Skip to main content Accessibility help
×
Hostname: page-component-7c8c6479df-8mjnm Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-03-29T09:34:50.176Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

10 - A brain system for declarative memory

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  08 August 2009

Seth J. Ramus
Affiliation:
Department of Psychology and Program in Neuroscience Bowdoin College Brunswick, ME 04011
Howard B. Eichenbaum
Affiliation:
Director Cognitive Neurobiology Laboratory; Director Center for Memory and Brain; University Professor and Chairman Department of Psychology Boston University Center for Memory and Brain 2 Cummington Street Boston, MA 02215
James R. Pomerantz
Affiliation:
Rice University, Houston
Get access

Summary

Our understanding about the brain system that mediates memory began in the 1950s with the landmark case study of patient HM (Scoville & Milner, 1957). To relieve epilepsy that was intractable to pharmacological intervention, surgeons removed a large part of this patient's temporal lobes, including the amygdala, part of the hippocampus, and the cortex immediately surrounding the hippocampus and amygdala. Following surgery, HM exhibited a severe amnesia, leaving nonmemory aspects of intelligence and cognition intact. This observation demonstrated that memory could be separated from other cognitive functions and that structures of the medial temporal lobe are critical to memory.

While the early neuropsychological reports clearly pointed to the importance of the temporal lobes in memory, there was debate over precisely which temporal structures were important. Because the available clinical cases did not provide highly specific anatomical resolution, efforts were made to develop animal models in which experimental brain lesions could be performed with the necessary anatomical specificity. However, the early efforts to model amnesia in monkeys and rats did not yield a consistent pattern of severe and selective amnesia, precluding useful insights into the anatomical identification of the memory system. With hindsight, it is now clear that the difficulty in characterizing the brain system responsible for memory arose for two reasons (Eichenbaum et al., 2000). First, while the memory deficit following medial temporal damage was initially thought to be global in nature, it is now understood that damage to the medial temporal region causes amnesia that is limited to a specific domain of memory, and that other brain systems mediate other types of memory.

Type
Chapter
Information
Topics in Integrative Neuroscience
From Cells to Cognition
, pp. 265 - 298
Publisher: Cambridge University Press
Print publication year: 2008

Access options

Get access to the full version of this content by using one of the access options below. (Log in options will check for institutional or personal access. Content may require purchase if you do not have access.)

References

Alvarez, P. and Squire, L. R. (1994). Memory consolidation and the medial temporal lobe: a simple network model. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences USA, 91, 7041–5.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Alvarez, P., Zola-Morgan, S., and Squire, L. R. (1994). The animal model of human amnesia: long-term memory impaired and short-term memory intact. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences USA, 91, 5637–41.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Amaral, D. G. and Witter, M. P. (1989). The three-dimensional organization of the hippocampal formation: a review of anatomical data. Neuroscience, 31, 571–91.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Bechara, A., Tranel, D., Damasio, H. D., et al. (1995). Double dissociation of conditioning and declarative knowledge relative to the amygdala and hippocampus in humans. Science, 269, 1115–18.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Berger, T. W., Alger, B. E., and Thompson, R. F. (1976). Neuronal substrates of classical conditioning in the hippocampus. Science, 192, 483–5.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Bliss, T. V. P. and Collingridge, G. L. (1993). A synaptic model of memory: long-term potentiation in the hippocampus. Nature, 361, 31–9.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Buckner, R. L. and Wheeler, M. E. (2001). The cognitive neuroscience of remembering. Nature Reviews, Neuroscience, 2, 624–34.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Buffalo, E. A., Ramus, S. J., Clark, R. E., et al. (1999). Dissociation between the effects of damage to perirhinal cortex and area TE. Learning and Memory, 6, 572–99.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Buffalo, E. A., Ramus, S. J., Squire, L. R., and Zola, S. M. (2000). Perception and recognition memory in monkeys following lesions of area TE and perirhinal cortex. Learning and Memory, 7, 375–82.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Buffalo, E. A., Reber, P. J., and Squire, L. R. (1998). The human perirhinal cortex and recognition memory. Hippocampus, 8, 330–9.3.0.CO;2-L>CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Bunsey, M. and Eichenbaum, H. (1995). Selective damage to the hippocampal region blocks long-term retention of a natural and nonspatial stimulus-stimulus association. Hippocampus, 5, 546–56.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Bunsey, M. and Eichenbaum, H. (1996). Conservation of hippocampal memory function in rats and humans. Nature, 379, 255–7.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Burwell, R. D. and Amaral, D. G. (1998). Cortical afferents of the perirhinal, postrhinal and entorhinal cortices. Journal of Comparative Neurology, 398, 179–205.3.0.CO;2-Y>CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Burwell, R. D., Witter, M. P., and Amaral, D. G. (1995). Perirhinal and postrhinal cortices in the rat: a review of the neuroanatomical literature and comparison with findings from the monkey brain. Hippocampus, 5, 390–408.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Cohen, N. J. and Eichenbaum, H. (1993). Memory, Amnesia, and the Hippocampal System. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.Google Scholar
Cohen, N. J. and Squire, L. R. (1980). Preserved learning and retention of a pattern-analyzing skill in amnesia: dissociation of knowing how and knowing that. Science, 210, 207–10.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Corkin, S. (1984). Lasting consequences of bilateral medial temporal lobectomy: clinical course and experimental findings in H. M. Seminars in Neurology, 4, 249–59.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Deacon, T. W., Eichenbaum, H., Rosenberg, P., and Eckman, K. W. (1983). Afferent connections of the perirhinal cortex in the rat. Journal of Comparative Neurology, 220, 168–290.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Drachman, D. A. and Arbit, J. (1966). Memory and the hippocampal complex. II. Is memory a multiple process? Archives of Neurology, 15, 52–61.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Dusek, J. A. and Eichenbaum, H. (1997). The hippocampus and memory for orderly stimulus relations. Proceedings of the National Academy of Science (United States of America), 94, 7109–14.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Eichenbaum, H. (1997). Declarative memory: insights from cognitive neurobiology. Annual Review of Psychology, 48, 547–72.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Eichenbaum, H. (2000). A cortical-hippocampal system for declarative memory. Nature Reviews, Neuroscience, 1, 41–50.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Eichenbaum, H., Alvarez, P., and Ramus, S. J. (2000). Animal models of amnesia. In Cermak, L. S., ed., Memory and its Disorders. Vol. 2 of Handbook of Neuropsychology, 2nd edn. Amsterdam: Elsevier, pp. 1–24.Google Scholar
Eichenbaum, H., Dudchenko, P., Wood, E., Shapiro, M., and Tanila, H. (1999). The hippocampus, memory, and place cells: is it spatial memory or memory space? Neuron, 23, 1–20.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Eichenbaum, H., Stewart, C., and Morris, R. G. M. (1990). Hippocampal representation in spatial learning. The Journal of Neuroscience, 10, 331–9.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Fahy, F. L., Riches, I. P., and Brown, M. W. (1993). Neuronal activity related to visual recognition memory: long-term memory and encoding of recency and familiarity information in the primate anterior and medial inferior temporal and rhinal cortex. Experimental Brain Research, 96, 457–72.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Fletcher, P. C., Frith, C. D., and Rugg, M. D. (1997). The functional neuroanatomy of episodic memory. Trends in Neuroscience, 20, 213–18.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Fried, I., MacDonald, K. A., and Wilson, C. L. (1997). Single neuron activity in human hippocampus and amygdala during recognition of faces and objects. Neuron, 18, 753–65.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Funahashi, S., Bruce, C. J., and Goldman-Rakic, P. S. (1989). Mnemonic coding of visual space in the primate dorsolateral prefrontal cortex. Journal of Neurophysiology, 61, 331–49.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Fuster, J. (1995). Memory in the Cerebral Cortex. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.Google Scholar
Gabrieli, J. (1995). Contributions of the basal ganglia to skill learning and working memory in humans. In Houk, J. C., Davis, J. L., and Beiser, D. G., eds., Models of Information Processing in the Basal Ganglia. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press, pp. 227–94.Google Scholar
Galef, B. G., Mason, J. R., Preti, G., and Bean, N. J. (1988). Carbon disulfide: a semiochemical mediating socially-induced diet choice in rats. Physiology and Behavior, 42, 119–24.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Gaffan, D. (1974). Recognition impaired and association intact in the memory of monkeys after transection of the fornix. Journal of Comparative and Physiological Psychology, 86, 1100–9.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Gaffan, D. (1994). Scene-specific memory of objects: a model of episodic memory impairment in monkeys with fornix transection. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 6, 305–20.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Goldman-Rakic, P. S. (1995). Cellular basis of working memory. Neuron, 14, 477–85.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Gothard, K. M., Skaggs, W. E., and McNaughton, B. L. (1996). Dynamics of mismatch correction in the hippocampal ensemble code for space: interaction between path integration and environmental cues. The Journal of Neuroscience, 16, 8027–40.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Graf, P. and Schacter, D. L. (1985). Implicit and explicit memory for new association in normal and amnesic subjects. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory and Cognition, 11, 501–18.Google ScholarPubMed
Hallet, M., Pascual-Leone, A., and Topka, H. (1996). Adaptation and skill learning: evidence for different neural substrates. In Bloedel, J. R., Ebner, T. J., and Wise, S. P., eds., The Acquisition of Motor Behavior in Vertebrates. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press, pp. 289–302.Google Scholar
Hampson, R. E., Heyser, C. J., and Deadwyler, A. (1993). Hippocampal cell firing correlates of delayed-match-to-sample performance in the rat. Behavioral Neuroscience, 107, 715–39.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Hampson, R. E., Simeral, J. D., and Deadwyler, A. (1999). Distribution of spatial and nonspatial information in dorsal hippocampus. Nature, 402, 610–14.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Higuchi, S. and Miyashita, Y. (1996). Formation of mnemonic neural responses to visual paired associations in inferotemporal cortex is impaired by perirhinal and entorhinal lesions. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences USA, 93, 739–43.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Insausti, R., Amaral, D. G., and Cowan, W. M. (1987). The entorhinal cortex of the monkey: II. Cortical afferents. Journal of Comparative Neurology, 264, 356–95.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Knowlton, B. J., Mangels, J. A., and Squire, L. R. (1996). A neostriatal habit learning system in humans. Science, 273, 1399–401.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
LeDoux, J. E. (1994). Emotion, memory and the brain. Scientific American, 270, 32–9.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Markus, E. J., Qin, Y.-L., Leonard, B., et al. (1995). Interactions between location and task affect the spatial and directional firing of hippocampal neurons. The Journal of Neuroscience, 15, 7079–94.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Martin-Elkins, C. L. and Horel, J. A. (1992). Cortical afferents to behaviorally defined regions of the inferior temporal and parahippocampal gyri as demonstrated by WGA-HRP. Journal of Comparative Neurology, 321, 177–92.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
McClelland, J. L., McNaughton, B. L., and O'Reilly, R. C. (1995). Why there are complementary learning systems in the hippocampus and neocortex: insights from the successes and failures of connectionist models of learning and memory. Psychological Review, 102, 419–57.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
McNaughton, B. L., Barnes, C. A., and O'Keefe, J. (1983). The contributions of position, direction, and velocity to single unit activity in the hippocampus of freely-moving rats. Experimental Brain Research, 52, 41–9.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Messinger, A., Squire, L. R., Zola, S. M., and Albright, T. D. (2001). Neuronal representations of stimulus associations develop in the temporal lobe during learning. Proceedings of the National Academy of Science USA, 98, 12239–44.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Meunier, M., Bachevalier, J., Mishkin, M., and Murray, E. A. (1993). Effects on visual recognition of combined and separate ablations of the entorhinal and perirhinal cortex in rhesus monkeys. The Journal of Neuroscience, 13, 5418–32.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Miller, E. K. (2000). The prefrontal cortex and cognitive control. Nature Reviews, Neuroscience, 1, 9–65.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Miller, E. K., Erickson, C. A., and Desimone, R. (1996). Neural mechanism of visual working memory in prefrontal cortex of the macaque. The Journal of Neuroscience, 16, 5154–67.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Miller, E. K., Li, L., and Desimone, R. (1991). A neural mechanism for working and recognition memory in inferior temporal cortex. Science, 254, 1377–9.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Miller, E. K., Li, L., and Desimone, R. (1993). Activity of neurons in anterior inferior temporal cortex during a short-term memory task. The Journal of Neuroscience, 13, 1460–78.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Milner, B. (1971). Interhemispheric differences in the localization of psychological process in man. British Medical Bulletin, 27, 272–7.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Milner, B. (1974). Hemispheric specialization: scope and limits. In Schmitt, F. O. and Worden, F. G., eds., The Neurosciences: Third Research Program. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.Google Scholar
Mishkin, M. (1978). Memory in monkeys severely impaired by combined but not separate removal of the amygdala and hippocampus. Nature, 273, 297–8.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Mishkin, M. and Delacour, J. (1975). An analysis of short-term visual memory in the monkey. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Animal Behavior Processes, 1, 326–34.Google ScholarPubMed
Morris, R. G. M., Garrud, P., Rawlins, J. P., and O'Keefe, J. (1982). Place navigation impaired in rats with hippocampal lesions. Nature, 297, 681–3.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Muller, R. U., Bostock, E., Taube, J. S., and Kubie, J. L. (1994). On the directional firing properties of hippocampal place cells. The Journal of Neuroscience, 14, 7235–51.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Mumby, D. G., Wood, E. R., and Pinel, J. P. J. (1992). Object-recognition memory is only mildly impaired in rats with lesions of the hippocampus and amygdala. Psychobiology, 20, 18–27.Google Scholar
Murray, E. A. and Mishkin, M. (1986). Visual recognition in monkeys following rhinal cortical ablations combined with either amygdalectomy or hippocampectomy. The Journal of Neuroscience, 6, 1991–2003.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Murray, E. A. and Mishkin, M. (1998). Object recognition and location memory in monkeys with excitotoxic lesions of the amygdala and hippocampus. The Journal of Neuroscience, 18, 6568–82.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Nadel, L. and Moscovitch, M. (1997). Memory consolidation, retrograde amnesia and the hippocampal complex. Current Opinion in Neurobiology, 7, 217–27.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Naya, Y., Yoshida, M., and Miyashita, Y. (2001). Backward spreading of memory-retrieval signal in the primate temporal cortex. Science, 291, 661–4.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Nyberg, L., Habib, R., McIntosh, A. R., and Tulving, E. (2000). Reactivation of encoding-related brain activity during memory retrieval. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences USA, 97, 11120–4.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
O'Keefe, J. A. and Dostrovsky, J. (1971). The hippocampus as a spatial map. Preliminary evidence from single unit activity in the freely-moving rat. Brain Research, 34, 71–175.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
O'Keefe, J. A. and Nadel, L. (1978). The Hippocampus as a Cognitive Map. New York: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Otto, T. and Eichenbaum, H. (1992a). Complementary roles of orbital prefrontal cortex and the perirhinal-entorhinal cortices in an odor-guided delayed non-matching to sample task. Behavioral Neuroscience, 106, 763–76.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Otto, T. and Eichenbaum, H. (1992b). Neuronal activity in the hippocampus during delayed non-match to sample performance in rats: evidence for hippocampal processing in recognition memory. Hippocampus, 2, 323–34.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Ramus, S. J. and Eichenbaum, H. (2000). Neural correlates of olfactory recognition memory in the rat orbitofrontal cortex. The Journal of Neuroscience, 20, 8199–208.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Rempel-Clower, N. L., Zola, S. M., Squire, L. R., and Amaral, D. G. (1996). Three cases of enduring memory impairment following bilateral damage limited to the hippocampal formation. The Journal of Neuroscience, 16, 5233–55.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Rothblat, L. A. and Kromer, L. F. (1991). Object recognition memory in the rat: the role of the hippocampus. Behavioural Brain Research, 42, 25–32.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Sakai, K. and Miyashita, Y. (1991). Neural organization for the long-term memory of paired associates. Nature, 354, 152–5.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Salmon, D. P. and Butters, N. (1995). Neurobiology of skill and habit learning. Current Opinion in Neurobiology, 5, 184–90.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Schacter, D. L. (1985). Multiple forms of memory in humans and animals. In Weinberger, N. M., McGaugh, J. L., and Lynch, G., eds., Memory Systems of the Brain. New York: Guilford Press, pp. 351–80.Google Scholar
Schacter, D. L. and Tulving, E. (1994). What are the memory systems of 1994? In Schacter, D. L. and Tulving, E., eds., Memory Systems. Cambridge: MIT Press, pp. 1–38.Google Scholar
Scoville, W. B. and Milner, B. (1957). Loss of recent memory after bilateral hippocampal lesions. Journal of Neurology, Neurosurgery and Psychiatry, 20, 11–12.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Squire, L. R. (1992). Memory and the hippocampus: a synthesis of findings with rats, monkeys, and humans. Psychological Review, 99, 195–231.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Squire, L. R. and Alvarez, P. (1995). Retrograde amnesia and memory consolidation: a neurobiological perspective. Current Opinion in Neurobiology, 5, 169–77.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Squire, L. R., Knowlton, B., and Musen, G. (1993). The structure and organization of memory. Annual Review of Psychology, 44, 453–95.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Squire, L. R. and Zola, S. M. (1998). Episodic memory, semantic memory and amnesia. Hippocampus, 8, 205–11.3.0.CO;2-I>CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Squire, L. R. and Zola-Morgan, S. M. (1991). The medial temporal lobe memory system. Science, 253, 1380–6.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Suzuki, W. A. (1996). Neuroanatomy of the monkey entorhinal, perirhinal, and parahippocampal cortices: organization of cortical inputs and interconnections with amygdala and striatum. Neuroscience, 8, 3–12.Google Scholar
Suzuki, W. A. and Amaral, D. G. (1994). The perirhinal and parahippocampal cortices of the macaque monkey: cortical afferents. Journal of Comparative Neurology, 350, 497–533.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Suzuki, W. A. and Eichenbaum, H. (2000). The neurophysiology of memory. Annals of the New York Academy of Sciences, 911, 175–91.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Suzuki, W. A., Miller, E. K., and Desimone, R. (1997). Object and place memory in the macaque entorhinal cortex. Journal of Neurophysiology, 78, 1062–81.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Teng, E. and Squire, L. R. (1999). Memory for places learned long ago is intact after hippocampal damage. Nature, 400, 675–7.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Tulving, E. (1993). What is episodic memory? Current Directions in Psychological Science, 2, 67–70.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Tulving, E. and Schacter, D. L. (1990). Priming and human memory systems. Science, 247, 301–6.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Vallar, G. and Shallice, T. (eds.) (1990). Neuropsychological Impairments of Short-Term Memory. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Hoesen, G. W. and Pandya, D. N. (1975a). Some connections of the entorhinal (area 28) and perirhinal (area 35) cortices of the rhesus monkey. I. Temporal lobe afferents. Brain Research, 95, 1–24.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Hoesen, G. W. and Pandya, D. N. (1975b). Some connections of the entorhinal (area 28) and perirhinal (area 35) cortices of the rhesus monkey. III. Efferent connection, Brain Research, 95, 48–67.Google Scholar
Hoesen, G. W., Pandya, D. N., and Butters, N. (1972). Cortical afferents to the entorhinal cortex of the rhesus monkey. Science, 175, 1471–3.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Hoesen, G. W., Pandya, D. N., and Butters, N. (1975). Some connections of the entorhinal (area 28) and perirhinal (area 35) cortices of the rhesus monkey. II. Frontal lobe afferents. Brain Research, 95, 25–38.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Vargha-Khadem, F., Gadin, D. G., Watkins, K. E., et al. (1997). Differential effects of early hippocampal pathology on episodic and semantic memory. Science, 277, 376–80.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Victor, M. and Agamanolis, D. (1990). Amnesia due to lesions confined to the hippocampus: a clinical-pathological study. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 2, 246–57.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Wheeler, M. E., Petersen, S. E., and Buckner, R. L. (2000). Memory's echo: vivid remembering reactivates sensory-specific cortex. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences USA, 97, 11125–9.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Wiebe, S. P. and Stäubli, U. V. (1999). Dynamic filtering of recognition memory codes in the hippocampus. The Journal of Neuroscience, 19, 10562–74.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Wiebe, S. P. and Stäubli, U. V. (2001). Recognition memory correlates of hippocampal theta cells. The Journal of Neuroscience, 21, 3955–67.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Wiener, S. I., Paul, C. A., and Eichenbaum, H. (1989). Spatial and behavioral correlates of hippocampal neuronal activity. The Journal of Neuroscience, 9, 2737–63.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Winocur, G. (1990). Anterograde and retrograde amnesia in rats with dorsal hippocampal or dorsomedial thalamic lesions. Behavioural Brain Research, 38, 145–54.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Wood, E., Dudchenko, P. A., and Eichenbaum, H. (1999). The global record of memory in hippocampal neuronal activity. Nature, 397, 613–16.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Wood, E., Dudchenko, P. A., Robitsek, R. J., and Eichenbaum, H. (2000). Hippocampal neurons encode information about different types of memory episodes occurring in the same location. Neuron, 27, 623–33.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Woodruff-Pak, D. S., Papka, M., and Ivry, R. B. (1996). Cerebellar involvement in eyeblink classical conditioning in humans. Neuropsychology, 10, 443–58.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Xiang, J. Z. and Brown, M. W. (1998). Differential neuronal encoding of novelty, familiarity and recency in regions of the anterior temporal lobe. Neuropharmacology, 37, 657–76.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Young, B. J., Otto, T., Fox, G. D., and Eichenbaum, H. (1997). Memory representation within the parahippocampal region. The Journal of Neuroscience, 17, 5183–95.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Zola, S. M., Squire, L. R., Teng, E., et al. (2000). Impaired recognition memory in monkeys after damage limited to the hippocampal region. The Journal of Neuroscience, 20, 451–63.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Zola-Morgan, S., Squire, L. R., and Amaral, D. G. (1986). Human amnesia and the medial temporal region: enduring memory impairment following a bilateral lesion limited to field CA1 of the hippocampus. The Journal of Neuroscience, 6, 2950–67.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Zola-Morgan, S., Squire, L. R., and Amaral, D. G. (1989a). Lesions of the amygdala that spare adjacent cortical regions do not impair memory or exacerbate the impairment following lesions of the hippocampal formation. The Journal of Neuroscience, 9, 1922–36.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Zola-Morgan, S., Squire, L. R., Amaral, D. G., and Suzuki, W. A. (1989b). Lesions of perirhinal and parahippocampal cortex that spare the amygdala and hippocampal formation produce severe memory impairment. The Journal of Neuroscience, 9, 4355–70.CrossRefGoogle Scholar

Save book to Kindle

To save this book to your Kindle, first ensure coreplatform@cambridge.org is added to your Approved Personal Document E-mail List under your Personal Document Settings on the Manage Your Content and Devices page of your Amazon account. Then enter the ‘name’ part of your Kindle email address below. Find out more about saving to your Kindle.

Note you can select to save to either the @free.kindle.com or @kindle.com variations. ‘@free.kindle.com’ emails are free but can only be saved to your device when it is connected to wi-fi. ‘@kindle.com’ emails can be delivered even when you are not connected to wi-fi, but note that service fees apply.

Find out more about the Kindle Personal Document Service.

  • A brain system for declarative memory
    • By Seth J. Ramus, Department of Psychology and Program in Neuroscience Bowdoin College Brunswick, ME 04011, Howard B. Eichenbaum, Director Cognitive Neurobiology Laboratory; Director Center for Memory and Brain; University Professor and Chairman Department of Psychology Boston University Center for Memory and Brain 2 Cummington Street Boston, MA 02215
  • Edited by James R. Pomerantz, Rice University, Houston
  • Book: Topics in Integrative Neuroscience
  • Online publication: 08 August 2009
  • Chapter DOI: https://doi.org/10.1017/CBO9780511541681.015
Available formats
×

Save book to Dropbox

To save content items to your account, please confirm that you agree to abide by our usage policies. If this is the first time you use this feature, you will be asked to authorise Cambridge Core to connect with your account. Find out more about saving content to Dropbox.

  • A brain system for declarative memory
    • By Seth J. Ramus, Department of Psychology and Program in Neuroscience Bowdoin College Brunswick, ME 04011, Howard B. Eichenbaum, Director Cognitive Neurobiology Laboratory; Director Center for Memory and Brain; University Professor and Chairman Department of Psychology Boston University Center for Memory and Brain 2 Cummington Street Boston, MA 02215
  • Edited by James R. Pomerantz, Rice University, Houston
  • Book: Topics in Integrative Neuroscience
  • Online publication: 08 August 2009
  • Chapter DOI: https://doi.org/10.1017/CBO9780511541681.015
Available formats
×

Save book to Google Drive

To save content items to your account, please confirm that you agree to abide by our usage policies. If this is the first time you use this feature, you will be asked to authorise Cambridge Core to connect with your account. Find out more about saving content to Google Drive.

  • A brain system for declarative memory
    • By Seth J. Ramus, Department of Psychology and Program in Neuroscience Bowdoin College Brunswick, ME 04011, Howard B. Eichenbaum, Director Cognitive Neurobiology Laboratory; Director Center for Memory and Brain; University Professor and Chairman Department of Psychology Boston University Center for Memory and Brain 2 Cummington Street Boston, MA 02215
  • Edited by James R. Pomerantz, Rice University, Houston
  • Book: Topics in Integrative Neuroscience
  • Online publication: 08 August 2009
  • Chapter DOI: https://doi.org/10.1017/CBO9780511541681.015
Available formats
×