Skip to main content Accessibility help
×
Hostname: page-component-76fb5796d-25wd4 Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-04-25T14:10:30.282Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

Perception and Action in a Social Context

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  26 November 2021

Shaheed Azaad
Affiliation:
Central European University, Budapest
Günther Knoblich
Affiliation:
Central European University, Budapest
Natalie Sebanz
Affiliation:
Central European University, Budapest

Summary

Even the simplest social interactions require us to gather, integrate, and act upon, multiple streams of information about others and our surroundings. In this Element, we discuss how perceptual processes provide us with an accurate account of action-relevant information in social contexts. We overview contemporary theories and research that explores how: (1) individuals perceive others' mental states and actions, (2) individuals perceive affordances for themselves, others, and the dyad, and (3) how social contexts guide our attention to modulate what we perceive. Finally, we review work on the cognitive mechanisms that make joint action possible and discuss their links to perception.
Get access
Type
Element
Information
Online ISBN: 9781009029049
Publisher: Cambridge University Press
Print publication: 23 December 2021

Access options

Get access to the full version of this content by using one of the access options below. (Log in options will check for institutional or personal access. Content may require purchase if you do not have access.)

References

Ambrosini, E., Pezzulo, G., & Costantini, M. (2015). The eye in hand: Predicting others’ behavior by integrating multiple sources of information. Journal of Neurophysiology, 113(7), 2271–9. https://doi.org/10.1152/jn.00464.2014Google Scholar
Ambrosini, E., Reddy, V., de Looper, A., Costantini, M., Lopez, B., & Sinigaglia, C. (2013). Looking ahead: Anticipatory gaze and motor ability in infancy. PLOS ONE, 8(7), e67916. https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0067916CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Ambrosini, E., Sinigaglia, C., & Costantini, M. (2012). Tie my hands, tie my eyes. Journal of Experimental Psychology. Human Perception and Performance, 38(2), 263–6. https://doi.org/10.1037/a0026570Google Scholar
Anzures, G., Quinn, P. C., Pascalis, O., Slater, A. M., & Lee, K. (2010). Categorization, categorical perception, and asymmetry in infants’ representation of face race. Developmental Science, 13(4), 553–64. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1467-7687.2009.00900.xGoogle Scholar
Atherton, G., Sebanz, N., & Cross, L. (2019). Imagine all the synchrony: The effects of actual and imagined synchronous walking on attitudes towards marginalised groups. PLOS ONE, 14(5), e0216585. https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0216585CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Atkinson, M. A., Simpson, A., Skarratt, P. A., & Cole, G. G. (2014). Is social inhibition of return due to action co-representation? Acta Psychologica, 150, 8593. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.actpsy.2014.04.003Google Scholar
Avenanti, A., Sirigu, A., & Aglioti, S. M. (2010). Racial bias reduces empathic sensorimotor resonance with other-race pain. Current Biology, 20(11), 1018–22. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.cub.2010.03.071CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Aviezer, H., Hassin, R. R., Ryan, J., Grady, C., Susskind, J., Anderson, A., Moscovitch, M., & Bentin, S. (2008). Angry, disgusted, or afraid? Studies on the malleability of emotion perception. Psychological Science, 19(7), 724–32. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1467-9280.2008.02148.xGoogle Scholar
Bach, P., Nicholson, T., & Hudson, M. (2014). The affordance-matching hypothesis: How objects guide action understanding and prediction. Frontiers in Human Neuroscience, 8, 254. https://doi.org/10.3389/fnhum.2014.00254Google Scholar
Bach, P., & Schenke, K. C. (2017). Predictive social perception: Towards a unifying framework from action observation to person knowledge. Social and Personality Psychology Compass, 11(7), 117. https://doi.org/10.1111/spc3.12312CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Bakeman, R., & Adamson, L. B. (1984). Coordinating attention to people and objects in mother-infant and peer-infant interaction. Child Development, 55(4), 1278–89. https://doi.org/10.2307/1129997CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Baldauf, D., & Deubel, H. (2010). Attentional landscapes in reaching and grasping. Vision Research, 50(11), 9991013. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.visres.2010.02.008Google Scholar
Bang, D., Aitchison, L., Moran, R., Herce Castanon, S., Rafiee, B., Mahmoodi, A., Lau, J. Y. F., Latham, P. E., Bahrami, B., & Summerfield, C. (2017). Confidence matching in group decision-making. Nature Human Behaviour, 1(6), 17. https://doi.org/10.1038/s41562-017-0117CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Barrett, L. F., Mesquita, B., & Gendron, M. (2011). Context in emotion perception. Current Directions in Psychological Science, 20(5), 286–90. https://doi.org/10.1177/0963721411422522Google Scholar
Barrett, L. F., & Westlin, C. (2021). Chapter 2 – Navigating the science of emotion. In Meiselman, H. L. (Ed.), Emotion Measurement (Second Edition) (pp. 39–84). Woodhead Publishing. https://doi.org/10.1016/B978-0-12-821124-3.00002-8Google Scholar
Bartley, S. H. (1958). Principles of Perception. Harper.Google Scholar
Bates, A. T., Patel, T. P., & Liddle, P. F. (2005). External behavior monitoring mirrors internal behavior monitoring. Journal of Psychophysiology, 19(4), 281–8. https://doi.org/10.1027/0269-8803.19.4.281Google Scholar
Bauminger-Zviely, N., Eytan, D., Hoshmand, S., & Rajwan Ben-Shlomo, O. (2020). Preschool peer social intervention (PPSI) to enhance social play, interaction, and conversation: Study outcomes. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 50(3), 844–63. https://doi.org/10.1007/s10803-019-04316-2Google Scholar
Bekkering, H., & Neggers, S. F. W. (2002). Visual search is modulated by action intentions. Psychological Science, 13(4), 370–4. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.0956-7976.2002.00466.xGoogle Scholar
Binkofski, F., & Buccino, G. (2006). The role of ventral premotor cortex in action execution and action understanding. Journal of Physiology-Paris, 99(4), 396405. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jphysparis.2006.03.005CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Böckler, A., Knoblich, G., & Sebanz, N. (2011). Observing shared attention modulates gaze following. Cognition, 120(2), 292–8. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.cognition.2011.05.002Google Scholar
Böckler, A., Knoblich, G., & Sebanz, N. (2012). Effects of a coactor’s focus of attention on task performance. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 38(6), 1404–15. https://doi.org/10.1037/a0027523Google Scholar
Brass, M., Bekkering, H., & Prinz, W. (2001). Movement observation affects movement execution in a simple response task. Acta Psychologica, 106(1), 322. https://doi.org/10.1016/S0001-6918(00)00024-XGoogle Scholar
Brennan, S. E., Chen, X., Dickinson, C. A., Neider, M. B., & Zelinsky, G. J. (2008). Coordinating cognition: The costs and benefits of shared gaze during collaborative search. Cognition, 106(3), 1465–77. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.cognition.2007.05.012Google Scholar
Brooks, R., & Meltzoff, A. N. (2002). The importance of eyes: How infants interpret adult looking behavior. Developmental Psychology, 38(6), 958–66. https://doi.org/10.1037/0012-1649.38.6.958Google Scholar
Brooks, R., & Meltzoff, A. N. (2005). The development of gaze following and its relation to language. Developmental Science, 8(6), 535–43. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1467-7687.2005.00445.xGoogle Scholar
Buccino, G., Binkofski, F., Fink, G. R., Fadiga, L., Fogassi, L., Gallese, V., Seitz, R. J., Zilles, K., Rizzolatti, G., & Freund, H.-J. (2001). Action observation activates premotor and parietal areas in a somatotopic manner: An fMRI study. European Journal of Neuroscience, 13(2), 400–4. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1460-9568.2001.01385.xGoogle Scholar
Buccino, G., Binkofski, F., & Riggio, L. (2004). The mirror neuron system and action recognition. Brain and Language, 89(2), 370–6. https://doi.org/10.1016/S0093-934X(03)00356-0CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Buccino, G., Sato, M., Cattaneo, L., Rodà, F., & Riggio, L. (2009). Broken affordances, broken objects: A TMS study. Neuropsychologia, 47(14), 3074–8. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2009.07.003Google Scholar
Calvo-Merino, B., Glaser, D. E., Grèzes, J., Passingham, R. E., & Haggard, P. (2005). Action observation and acquired motor skills: An fMRI study with expert dancers. Cerebral Cortex, 15(8), 1243–9. https://doi.org/10.1093/cercor/bhi007Google Scholar
Cardellicchio, P., Sinigaglia, C., & Costantini, M. (2011). The space of affordances: A TMS study. Neuropsychologia, 49(5), 1369–72. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2011.01.021Google Scholar
Cardellicchio, P., Sinigaglia, C., & Costantini, M. (2013). Grasping affordances with the other’s hand: A TMS study. Social Cognitive and Affective Neuroscience, 8(4), 455–9. https://doi.org/10.1093/scan/nss017Google Scholar
Carello, C., Grosofsky, A., Reichel, F. D., Solomon, H. Y., & Turvey, M. T. (1989). Visually perceiving what is reachable. Ecological Psychology, 1(1), 2754. https://doi.org/10.1207/s15326969eco0101_3CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Carroll, J. M., & Russell, J. A. (1996). Do facial expressions signal specific emotions? Judging emotion from the face in context. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 70(2), 205–18. https://doi.org/10.1037/0022-3514.70.2.205Google Scholar
Cavallo, A., Koul, A., Ansuini, C., Capozzi, F., & Becchio, C. (2016). Decoding intentions from movement kinematics. Scientific Reports, 6(1), 37036. https://doi.org/10.1038/srep37036Google Scholar
Chang, C., Wade, M. G., & Stoffregen, T. A. (2009). Perceiving affordances for aperture passage in an environment–person–person system. Journal of Motor Behavior, 41(6), 495500. https://doi.org/10.3200/35-08-095Google Scholar
Chao, L. L., & Martin, A. (2000). Representation of manipulable man-made objects in the dorsal stream. NeuroImage, 12(4), 478–84. https://doi.org/10.1006/nimg.2000.0635CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Chater, N., Misyak, J., Watson, D., Griffiths, N., & Mouzakitis, A. (2018). Negotiating the traffic: Can cognitive science help make autonomous vehicles a reality? Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 22(2), 93–5. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.tics.2017.11.008Google Scholar
Chemero, A. (2003). An outline of a theory of affordances. Ecological Psychology, 15(2), 181–95. https://doi.org/10.1207/s15326969eco1502_5Google Scholar
Cho, D. T., & Proctor, R. W. (2013). Object-based correspondence effects for action-relevant and surface-property judgments with keypress responses: Evidence for a basis in spatial coding. Psychological Research, 77(5), 618–36. https://doi.org/10.1007/s00426-012-0458-4Google Scholar
Chong, I., & Proctor, R. W. (2020). On the evolution of a radical concept: Affordances according to Gibson and their subsequent use and development. Perspectives on Psychological Science, 15(1), 117–32. https://doi.org/10.1177/1745691619868207Google Scholar
Clarke, T. J., Bradshaw, M. F., Field, D. T., Hampson, S. E., & Rose, D. (2005). The perception of emotion from body movement in point-light displays of interpersonal dialogue. Perception, 34(10), 1171–80. https://doi.org/10.1068/p5203Google Scholar
Cole, G. G., Welsh, T. N., & Skarratt, P. A. (2019). The role of transients in action observation. Attention, Perception, & Psychophysics, 81(7), 2177–91. https://doi.org/10.3758/s13414-019-01740-5Google Scholar
Constable, M. D., Bayliss, A. P., Tipper, S. P., Spaniol, A. P., Pratt, J., & Welsh, T. N. (2016). Ownership status influences the degree of joint facilitatory behavior. Psychological Science, 27(10), 1371–8. https://doi.org/10.1177/0956797616661544Google Scholar
Constable, M. D., Kritikos, A., & Bayliss, A. P. (2011). Grasping the concept of personal property. Cognition, 119(3), 430–7. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.cognition.2011.02.007Google Scholar
Constable, M. D., Kritikos, A., Lipp, O. V., & Bayliss, A. P. (2014). Object ownership and action: The influence of social context and choice on the physical manipulation of personal property. Experimental Brain Research, 232(12), 3749–61. https://doi.org/10.1007/s00221-014-4063-1Google Scholar
Cordovil, R., Andrade, C., & Barreiros, J. (2013). Perceiving children’s affordances: Recalibrating estimation following single-trial observation of three different tasks. Human Movement Science, 32(1), 270–8. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.humov.2013.01.001Google Scholar
Cordovil, R., & Barreiros, J. (2010). Adults’ perception of children’s height and reaching capability. Acta Psychologica, 135(1), 24–9. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.actpsy.2010.04.008Google Scholar
Cordovil, R., & Barreiros, J. (2011). Egocentric or allocentric frameworks for the evaluation of other people’s reachability. Human Movement Science, 30(5), 976–83. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.humov.2010.08.011Google Scholar
Coulson, M. (2004). Attributing emotion to static body postures: Recognition accuracy, confusions, and viewpoint dependence. Journal of Nonverbal Behavior, 28(2), 117–39. https://doi.org/10.1023/B:JONB.0000023655.25550.beGoogle Scholar
Creem-Regehr, S. H., Gagnon, K. T., Geuss, M. N., & Stefanucci, J. K. (2013). Relating spatial perspective taking to the perception of other’s affordances: Providing a foundation for predicting the future behavior of others. Frontiers in Human Neuroscience, 7, 114. https://doi.org/10.3389/fnhum.2013.00596CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Cross, L., Turgeon, M., & Atherton, G. (2019). Moving with the in-crowd: Cooperation and interpersonal entrainment in in- vs. out- groups. Current Psychology, 40(7), 3393–400. https://doi.org/10.1007/s12144-019-00283-0Google Scholar
Csibra, G. (2008). Action mirroring and action understanding: An alternative account. In Haggard, P., Rossetti, Y., & Kawato, M. (Eds.), Sensorimotor Foundations of Higher Cognition (pp. 435–9). Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Curioni, A., Vesper, C., Knoblich, G., & Sebanz, N. (2019). Reciprocal information flow and role distribution support joint action coordination. Cognition, 187, 2131. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.cognition.2019.02.006Google Scholar
D’Ausilio, A., Novembre, G., Fadiga, L., & Keller, P. E. (2015). What can music tell us about social interaction? Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 19(3), 111–14. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.tics.2015.01.005Google Scholar
Davis, T. J., Riley, M. A., Shockley, K., & Cummins-Sebree, S. (2010). Perceiving affordances for joint actions. Perception, 39(12), 1624–44. https://doi.org/10.1068/p6712Google Scholar
De Gelder, B., & Van den Stock, J. (2011). The bodily expressive action stimulus test (BEAST). Construction and validation of a stimulus basis for measuring perception of whole body expression of emotions. Frontiers in Psychology, 2, 116. https://doi.org/10.3389/fpsyg.2011.00181Google Scholar
Derks, B., Stedehouder, J., & Ito, T. A. (2015). Social identity modifies face perception: An ERP study of social categorization. Social Cognitive and Affective Neuroscience, 10(5), 672–9. https://doi.org/10.1093/scan/nsu107CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Deubel, H., Schneider, W. X., & Paprotta, I. (1998). Selective dorsal and ventral processing: Evidence for a common attentional mechanism in reaching and perception. Visual Cognition, 5(1–2), 81107. https://doi.org/10.1080/713756776Google Scholar
Ding, N., & Simon, J. Z. (2012). Emergence of neural encoding of auditory objects while listening to competing speakers. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences, 109(29), 11854–9. https://doi.org/10.1073/pnas.1205381109CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Dittrich, W. H., Troscianko, T., Lea, S. E. G., & Morgan, D. (1996). Perception of emotion from dynamic point-light displays represented in dance. Perception, 25(6), 727–38. https://doi.org/10.1068/p250727Google Scholar
Doerrfeld, A., Sebanz, N., & Shiffrar, M. (2012). Expecting to lift a box together makes the load look lighter. Psychological Research, 76(4), 467–75. https://doi.org/10.1007/s00426-011-0398-4Google Scholar
Doneva, S. P., Atkinson, M. A., Skarratt, P. A., & Cole, G. G. (2017). Action or attention in social inhibition of return? Psychological Research, 81(1), 4354. https://doi.org/10.1007/s00426-015-0738-xCrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Donnarumma, F., Costantini, M., Ambrosini, E., Friston, K., & Pezzulo, G. (2017). Action perception as hypothesis testing. Cortex, 89, 4560. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.cortex.2017.01.016Google Scholar
Driver, J., Davis, G., Ricciardelli, P., Kidd, P., Maxwell, E., & Baron-Cohen, S. (1999). Gaze perception triggers reflexive visuospatial orienting. Visual Cognition, 6(5), 509–40. https://doi.org/10.1080/135062899394920Google Scholar
Ekman, P., Davidson, R. J., & Friesen, W. V. (1990). The Duchenne smile: Emotional expression and brain physiology: II. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 58(2), 342–53. https://doi.org/10.1037/0022-3514.58.2.342Google Scholar
Ekman, P., Friesen, W. V., & Tomkins, S. S. (1971). Facial affect scoring technique: A first validity study. Semiotica, 3(1), 3758. https://doi.org/10.1515/semi.1971.3.1.37Google Scholar
Elekes, F., Varga, M., & Király, I. (2016). Evidence for spontaneous level-2 perspective taking in adults. Consciousness and Cognition, 41, 93103. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.concog.2016.02.010Google Scholar
Ellis, R., Swabey, D., Bridgeman, J., May, B., Tucker, M., & Hyne, A. (2013). Bodies and other visual objects: The dialectics of reaching toward objects. Psychological Research, 77(1), 31–9. https://doi.org/10.1007/s00426-011-0391-yGoogle Scholar
Fajen, B. R., Riley, M. A., & Turvey, M. T. (2009). Information, affordances, and the control of action in sport. International Journal of Sport Psychology, 40(1), 79107.Google Scholar
Farroni, T., Johnson, M. H., Menon, E., Zulian, L., Faraguna, D., & Csibra, G. (2005). Newborns’ preference for face-relevant stimuli: Effects of contrast polarity. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences, 102(47), 17245–50. https://doi.org/10.1073/pnas.0502205102Google Scholar
Fink, B., Neave, N., Manning, J. T., & Grammer, K. (2006). Facial symmetry and judgements of attractiveness, health and personality. Personality and Individual Differences, 41(3), 491–9. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.paid.2006.01.017Google Scholar
Finkel, L., Engler, S., & Randerath, J. (2019). Does it fit? – Trainability of affordance judgments in young and older adults. PLOS ONE, 14(2), e0212709. https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0212709Google Scholar
Finkel, L., Schmidt, K., Scheib, J. P. P., & Randerath, J. (2019). Does it still fit? – Adapting affordance judgments to altered body properties in young and older adults. PLoS ONE, 14(12), e0226729. https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0226729Google Scholar
Flavell, J. H., Everett, B. A., Croft, K., & Flavell, E. R. (1981). Young children’s knowledge about visual perception: Further evidence for the Level 1-Level 2 distinction. Developmental Psychology, 17(1), 99103. https://doi.org/10.1037/0012-1649.17.1.99Google Scholar
Flavell, J. H., Shipstead, S. G., & Croft, K. (1978). Young children’s knowledge about visual perception: Hiding objects from others. Child Development, 49(4), 1208–11. https://doi.org/10.2307/1128761Google Scholar
Frank, M. C., Vul, E., & Johnson, S. P. (2009). Development of infants’ attention to faces during the first year. Cognition, 110(2), 160–70. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.cognition.2008.11.010Google Scholar
Freundlieb, M., Kovács, Á. M., & Sebanz, N. (2016). When do humans spontaneously adopt another’s visuospatial perspective? Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 42(3), 401–12. https://doi.org/10.1037/xhp0000153Google ScholarPubMed
Freundlieb, M., Kovács, Á. M., & Sebanz, N. (2018). Reading your mind while you are reading – Evidence for spontaneous visuospatial perspective taking during a semantic categorization task. Psychological Science, 29(4), 614–22. https://doi.org/10.1177/0956797617740973Google Scholar
Friesen, C. K., & Kingstone, A. (1998). The eyes have it! Reflexive orienting is triggered by nonpredictive gaze. Psychonomic Bulletin & Review, 5(3), 490–5. https://doi.org/10.3758/BF03208827Google Scholar
Friesen, C. K., & Kingstone, A. (2003). Covert and overt orienting to gaze direction cues and the effects of fixation offset. NeuroReport, 14(3), 489–93. DOI:10.1097/00001756-200303030-00039Google Scholar
Frischen, A., Loach, D., & Tipper, S. P. (2009). Seeing the world through another person’s eyes: Simulating selective attention via action observation. Cognition, 111(2), 212–18. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.cognition.2009.02.003Google Scholar
Frischen, A., Smilek, D., Eastwood, J. D., & Tipper, S. P. (2007). Inhibition of return in response to gaze cues: The roles of time course and fixation cue. Visual Cognition, 15(8), 881–95. https://doi.org/10.1080/13506280601112493Google Scholar
Gallese, V., Fadiga, L., Fogassi, L., & Rizzolatti, G. (1996). Action recognition in the premotor cortex. Brain, 119(2), 593609. https://doi.org/10.1093/brain/119.2.593Google Scholar
Gehring, W. J., Liu, Y., Orr, J. M., & Carp, J. (2012). The error-related negativity (ERN/Ne). In S. J. Luck & E. S. Kappenman (Eds.), The Oxford Handbook of Event-Related Potential Components (pp. 231–91). Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Gibson, J. J. (1979). The Ecological Approach to Visual Perception. Houghton Mifflin.Google Scholar
Gonzalez, D. A., Studenka, B. E., Glazebrook, C. M., & Lyons, J. L. (2011). Extending end-state comfort effect: Do we consider the beginning state comfort of another? Acta Psychologica, 136(3), 347–53. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.actpsy.2010.12.009Google Scholar
Gordon, I. E. (2004). Theories of Visual Perception (3rd ed.). Psychology Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Gray, K. L. H., Barber, L., Murphy, J., & Cook, R. (2017). Social interaction contexts bias the perceived expressions of interactants. Emotion, 17(4), 567–71. https://doi.org/10.1037/emo0000257Google Scholar
Grèzes, J., & Decety, J. (2001). Functional anatomy of execution, mental simulation, observation, and verb generation of actions: A meta-analysis. Human Brain Mapping, 12(1), 119. https://doi.org/10.1002/1097-0193(200101)12:1<1::aid-hbm10>3.0.co;2-vGoogle Scholar
Grèzes, J., & Decety, J. (2002). Does visual perception of object afford action? Evidence from a neuroimaging study. Neuropsychologia, 40(2), 212–22. https://doi.org/10.1016/s0028-3932(01)00089-6Google Scholar
Grèzes, J., & Dezecache, G. (2014). How do shared-representations and emotional processes cooperate in response to social threat signals? Neuropsychologia, 55, 105–14. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2013.09.019Google Scholar
Gurney, D. J., Howlett, N., Pine, K., Tracey, M., & Moggridge, R. (2017). Dressing up posture: The interactive effects of posture and clothing on competency judgements. British Journal of Psychology, 108(2), 436–51. https://doi.org/10.1111/bjop.12209Google Scholar
Hackney, A. L., & Cinelli, M. E. (2011). Action strategies of older adults walking through apertures. Gait & Posture, 33(4), 733–6. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.gaitpost.2011.02.019Google Scholar
Hall, J. A., Andrzejewski, S. A., Murphy, N. A., Mast, M. S., & Feinstein, B. A. (2008). Accuracy of judging others’ traits and states: Comparing mean levels across tests. Journal of Research in Personality, 42(6), 1476–89. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jrp.2008.06.013Google Scholar
Halovic, S., & Kroos, C. (2018). Not all is noticed: Kinematic cues of emotion-specific gait. Human Movement Science, 57, 478–88. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.humov.2017.11.008Google Scholar
Handy, T. C., Grafton, S. T., Shroff, N. M., Ketay, S., & Gazzaniga, M. S. (2003). Graspable objects grab attention when the potential for action is recognized. Nature Neuroscience, 6(4), 421–7. https://doi.org/10.1038/nn1031Google Scholar
Hart, Y., Noy, L., Feniger-Schaal, R., Mayo, A. E., & Alon, U. (2014). Individuality and togetherness in joint improvised motion. PLoS ONE, 9(2), e87213. https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0087213CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Heft, H. (1981). An examination of constructivist and Gibsonian approaches to environmental psychology. Population and Environment, 4(4), 227–45. https://doi.org/10.1007/BF01375628Google Scholar
Heyes, C., & Catmur, C. (2021). What happened to mirror neurons? Perspectives on Psychological Science, 174569162199063. https://doi.org/10.1177/1745691621990638Google Scholar
Hodsoll, J., Quinn, K. A., & Hodsoll, S. (2010). Attentional prioritization of infant faces is limited to own-race infants. PLOS ONE, 5(9), e12509. https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0012509Google Scholar
Hudson, M., Bach, P., & Nicholson, T. (2018). You said you would! The predictability of other’s behavior from their intentions determines predictive biases in action perception. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 44(2), 320–35. https://doi.org/10.1037/xhp0000451Google Scholar
Hügelschäfer, S., Jaudas, A., & Achtziger, A. (2016). Detecting gender before you know it: How implementation intentions control early gender categorization. Brain Research, 1649, 922. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.brainres.2016.08.026Google Scholar
Isenhower, R. W., Richardson, M. J., Carello, C., Baron, R. M., & Marsh, K. L. (2010). Affording cooperation: Embodied constraints, dynamics, and action-scaled invariance in joint lifting. Psychonomic Bulletin & Review, 17(3), 342–7. https://doi.org/10.3758/PBR.17.3.342Google Scholar
Ishak, S., Adolph, K. E., & Lin, G. C. (2008). Perceiving affordances for fitting through apertures. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 34(6), 1501–14. https://doi.org/10.1037/a0011393Google Scholar
Jack, R. E. (2013). Culture and facial expressions of emotion. Visual Cognition, 21(9–10), 1248–86. https://doi.org/10.1080/13506285.2013.835367Google Scholar
Jack, R. E., Garrod, O. G. B., Yu, H., Caldara, R., & Schyns, P. G. (2012). Facial expressions of emotion are not culturally universal. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences, 109(19), 7241–4. https://doi.org/10.1073/pnas.1200155109Google Scholar
Jacquet, P. O., & Avenanti, A. (2015). Perturbing the action observation network during perception and categorization of actions’ goals and grips: State-dependency and virtual lesion TMS effects. Cerebral Cortex, 25(3), 598608. https://doi.org/10.1093/cercor/bht242Google Scholar
James, W. (1980). The Principles of Psychology. Holt.Google Scholar
Jentzsch, I., & Dudschig, C. (2009). Short article: Why do we slow down after an error? Mechanisms underlying the effects of posterror slowing. Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 62(2), 209–18. https://doi.org/10.1080/17470210802240655CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Ji, H., & Pan, J. S. (2019). Can I choose a throwable object for you? Perceiving affordances for other individuals. Frontiers in Psychology, 10, 112. https://doi.org/10.3389/fpsyg.2019.02205Google Scholar
Kilner, J. M., & Lemon, R. N. (2013). What we know currently about mirror neurons. Current Biology, 23(23), R1057–62. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.cub.2013.10.051Google Scholar
Klein, R. M. (2000). Inhibition of return. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 4(4), 138–47. https://doi.org/10.1016/S1364-6613(00)01452-2Google Scholar
Konczak, J., Meeuwsen, H. J., & Cress, M. E. (1992). Changing affordances in stair climbing: The perception of maximum climbability in young and older adults. Journal of Experimental Psychology. Human Perception and Performance, 18(3), 691–7. https://doi.org/10.1037//0096-1523.18.3.691Google Scholar
Koriat, A., & Norman, J. (1985). Reading rotated words. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 11(4), 490508. https://doi.org/10.1037/0096-1523.11.4.490Google Scholar
Kourtis, D., Jacob, P., Sebanz, N., Sperber, D., & Knoblich, G. (2020). Making sense of human interaction benefits from communicative cues. Scientific Reports, 10(1), 18135. https://doi.org/10.1038/s41598-020-75283-3Google Scholar
Kourtis, D., Knoblich, G., Woźniak, M., & Sebanz, N. (2014). Attention allocation and task representation during joint action planning. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 26(10), 2275–86. https://doi.org/10.1162/jocn_a_00634Google Scholar
Kourtis, D., Sebanz, N., & Knoblich, G. (2013). Predictive representation of other people’s actions in joint action planning: An EEG study. Social Neuroscience, 8(1), 3142. https://doi.org/10.1080/17470919.2012.694823Google Scholar
Kourtis, D., Vandemaele, P., & Vingerhoets, G. (2018). Concurrent cortical representations of function- and size-related object affordances: An fMRI study. Cognitive, Affective, & Behavioral Neuroscience, 18(6), 1221–32. https://doi.org/10.3758/s13415-018-0633-1Google Scholar
Kramer, R. S. S., & Ward, R. (2010). Internal facial features are signals of personality and health. The Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 63(11), 2273–87. https://doi.org/10.1080/17470211003770912Google Scholar
Lakens, D., & Stel, M. (2011). If they move in sync, they must feel in sync: Movement synchrony leads to attributions of rapport and entitativity. Social Cognition, 29(1), 114. https://doi.org/10.1521/soco.2011.29.1.1Google Scholar
Leuthold, H., Sommer, W., & Ulrich, R. (2004). Preparing for action: inferences from CNV and LRP. Journal of Psychophysiology, 18(2/3), 7788. https://doi.org/10.1027/0269-8803.18.23.77CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Levin, D. T. (1996). Classifying faces by race: The structure of face categories. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 22(6), 1364–82. https://doi.org/10.1037/0278-7393.22.6.1364Google Scholar
Little, A. C., & Perrett, D. I. (2007). Using composite images to assess accuracy in personality attribution to faces. British Journal of Psychology, 98(1), 111–26. https://doi.org/10.1348/000712606x109648Google Scholar
Lobo, L., Heras-Escribano, M., & Travieso, D. (2018). The history and philosophy of ecological psychology. Frontiers in Psychology, 9, 115. https://doi.org/10.3389/fpsyg.2018.02228Google Scholar
Loehr, J. D., Kourtis, D., & Brazil, I. A. (2015). It’s not just my fault: Neural correlates of feedback processing in solo and joint action. Biological Psychology, 111, 17. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.biopsycho.2015.08.004Google Scholar
Loehr, J. D., Kourtis, D., Vesper, C., Sebanz, N., & Knoblich, G. (2013). Monitoring individual and joint action outcomes in duet music performance. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 25(7), 1049–61. https://doi.org/10.1162/jocn_a_00388Google Scholar
Mack, A., & Rock, I. (1998). Inattentional Blindness. MIT Press.Google Scholar
Maister, L., Sebanz, N., Knoblich, G., & Tsakiris, M. (2013). Experiencing ownership over a dark-skinned body reduces implicit racial bias. Cognition, 128(2), 170–8. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.cognition.2013.04.002Google Scholar
Manera, V., Becchio, C., Cavallo, A., Sartori, L., & Castiello, U. (2011). Cooperation or competition? Discriminating between social intentions by observing prehensile movements. Experimental Brain Research, 211(3), 547–56. https://doi.org/10.1007/s00221-011-2649-4Google Scholar
Manera, V., Schouten, B., Becchio, C., Bara, B. G., & Verfaillie, K. (2010). Inferring intentions from biological motion: A stimulus set of point-light communicative interactions. Behavior Research Methods, 42(1), 168–78. https://doi.org/10.3758/BRM.42.1.168Google Scholar
Marinato, G., & Baldauf, D. (2019). Object-based attention in complex, naturalistic auditory streams. Scientific Reports, 9(1), 2854–. https://doi.org/10.1038/s41598-019-39166-6Google Scholar
Mark, L. S. (1987). Eyeheight-scaled information about affordances: A study of sitting and stair climbing. Journal of Experimental Psychology. Human Perception and Performance, 13(3), 361–70. https://doi.org/10.1037//0096-1523.13.3.361Google Scholar
Mark, L. S. (2007). Perceiving the actions of other people. Ecological Psychology, 19(2), 107–36. https://doi.org/10.1080/10407410701331967Google Scholar
Mark, L. S., & Vogele, D. (1987). A biodynamic basis for perceived categories of action: A study of sitting and stair climbing. Journal of Motor Behavior, 19(3), 367–84. https://doi.org/10.1080/00222895.1987.10735418Google Scholar
Marsh, K. L., Richardson, M. J., Baron, R. M., & Schmidt, R. C. (2006). Contrasting approaches to perceiving and acting with others. Ecological Psychology, 18(1), 138. https://doi.org/10.1207/s15326969eco1801_1Google Scholar
Masangkay, Z. S., McCluskey, K. A., McIntyre, C. W., Sims-Knight, J., Vaughn, B. E., & Flavell, J. H. (1974). The early development of inferences about the visual percepts of others. Child Development, 45(2), 357–66. https://doi.org/10.2307/1127956Google Scholar
McDonough, K. L., Costantini, M., Hudson, M., & Bach, P. (2020). Affordance matching predictively shapes the perceptual representation of others’ ongoing actions. Journal of Experimental Psychology. Human Perception and Performance, 46(8), 847–59. https://doi.org/10.1037/xhp0000745Google Scholar
McEllin, L., Knoblich, G., & Sebanz, N. (2018). Distinct kinematic markers of demonstration and joint action coordination? Evidence from virtual xylophone playing. Journal of Experimental Psychology. Human Perception and Performance, 44(6), 885–97. https://doi.org/10.1037/xhp0000505Google Scholar
McEllin, L., Knoblich, G., & Sebanz, N. (2020). Synchronicities that shape the perception of joint action. Scientific Reports, 10(1), 15554. https://doi.org/10.1038/s41598-020-72729-6Google Scholar
McEllin, L., Sebanz, N., & Knoblich, G. (2018). Identifying others’ informative intentions from movement kinematics. Cognition, 180, 246–58. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.cognition.2018.08.001Google Scholar
McGuigan, N., & Doherty, M. J. (2002). The relation between hiding skill and judgment of eye direction in preschool children. Developmental Psychology, 38(3), 418–27. https://doi.org/10.1037//0012-1649.38.3.418Google Scholar
Mehu, M., Little, A. C., & Dunbar, R. I. M. (2007). Duchenne smiles and the perception of generosity and sociability in faces. Journal of Evolutionary Psychology, 5(1), 183–96. https://doi.org/10.1556/jep.2007.1011Google Scholar
Meltzoff, A. N., & Brooks, R. (2007). Eyes wide shut: The importance of eyes in infant gaze following and understanding other minds. In R. Flom, K. Lee, and D Muir (Eds.), Gaze Following: Its Development and Significance (pp.217–41). Lawrence Erlbaum Associates.Google Scholar
Meyer, M., van der Wel, R. P. R. D., & Hunnius, S. (2013). Higher-order action planning for individual and joint object manipulations. Experimental Brain Research, 225(4), 579–88. https://doi.org/10.1007/s00221-012-3398-8Google Scholar
Michael, J., McEllin, L., & Felber, A. (2020). Prosocial effects of coordination – What, how and why? Acta Psychologica, 207, 10308311. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.actpsy.2020.103083Google Scholar
Miles, L. K., Nind, L. K., & Macrae, C. N. (2009). The rhythm of rapport: Interpersonal synchrony and social perception. Journal of Experimental Social Psychology, 45(3), 585–9. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jesp.2009.02.002Google Scholar
Mogan, R., Fischer, R., & Bulbulia, J. A. (2017). To be in synchrony or not? A meta-analysis of synchrony’s effects on behavior, perception, cognition and affect. Journal of Experimental Social Psychology, 72, 1320. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jesp.2017.03.009Google Scholar
Moll, H., & Meltzoff, A. N. (2011). How does it look? Level 2 perspective-taking at 36 months of age. Child Development, 82(2), 661–73. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1467-8624.2010.01571.xGoogle Scholar
Moll, H., & Tomasello, M. (2006). Level 1 perspective-taking at 24 months of age. British Journal of Developmental Psychology, 24(3), 603–13. https://doi.org/10.1348/026151005x55370Google Scholar
Mouchetant‐Rostaing, Y., Giard, M.-H., Bentin, S., Aguera, P.-E., & Pernier, J. (2000). Neurophysiological correlates of face gender processing in humans. European Journal of Neuroscience, 12(1), 303–10. https://doi.org/10.1046/j.1460-9568.2000.00888.xGoogle Scholar
Mundy, P. (2018). A review of joint attention and social-cognitive brain systems in typical development and autism spectrum disorder. European Journal of Neuroscience, 47(6), 497514. https://doi.org/10.1111/ejn.13720Google Scholar
Mundy, P., Block, J., Delgado, C., Pomares, Y., Hecke, A. V. V., & Parlade, M. V. (2007). Individual differences and the development of joint attention in infancy. Child Development, 78(3), 938–54. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1467-8624.2007.01042.xGoogle Scholar
Naumann, L. P., Vazire, S., Rentfrow, P. J., & Gosling, S. D. (2009). Personality judgments based on physical appearance. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 35(12), 1661–71. https://doi.org/10.1177/0146167209346309Google Scholar
Navon, D. (1977). Forest before trees: The precedence of global features in visual perception. Cognitive Psychology, 9(3), 353–83. https://doi.org/10.1016/0010-0285(77)90012-3Google Scholar
Nobre, A. C. (Kia). (2018). Attention. In Wixted, J. T. (Ed.), Stevens’ Handbook of Experimental Psychology and Cognitive Neuroscience. John Wiley & Sons, Inc. https://doi.org/10.1002/9781119170174Google Scholar
Nobre, A. C. (Kia), & Kastner, S. (Eds.). (2014a). The Oxford Handbook of Attention(pp. 241–316). Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Nobre, A. C. (Kia), & Kastner, S. (2014b). Attention: Time capsule 2013. In A. C. (Kia) Nobre & S. Kastner (Eds.), The Oxford Handbook of Attention (Vol. 1). Oxford University Press. https://doi.org/10.1093/oxfordhb/9780199675111.013.040Google Scholar
Norman, J. (2002). Two visual systems and two theories of perception: An attempt to reconcile the constructivist and ecological approaches. Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 25(1), 7396. https://doi.org/10.1017/s0140525x0200002xGoogle Scholar
Noy, L., Dekel, E., & Alon, U. (2011). The mirror game as a paradigm for studying the dynamics of two people improvising motion together. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences, 108(52), 20947–52. https://doi.org/10.1073/pnas.1108155108Google Scholar
Olivola, C. Y., & Todorov, A. (2010). Elected in 100 milliseconds: Appearance-based trait inferences and voting. Journal of Nonverbal Behavior, 34(2), 83110. https://doi.org/10.1007/s10919-009-0082-1Google Scholar
Osiurak, F., & Badets, A. (2016). Tool use and affordance: Manipulation-based versus reasoning-based approaches. Psychological Review, 123(5), 534–68. https://doi.org/10.1037/rev0000027Google Scholar
Oxley, J. A., Ihsen, E., Fildes, B. N., Charlton, J. L., & Day, R. H. (2005). Crossing roads safely: An experimental study of age differences in gap selection by pedestrians. Accident Analysis & Prevention, 37(5), 962–71. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.aap.2005.04.017Google Scholar
Papeo, L. (2020). Twos in human visual perception. Cortex, 132, 473–8. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.cortex.2020.06.005Google Scholar
Papeo, L., & Abassi, E. (2019). Seeing social events: The visual specialization for dyadic human–human interactions. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 45(7), 877–88. https://doi.org/10.1037/xhp0000646Google Scholar
Papeo, L., Goupil, N., & Soto-Faraco, S. (2019). Visual search for people among people. Psychological Science, 30(10), 1483–96. https://doi.org/10.1177/0956797619867295Google Scholar
Papeo, L., Stein, T., & Soto-Faraco, S. (2017). The two-body inversion effect. Psychological Science, 28(3), 369–79. https://doi.org/10.1177/0956797616685769Google Scholar
Pappas, Z. (2014). Dissociating Simon and affordance compatibility effects: Silhouettes and photographs. Cognition, 133(3), 716–28. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.cognition.2014.08.018Google Scholar
Patri, J.-F., Cavallo, A., Pullar, K., Soriano, M., Valente, M., Koul, A., Avenanti, A., Panzeri, S., & Becchio, C. (2020). Transient disruption of the inferior parietal lobule impairs the ability to attribute intention to action. Current Biology, 30(23), 4594–605.e7. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.cub.2020.08.104Google Scholar
Pavlova, M., Krägeloh-Mann, I., Sokolov, A., & Birbaumer, N. (2001). Recognition of point-light biological motion displays by young children. Perception, 30(8), 925–33. https://doi.org/10.1068/p3157Google Scholar
Penton-Voak, I. S., Pound, N., Little, A. C., & Perrett, D. I. (2006). Personality judgments from natural and composite facial images: More evidence for a “kernel of truth” in social perception. Social Cognition, 24(5), 607–40. https://doi.org/10.1521/soco.2006.24.5.607Google Scholar
Pepping, G., & Li, F.-X. (1997). Perceiving action boundaries in the volleyball block. In Schmuckler, M. A. & Kennedy, J. M. (Eds.), Studies in Perception and Action IV: Ninth Annual Conference on Perception and Action (pp. 134–40). Lawrence Erlbaum Associates Inc.Google Scholar
Pesquita, A., & Enns, J. T. (2021). Are facing bodies a new gestalt or evidence of social binding? Cortex, 135, 352–4. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.cortex.2020.09.019Google Scholar
Pesquita, A., Whitwell, R. L., & Enns, J. T. (2018). Predictive joint-action model: A hierarchical predictive approach to human cooperation. Psychonomic Bulletin & Review, 25(5), 1751–69. https://doi.org/10.3758/s13423-017-1393-6Google Scholar
Pezzulo, G., Donnarumma, F., & Dindo, H. (2013). Human sensorimotor communication: A theory of signaling in online socialiInteractions. PLOS ONE, 8(11), e79876. https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0079876Google Scholar
Pezzulo, G., Donnarumma, F., Dindo, H., D’Ausilio, A., Konvalinka, I., & Castelfranchi, C. (2019). The body talks: Sensorimotor communication and its brain and kinematic signatures. Physics of Life Reviews, 28, 121. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.plrev.2018.06.014Google Scholar
Pezzulo, G., Roche, L., & Saint-Bauzel, L. (2021). Haptic communication optimises joint decisions and affords implicit confidence sharing. Scientific Reports, 11(1), 10511. https://doi.org/10.1038/s41598-020-80041-6Google Scholar
Picton, L., Saunders, B., & Jentzsch, I. (2012). “I will fix only my own mistakes”: An ERP study investigating error processing in a joint choice-RT task. Neuropsychologia, 50(5), 777–85. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2012.01.011Google Scholar
Pobric, G., & de C. Hamilton, A. F. (2006). Action understanding requires the left inferior frontal cortex. Current Biology, 16(5), 524–9. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.cub.2006.01.033Google Scholar
Pomiechowska, B., & Csibra, G. (2017). Motor activation during action perception depends on action interpretation. Neuropsychologia, 105, 8491. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2017.01.032Google Scholar
Posner, M. I. (1980). Orienting of attention. Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 32(1), 325. https://doi.org/10.1080/00335558008248231Google Scholar
Posner, M. I., & Cohen, Y. (1984). Components of visual orienting. In Bouma, H. & Bouwhuis, D. G. (Eds.), Attention and Performance X: Control of Language Processes (Vol. 32, pp. 531–56). L. Erlbaum Associates.Google Scholar
Prinz, W. (1990). A common coding approach to perception and action. In Neumann, O. & Prinz, W. (Eds.), Relationships between Perception and Action: Current Approaches (pp. 167201). Springer. https://doi.org/10.1007/978-3-642-75348-0_7Google Scholar
Prinz, W. (1997). Perception and action planning. European Journal of Cognitive Psychology, 9(2), 129–54. https://doi.org/10.1080/713752551Google Scholar
Prinz, W., & Bridgeman, B. (1995). Handbook of Perception and Action. Academic Press.Google Scholar
Proctor, R. W., Lien, M. C., & Thompson, L. (2017). Do silhouettes and photographs produce fundamentally different object-based correspondence effects? Cognition, 169, 91101. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.cognition.2017.08.009Google Scholar
Proverbio, A. M., Adorni, R., & D’Aniello, G. E. (2011). 250 ms to code for action affordance during observation of manipulable objects. Neuropsychologia, 49(9), 2711–17. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2011.05.019Google Scholar
Quinn, P. C., Yahr, J., Kuhn, A., Slater, A. M., & Pascalis, O. (2002). Representation of the gender of human faces by infants: A preference for female. Perception, 31(9), 1109–21. https://doi.org/10.1068/p3331Google Scholar
Raffard, S., Salesse, R. N., Marin, L., Del-Monte, J., Schmidt, R. C., Varlet, M., Bardy, B. G., Boulenger, J.-P., & Capdevielle, D. (2015). Social priming enhances interpersonal synchronization and feeling of connectedness towards schizophrenia patients. Scientific Reports, 5(1), 81561. https://doi.org/10.1038/srep08156Google Scholar
Ramenzoni, V. C., Davis, T. J., Riley, M. A., & Shockley, K. (2010). Perceiving action boundaries: Learning effects in perceiving maximum jumping-reach affordances. Attention, Perception, & Psychophysics, 72(4), 1110–19. https://doi.org/10.3758/APP.72.4.1110Google Scholar
Ramenzoni, V. C., Riley, M. A., Davis, T., Shockley, K., & Armstrong, R. (2008). Tuning in to another person’s action capabilities: Perceiving maximal jumping-reach height from walking kinematics. Journal of Experimental Psychology. Human Perception and Performance, 34(4), 919–28. https://doi.org/10.1037/0096-1523.34.4.919Google Scholar
Ramenzoni, V. C., Riley, M. A., Shockley, K., & Davis, T. (2008a). An information-based approach to action understanding. Cognition, 106(2), 1059–70. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.cognition.2007.04.008Google Scholar
Ramenzoni, V. C., Riley, M. A., Shockley, K., & Davis, T. (2008b). Carrying the height of the world on your ankles: Encumbering observers reduces estimates of how high an actor can jump. The Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 61(10), 1487–95. https://doi.org/10.1080/17470210802100073Google Scholar
Ray, M., de Grosbois, J., & Welsh, T. N. (2017). Index of difficulty and side of space are accommodated during the selection and planning of a joint action. Human Movement Science, 54, 197209. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.humov.2017.05.009Google Scholar
Richardson, D. C., & Dale, R. (2005). Looking to understand: The coupling between speakers’ and listeners’ eye movements and its relationship to discourse comprehension. Cognitive Science, 29(6), 1045–60. https://doi.org/10.1207/s15516709cog0000_29Google Scholar
Richardson, D. C., Dale, R., & Kirkham, N. Z. (2007). The art of conversation is coordination: Common ground and the coupling of eye movements during dialogue. Psychological Science, 18(5), 407–13. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1467-9280.2007.01914.xGoogle Scholar
Richardson, M. J., Marsh, K. L., & Baron, R. M. (2007). Judging and actualizing intrapersonal and interpersonal affordances. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 33(4), 845–59. https://doi.org/10.1037/0096-1523.33.4.845Google ScholarPubMed
Richardson, M. J., Marsh, K. L., Isenhower, R. W., Goodman, J. R. L., & Schmidt, R. C. (2007). Rocking together: Dynamics of intentional and unintentional interpersonal coordination. Human Movement Science, 26(6), 867–91. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.humov.2007.07.002Google Scholar
Rizzolatti, G., Fogassi, L., & Gallese, V. (2001). Neurophysiological mechanisms underlying the understanding and imitation of action. Nature Reviews Neuroscience, 2(9), 661–70. https://doi.org/10.1038/35090060Google Scholar
Rock, I. (1983). The Logic of Perception. MIT Press.Google Scholar
Roether, C. L., Omlor, L., Christensen, A., & Giese, M. A. (2009). Critical features for the perception of emotion from gait. Journal of Vision, 9(6), 111–32. https://doi.org/10.1167/9.6.15Google Scholar
Sabu, S., Curioni, A., Vesper, C., Sebanz, N., & Knoblich, G. (2020). How does a partner’s motor variability affect joint action? PLOS ONE, 15(10), e0241417. https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0241417Google Scholar
Sacheli, L. M., Musco, M. A., Zazzera, E., & Paulesu, E. (2021). Mechanisms for mutual support in motor interactions. Scientific Reports, 11(1), 30601. https://doi.org/10.1038/s41598-021-82138-yGoogle Scholar
Sacheli, L. M., Tidoni, E., Pavone, E. F., Aglioti, S. M., & Candidi, M. (2013). Kinematics fingerprints of leader and follower role-taking during cooperative joint actions. Experimental Brain Research, 226(4), 473–86. https://doi.org/10.1007/s00221-013-3459-7Google Scholar
Samson, D., Apperly, I. A., Braithwaite, J. J., Andrews, B. J., & Bodley Scott, S. E. (2010). Seeing it their way: Evidence for rapid and involuntary computation of what other people see. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 36(5), 1255–66. https://doi.org/10.1037/a0018729Google Scholar
Santamaria, J. P., & Rosenbaum, D. A. (2011). Etiquette and effort: Holding doors for others. Psychological Science, 22(5), 584–8. https://doi.org/10.1177/0956797611406444Google Scholar
Schenke, K. C., Wyer, N. A., & Bach, P. (2016). The things you do: Internal models of others’ expected behaviour guide action observation. PLOS ONE, 11(7), e0158910. https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0158910Google Scholar
Schmitz, L., Vesper, C., Sebanz, N., & Knoblich, G. (2017). Co-representation of others’ task constraints in joint action. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 43(8), 1480–93. https://doi.org/10.1037/xhp0000403Google Scholar
Schmitz, L., Vesper, C., Sebanz, N., & Knoblich, G. (2018). Co-actors represent the order of each other’s actions. Cognition, 181, 6579. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.cognition.2018.08.008Google Scholar
Schuch, S., & Tipper, S. P. (2007). On observing another person’s actions: Influences of observed inhibition and errors. Perception & Psychophysics, 69(5), 828–37. https://doi.org/10.3758/BF03193782Google Scholar
Scorolli, C., Miatton, M., Wheaton, L. A., & Borghi, A. M. (2014). I give you a cup, I get a cup: A kinematic study on social intention. Neuropsychologia, 57(1), 196204. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2014.03.006Google Scholar
Sebanz, N., & Knoblich, G. (2009). Prediction in joint action: What, when, and where. Topics in Cognitive Science, 1(2), 353–67. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1756-8765.2009.01024.xGoogle Scholar
Sebanz, N., & Knoblich, G. (2021). Progress in joint-action research. Current Directions in Psychological Science, 30(2), 0963721420984425. https://doi.org/10.1177/0963721420984425Google Scholar
Shepard, R. N., & Metzler, J. (1971). Mental rotation of three-dimensional objects. Science, 171(3972), 701–3. https://doi.org/10.1126/science.171.3972.701Google Scholar
Simons, D. J. (2000). Attentional capture and inattentional blindness. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 4(4), 147–55. https://doi.org/10.1016/S1364-6613(00)01455-8Google Scholar
Simons, D. J., & Chabris, C. F. (1999). Gorillas in our midst: Sustained inattentional blindness for dynamic events. Perception, 28(9), 1059–74. https://doi.org/10.1068/p281059Google Scholar
Skantze, G., Hjalmarsson, A., & Oertel, C. (2014). Turn-taking, feedback and joint attention in situated human–robot interaction. Speech Communication, 65, 5066. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.specom.2014.05.005Google Scholar
Skarratt, P. A., Cole, G. G., & Kingstone, A. (2010). Social inhibition of return. Acta Psychologica, 134(1), 4854. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.actpsy.2009.12.003Google Scholar
Sokolov, E. N., Spinks, J. A., Näätänen, R., & Lyytinen, H. (2002). The Orienting Response in Information Processing. Lawrence Erlbaum Associates Publishers.Google Scholar
Stoffregen, T. A., Gorday, K. M., Sheng, Y.-Y., & Flynn, S. B. (1999). Perceiving affordances for another person’s actions. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 25(1), 120–36. https://doi.org/10.1037/0096-1523.25.1.120Google Scholar
Stoffregen, T. A., Yang, C.-M., & Bardy, B. G. (2005). Affordance judgments and nonlocomotor body movement. Ecological Psychology, 17(2), 75104. https://doi.org/10.1207/s15326969eco1702_2Google Scholar
Strachan, J. W. A., Sebanz, N., & Knoblich, G. (2019). The role of emotion in the dyad inversion effect. PLOS ONE, 14(7), e0219185. https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0219185Google Scholar
Streuber, S., Knoblich, G., Sebanz, N., Bülthoff, H. H., & de la Rosa, S. (2011). The effect of social context on the use of visual information. Experimental Brain Research, 214(2), 273–84. https://doi.org/10.1007/s00221-011-2830-9Google Scholar
Surtees, A., & Apperly, I. A. (2012). Egocentrism and automatic perspective taking in children and adults. Child Development, 83(2), 452–60. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1467-8624.2011.01730.xGoogle Scholar
Surtees, A., Apperly, I. A., & Samson, D. (2013a). The use of embodied self-rotation for visual and spatial perspective-taking. Frontiers in Human Neuroscience, 7, 112. https://doi.org/10.3389/fnhum.2013.00698Google Scholar
Surtees, A., Apperly, I., & Samson, D. (2013b). Similarities and differences in visual and spatial perspective-taking processes. Cognition, 129(2), 426–38. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.cognition.2013.06.008Google Scholar
Surtees, A., Samson, D., & Apperly, I. (2016). Unintentional perspective-taking calculates whether something is seen, but not how it is seen. Cognition, 148, 97105. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.cognition.2015.12.010Google Scholar
Sutherland, C. A. M., Young, A. W., & Rhodes, G. (2017). Facial first impressions from another angle: How social judgements are influenced by changeable and invariant facial properties. British Journal of Psychology, 108(2), 397415. https://doi.org/10.1111/bjop.12206Google Scholar
Thomaz, A., Hoffman, G., & Cakmak, M. (2016). Computational human-robot interaction. Foundations and Trends® in Robotics, 4(2–3), 105223. https://doi.org/10.1561/2300000049Google Scholar
Thompson, E. L., Bird, G., & Catmur, C. (2019). Conceptualizing and testing action understanding. Neuroscience & Biobehavioral Reviews, 105, 106–14. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neubiorev.2019.08.002Google Scholar
Tipper, S. P., Howard, L. A., & Jackson, S. R. (1997). Selective reaching to grasp: Evidence for distractor interference effects. Visual Cognition, 4(1), 138. https://doi.org/10.1080/713756749Google Scholar
Todorov, A., Mandisodza, A. N., Goren, A., & Hall, C. C. (2005). Inferences of competence from faces predict election outcomes. Science, 308(5728), 1623–26. https://doi.org/10.1126/science.1110589Google Scholar
Tomasello, M. (1995). Joint attention as social cognition. In C. Moore & P. J. Dunham (Eds.), Joint Attention: Its Origins and Role in Development (pp. 103130). Lawrence Erlbaum Associates, Inc.Google Scholar
Tomasello, M., & Carpenter, M. (2007). Shared intentionality. Developmental Science, 10(1), 121–5. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1467-7687.2007.00573.xGoogle Scholar
Török, G., Pomiechowska, B., Csibra, G., & Sebanz, N. (2019). Rationality in joint action: Maximizing coefficiency in coordination. Psychological Science, 30(6), 930–41. https://doi.org/10.1177/0956797619842550Google Scholar
Treisman, A. M., & Gelade, G. (1980). A feature-integration theory of attention. Cognitive Psychology, 12(1), 97136. https://doi.org/10.1016/0010-0285(80)90005-5Google Scholar
Trevisan, D. A., Enns, J. T., Birmingham, E., & Iarocci, G. (2021). Action coordination during a real-world task: Evidence from children with and without autism spectrum disorder. Development and Psychopathology, 33(1), 6575. https://doi.org/10.1017/S0954579419001561Google Scholar
Tucker, M., & Ellis, R. (1998). On the relations between seen objects and components of potential actions. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 24(3), 830–46. https://doi.org/10.1037//0096-1523.24.3.830Google Scholar
Valdesolo, P., Ouyang, J., & DeSteno, D. (2010). The rhythm of joint action: Synchrony promotes cooperative ability. Journal of Experimental Social Psychology, 46(4), 693–5. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jesp.2010.03.004Google Scholar
van der Meer, A. L. H. (1997). Visual guidance of passing under a barrier. Early Development and Parenting, 6(3–4), 149–58. https://doi.org/10.1002/1099-0917(199709/12)6:3/4<149::aid-edp154>3.0.co;2-2(sici)Google Scholar
van der Wel, R. P. R. D., Knoblich, G., & Sebanz, N. (2011). Let the force be with us: Dyads exploit haptic coupling for coordination. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 37(5), 1420–31. https://doi.org/10.1037/a0022337Google Scholar
van Schie, H. T., Mars, R. B., Coles, M. G. H., & Bekkering, H. (2004). Modulation of activity in medial frontal and motor cortices during error observation. Nature Neuroscience, 7(5), 549–54. https://doi.org/10.1038/nn1239Google Scholar
Vesper, C., Butterfill, S., Knoblich, G., & Sebanz, N. (2010). A minimal architecture for joint action. Neural Networks, 23(8), 9981003. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neunet.2010.06.002Google Scholar
Vesper, C., & Richardson, M. J. (2014). Strategic communication and behavioral coupling in asymmetric joint action. Experimental Brain Research, 232(9), 2945–56. https://doi.org/10.1007/s00221-014-3982-1Google Scholar
Vesper, C., Schmitz, L., Safra, L., Sebanz, N., & Knoblich, G. (2016). The role of shared visual information for joint action coordination. Cognition, 153, 118–23. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.cognition.2016.05.002Google Scholar
Vesper, C., Soutschek, A., & Schubö, A. (2009). Motion coordination affects movement parameters in a joint pick-and-place task. Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 62(12), 2418–32. https://doi.org/10.1080/17470210902919067Google Scholar
Vesper, C., van der Wel, R. P. R. D., Knoblich, G., & Sebanz, N. (2011). Making oneself predictable: Reduced temporal variability facilitates joint action coordination. Experimental Brain Research, 211(3), 517–30. https://doi.org/10.1007/s00221-011-2706-zGoogle Scholar
Vesper, C., van der Wel, R. P. R. D., Knoblich, G., & Sebanz, N. (2013). Are you ready to jump? Predictive mechanisms in interpersonal coordination. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 39(1), 4861. https://doi.org/10.1037/a0028066Google ScholarPubMed
Vestner, T., Gray, K. L. H., & Cook, R. (2020). Why are social interactions found quickly in visual search tasks? Cognition, 200, 104270. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.cognition.2020.104270Google Scholar
Vestner, T., Over, H., Gray, K. L. H., Tipper, S. P., & Cook, R. (2021). Searching for people: Non-facing distractor pairs hinder the visual search of social scenes more than facing distractor pairs. Cognition, 214, 104737. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.cognition.2021.104737Google Scholar
Vestner, T., Tipper, S. P., Hartley, T., Over, H., & Rueschemeyer, S.-A. (2019). Bound together: Social binding leads to faster processing, spatial distortion, and enhanced memory of interacting partners. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 148(7), 1251–68. https://doi.org/10.1037/xge0000545Google Scholar
Voinov, P. V., Sebanz, N., & Knoblich, G. (2017). Perceptual judgments made better by indirect interactions: Evidence from a joint localization task. PLOS ONE, 12(11), e0187428. https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0187428Google Scholar
Voinov, P. V., Sebanz, N., & Knoblich, G. (2019). Collective benefit in joint perceptual judgments: Partial roles of shared environments, meta-cognition, and feedback. Cognition, 189, 116–30. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.cognition.2019.03.016Google Scholar
von Helmholtz, H. (1962). Treatise on Physiological Optics (J. P. C. Southall, Ed. & Trans.; Vol. 3). Dover Publications.Google Scholar
Wagman, J. B. (2012). Perception of maximum reaching height reflects impending changes in reaching ability and improvements transfer to unpracticed reaching tasks. Experimental Brain Research, 219(4), 467–76. https://doi.org/10.1007/s00221-012-3104-xGoogle Scholar
Wagman, J. B., Stoffregen, T. A., Bai, J., & Schloesser, D. S. (2018). Perceiving nested affordances for another person’s actions. Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 71(3), 790–9. https://doi.org/10.1080/17470218.2016.1277249Google Scholar
Wagman, J. B., & Taylor, K. R. (2005). Perceiving affordances for aperture crossing for the person-plus-object system. Ecological Psychology, 17(2), 105–30. https://doi.org/10.1207/s15326969eco1702_3Google Scholar
Ward, E., Ganis, G., & Bach, P. (2019). Spontaneous vicarious perception of the content of another’s visual perspective. Current Biology, 29(5), 874–80.e4. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.cub.2019.01.046Google Scholar
Ward, E., Ganis, G., McDonough, K. L., & Bach, P. (2020). Perspective taking as virtual navigation? Perceptual simulation of what others see reflects their location in space but not their gaze. Cognition, 199, 1042411. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.cognition.2020.104241Google Scholar
Warren, W. H. (1984). Perceiving affordances: Visual guidance of stair climbing. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 10(5), 683703. https://doi.org/10.1037/0096-1523.10.5.683Google Scholar
Warren, W. H., & Whang, S. (1987). Visual guidance of walking through apertures: Body-scaled information for affordances. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 13(3), 371–83. https://doi.org/10.1037/0096-1523.13.3.371Google Scholar
Welsh, T. N., Wong, L., & Chandrasekharan, S. (2013). Factors that affect action possibility judgments: The assumed abilities of other people. Acta Psychologica, 143(2), 235–44. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.actpsy.2013.04.003Google Scholar
Wiese, H., Schweinberger, S. R., & Neumann, M. F. (2008). Perceiving age and gender in unfamiliar faces: Brain potential evidence for implicit and explicit person categorization. Psychophysiology, 45(6), 957–69. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1469-8986.2008.00707.xGoogle Scholar
Wilson, M., & Knoblich, G. (2005). The case for motor involvement in perceiving conspecifics. Psychological Bulletin, 131(3), 460–73. https://doi.org/10.1037/0033-2909.131.3.460Google Scholar
Wilson, S., & Gos, C. (2019). Perceiving social cohesion: Movement synchrony and task demands both matter. Perception, 48(4), 316–29. https://doi.org/10.1177/0301006619837878Google Scholar
Wolf, T., Sebanz, N., & Knoblich, G. (2018). Joint action coordination in expert-novice pairs: Can experts predict novices’ suboptimal timing? Cognition, 178, 103–8. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.cognition.2018.05.012Google Scholar
Zhao, L., & Bentin, S. (2008). Own- and other-race categorization of faces by race, gender, and age. Psychonomic Bulletin & Review, 15(6), 1093–9. https://doi.org/10.3758/PBR.15.6.1093Google Scholar
Zwickel, J., & Müller, H. J. (2013). On the relation between spontaneous perspective taking and other visuospatial processes. Memory & Cognition, 41(4), 558–70. https://doi.org/10.3758/s13421-012-0283-4Google Scholar

Save element to Kindle

To save this element to your Kindle, first ensure coreplatform@cambridge.org is added to your Approved Personal Document E-mail List under your Personal Document Settings on the Manage Your Content and Devices page of your Amazon account. Then enter the ‘name’ part of your Kindle email address below. Find out more about saving to your Kindle.

Note you can select to save to either the @free.kindle.com or @kindle.com variations. ‘@free.kindle.com’ emails are free but can only be saved to your device when it is connected to wi-fi. ‘@kindle.com’ emails can be delivered even when you are not connected to wi-fi, but note that service fees apply.

Find out more about the Kindle Personal Document Service.

Perception and Action in a Social Context
Available formats
×

Save element to Dropbox

To save content items to your account, please confirm that you agree to abide by our usage policies. If this is the first time you use this feature, you will be asked to authorise Cambridge Core to connect with your account. Find out more about saving content to Dropbox.

Perception and Action in a Social Context
Available formats
×

Save element to Google Drive

To save content items to your account, please confirm that you agree to abide by our usage policies. If this is the first time you use this feature, you will be asked to authorise Cambridge Core to connect with your account. Find out more about saving content to Google Drive.

Perception and Action in a Social Context
Available formats
×