in motivation to dust-bathe found between aviary and cage hens was mainly due to differences in laying systems rather than differences in rearing systems.

In our experiment, the substrate available during our tests (sawdust) was different from the litter of laying aviaries. Hens preferentially choose a familiar substrate for dust-bathing (Vestergaard and Hogan, Reference Vestergaard and Hogan1992). Consequently, aviary hens may have preferred to dust-bathe in a similar substrate to the litter of laying aviaries rather than in the sawdust provided in the test-arenas. This unfamiliar substrate probably do not prevent hens from dust-bathing, as it has been shown that when they are sufficiently motivated, hens can dust-bathe even if the substrate is inappropriate, as on wire floor (Olsson et al., Reference Olsson, Keeling and Duncan2002).

We cannot exclude the possibility that aviary hens were more disturbed by the test situation than cage hens. Indeed, the aviary hens were perhaps less familiar within each group tested than cage hens, and the former lived a greater change of environment between living conditions and test arenas (marked space reduction) and were certainly more disturbed by the absence of a nest-box in the test arenas. This may have decreased their motivation to dust-bathe (Vestergaard, Reference Vestergaard1982) and explains the fact that the average duration of dust baths performed by aviary hens was less than the values cited in the literature, which ranged from 20 to 30 min in favourable conditions such as ad libitum litter (no deprivation), low density and high luminosity (Vestergaard, Reference Vestergaard1982; Vestergaard et al., Reference Vestergaard, Hogan and Kruijt1990). However, we observed less than 21% of aviary hens showing pacing, attempts to exit or nesting behaviour during the tests, and in less than 9% of the cases it lasted more than 30 min. The time hens spent in the test-arenas before observation was considered to be long enough to limit disturbances.

According to our results, aviaries seem to provide an improvement on conventional cages in terms of dust-bathing behaviour. Such an improvement in furnished cages is not an obvious conclusion. Indeed, in parallel to our experiment, Guesdon (Reference Guesdon2004) performed a study on the dust-bathing motivation of hens housed in conventional and furnished cages. Hens' motivation was tested 16 to 27 weeks after their transfer to cages. The same testing method as ours was used to compare hens' motivation to dust-bathe between two types of conventional cages and four types of furnished cages. Different frequencies of sawdust distribution in the furnished cages were used: never, every 2 weeks, weekly and daily. Motivation to dust-bathe was similar in all treatments, with hens showing shorter mean latencies to dust-bathe than hens housed in cages in our experiment. These results indicate that in Guesdon's experiment (Guesdon, Reference Guesdon2004), hens housed in conventional and furnished cages were at least as well motivated to dust-bathe as our cage hens, probably because of a litter deprivation effect. Indeed, Guesdon (Reference Guesdon2004) reported that the dust boxes of furnished cages were empty of any litter most of the time.

The fact that sham dust baths are performed in furnished as well as in conventional cages (Lindberg and Nicol, Reference Lindberg and Nicol1997; Olsson and Keeling, Reference Olsson and Keeling2002) and are shorter than the dust baths performed in litter areas (Lindberg and Nicol, Reference Lindberg and Nicol1997) reinforces the supposition that there is certainly some amount of litter deprivation in furnished cages. Thus, in contrast with litter areas in aviaries, the litter areas of furnished cages tested in previous studies do not seem to offer any welfare improvement in terms of dust-bathing behaviour.

Conclusion

Aviary hens showed a low motivation to dust-bathe during our tests, whereas cage hens showed a high motivation. Therefore, in our experimental conditions, and in contrast to conventional cages, laying aviaries seemed to satisfy the hens' motivation to dust-bathe and also to provide an improvement in the hens' welfare in terms of dust-bathing behaviour compared with conventional cages.

Acknowledgements

This study was partly supported by the OFIVAL (Office National Interprofessionnel des Viandes de l'Elevage et de l'Aviculture) and the DGAl (Direction Générale de l'Alimentation). S. Colson was supported by a grant from the Conseil Général des Côtes d'Armor. We are grateful to V. Guesdon for discussions on methodology, and to the staff of the experimental farm (Service d'Expérimentations Avicoles et Cunicoles – AFSSA) for care of the hens.

References

Appleby, MC, Smith, SF and Hughes, BO 1993. Nesting, dust bathing and perching by laying hens in cages: effects of design on behaviour and welfare. British Poultry Science 34, 835-847.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Borchelt, PL 1974. The organization of dustbathing components in Bobwhite Quail (Colinus virginianus). Behaviour LIII, 217-237.Google Scholar
Carmichael, NL, Walker, AW and Hughes, BO 1999. Laying hens in large flocks in a perchery system: influence of stocking density on location, use of resources and behaviour. British Poultry Science 40, 165-176.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Chalimbaud, J 2003. Différents modes d'élevage de poulettes et de pondeuses en France: évolution des effectifs, des performances techniques et des coûts de production. Journée nationale des professionnels de la pondeuse et de l'oeuf de Consommation, Ploufragan, France.Google Scholar
Channing, CE, Hughes, BO and Walker, AW 2001. Spatial distribution and behaviour of laying hens housed in an alternative system. Applied Animal Behaviour Science 72, 335-345.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Colson, S, Arnould, C, Guémené, D and Michel, V 2005a. Bien-être de poules pondeuses logées en cage ou en volière: paramètres physiologiques et comportementaux. In Sixièmes Journées de la Recherche Avicole. St Malo, France, pp. 62-66.Google Scholar
Colson, S, Arnould, C, Huonnic, D, Boilletot, E and Michel, V 2005b. Bien-être de poules pondeuses logées en cage ou en volière: état corporel des animaux. In Sixièmes Journées de la Recherche Avicole. St Malo, France, pp. 77-81.Google Scholar
Colson, S, Arnould, C, Huonnic, D and Michel, V 2005c. Influence of two rearing systems for pullets, rearing aviaries and furnished floor, on space use and production in laying aviaries. Animal Science Papers and Reports 23, (suppl. 1) 85-93.Google Scholar
Colson, S, Michel, V and Arnould, C 2006. Welfare of laying hens housed in cages and in aviaries: what about fearfulness? Archiv für Geflügelkunde 70, 261–269.Google Scholar
Duncan, IJH, Widowski, TM, Malleau, AE, Lindberg, AC and Petherick, JC 1998. External factors and causation of dustbathing in domestic hens. Behavioural Processes 43, 219-228.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
European Food Safety Authority 2005. Behavioural priorities. In Scientific report: Welfare aspects of various systems for keeping laying hens. EFSA - AHAW Panel, EFSA-Q-2003-62, pp. 5265.Google Scholar
Guesdon, V (2004) Etude comparative de poules pondeuses épointées ou non, élevées en cages standard ou aménagées: estimation multicritère du bien-être. Ph.D. thesis, Université de Rennes I, France.Google Scholar
Hansen, I 1994. Behavioural expression of laying hens in aviaries and cages: frequencies, time budgets and facility utilisation. British Poultry Science 35, 491-508.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Hogan, JA and Van Boxel, F 1993. Causal factors controlling dustbathing in Burmese red junglefowl: some results and a model. Animal Behaviour 46, 627-635.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Hogan, JA, Honrado, GI and Vestergaard, KS 1991. Development of a behavior system: Dustbathing in the Burmese Red Junglefowl (Gallus gallus spadiceus): II. Internal factors. Journal of Comparative Psychology 105, 269-273.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Lindberg, AC and Nicol, CJ 1997. Dustbathing in modified battery cages: Is sham dustbathing an adequate substitute? Applied Animal Behaviour Science 55, 113-128.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Merrill, RJN, Cooper, JJ, Albentosa, MJ and Nicol, CJ 2006. The preferences of laying hens for perforated Astroturf over conventional wire as a dustbathing substrate in furnished cages. Animal Welfare 15, 173-178.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Michel, V, Huonnic, D, Colson, S and Maurice, R 2005. Bien-être de poules pondeuses logées en cage ou en volière: comparaison des performances des animaux, de la mortalité et de la qualité de l'air. In Sixièmes journées de la recherche avicole. St Malo, France, pp. 72-76.Google Scholar
Odén, K, Keeling, LJ and Algers, B 2002. Behaviour of laying hens in two types of aviary systems on 25 commercial farms in Sweden. British Poultry Science 43, 169-181.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Olsson, IAS and Keeling, LJ 2002. No effect of social competition on sham dustbathing in furnished cages for laying hens. Acta Agriculturae Scandinavica Section A Animal Science 52, 253-256.Google Scholar
Olsson, IAS, Keeling, LJ and Duncan, IJH 2002. Why do hens sham dustbathe when they have litter? Applied Animal Behaviour Science 76, 53-64.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Van Liere, DW 1992. The significance of fowls' bathing in dust. Animal Welfare 1, 187-202.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Van Liere, DW and Bokma, S 1987. Short-term feather maintenance as a function of dust-bathing in laying hens. Applied Animal Behaviour Science 18, 197-204.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Vestergaard, KS 1982. Dust-bathing in the domestic fowl - diurnal rhythm and dust deprivation. Applied Animal Ethology 8, 487-495.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Vestergaard, KS, Damm, BI, Abbott, UK and Bildsoe, M 1999. Regulation of dustbathing in feathered and featherless domestic chicks: the Lorenzian model revisited. Animal Behaviour 58, 1017-1025.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Vestergaard, KS and Hogan, JA 1992. The development of a behavior system: Dustbathing in the Burmese Red Junglefowl. III. Effects of experience on stimulus preference. Behaviour 121, 215-230.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Vestergaard, KS, Hogan, JA and Kruijt, JP 1990. The development of a behavior system: Dustbathing in the Burmese Red Junglefowl. I. The influence of the rearing environment on the organization of dustbathing. Behaviour 112, 99-116.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Figure 0

Figure 1 Cross-sectional view of the laying aviary (Natura Nova, Big Dutchman, Germany) housing the A2 hens (hens previously reared in rearing aviaries). The laying aviary where the A1 hens were housed (hens previously reared in floor pens furnished with perches and slatted platforms) was the mirror image of this one.

Figure 1

Figure 2 Means ± standard deviations for latency to dust-bathe (a), length of dust baths (b), number of dust baths (c) and number of hens dust-bathing (d) measured during the five hours of observation, for each treatment: C hens reared in floor pens and then housed in conventional cages - A1 hens reared in furnished-floor pens (with perches and slatted platforms) and then housed in a laying aviary - A2 hens reared in rearing aviaries and then housed in a laying aviary. (*** P < 0.001).