Skip to main content Accessibility help
×
Hostname: page-component-8448b6f56d-42gr6 Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-04-25T02:05:03.700Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

16 - The role of parents of children with Down syndrome and other disabilities in early intervention

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  05 July 2011

Gerald Mahoney
Affiliation:
Case Western Reserve University
Frida Perales
Affiliation:
Case Western Reserve University
Jean-Adolphe Rondal
Affiliation:
Université de Liège, Belgium
Juan Perera
Affiliation:
Universitat de les Illes Balears, Palma de Mallorca
Donna Spiker
Affiliation:
Stanford Research Institute International
Get access

Summary

Contemporary early intervention has been closely tied to the proposition that interventions that directly involve parents are more effective at promoting children's learning and development than those that do not (White et al., 1992). This proposition was part of the rationale for the design of the federally mandated early intervention program in the United States, which required that every family in this program have an Individualized Family Service Plan (IFSP). While one of the purposes of the IFSP was to ensure that parents have the resources and supports they need to care for their children, an equally important purpose was to help parents become active participants in their children's intervention. The committee report for the 1986 amendments to the Education of the Handicapped Act (Public Law 99–457) stated,

The committee received overwhelming testimony affirming the family as the primary learning environment for children less than six years of age and pointing out the critical need for parents and professionals to function in a collaborative manner.

(From House Report No. 99–860, as cited in Gilkerson et al., 1987, p. 20.)

The emphasis on involving parents in children's intervention services stems from ecological theories of child development (Bronfenbrenner, 1992, 1999; Dunst et al., 2000, 2006; Sameroff & Fiese, 2000). These theories postulate that early developmental learning is a continuous process that can be affected by each of the experiences children have in their daily environment.

Type
Chapter
Information
Publisher: Cambridge University Press
Print publication year: 2011

Access options

Get access to the full version of this content by using one of the access options below. (Log in options will check for institutional or personal access. Content may require purchase if you do not have access.)

References

Bayley, N. (1969). Bayley Scales of Infant Development. New York: Psychological Corporation.Google Scholar
Beckwith, L. & Cohen, S. E. (1989). Maternal responsiveness with preterm infants and later competency. In M. H. Bornstein (ed.), Maternal responsiveness: characteristics and consequences. New Directions for Child Development, 43, 75–87.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Beckwith, L., Rodning, C., Cohen, S. (1992). Preterm children at early adolescence and continuity and discontinuity in maternal responsiveness from infancy. Child Development, 63(5), 1198–1208.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Bornstein, M. H., Tamis-LeMonda, C. S., Haynes, O. M. (1999). First words in the second year: continuity, stability, and models of concurrent and predictive correspondence in vocabulary and verbal responsiveness across age and context. Infant Behavior and Development, 22(1), 65–85.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Bowlby, J. (1969). Attachment and Loss. New York: Basic Books.Google Scholar
Bradley, R. (1989). HOME measurement of maternal responsiveness. In M. H. Bornstein (ed.), Maternal responsiveness: characteristics and consequences. New Directions for Child Development, 43, 63–74.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Bronfenbrenner, U. (1992). Ecological systems theory. In Vasta, R. (ed.), Six Theories of Child Development, pp. 3–28. Philadelphia: Jessica Kingsley.Google Scholar
Bronfenbrenner, U. (1999). Environments in developmental perspective: theoretical and operational models. In Freidman, S. L. & Wachs, T. D. (eds.), Measuring Environment Across the Lifespan: Emerging Methods and Concepts. Washington, DC: American Psychological Association.Google Scholar
Brooks-Gunn, J., McCarton, C. M., Casey, P. H., et al. (1994). Early intervention in low birthweight, premature infants. Journal of the American Medical Association, 272, 1257–1262.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Chandler, S., Christie, P., Newson, E., Prevezer, W. (2002). Developing a diagnostic and intervention package for 2- to 3- year olds with autism. Autism, 6(1), 47–69.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Dunst, C. J., Hamby, D. W., Trivette, C. M., Raab, M., Bruder, M. B. (2000). Everyday family and community life and children's naturally occurring learning opportunities. Journal of Early Intervention, 23, 151–164.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Dunst, C. J., Trivette, C. M., Hamby, D. W., Bruder, M. B. (2006). Influences of contrasting natural learning environment experiences on child, parent and family well-being. Journal of Developmental and Physical Disabilities, 18(3), 235–250.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Fewell, R. R., Casal, S. G., Glick, M. P., Wheeden, C. A., Spiker, D. (1996). Maternal education and maternal responsiveness as predictors of play competence in low birth weight, premature infants: a preliminary report. Developmental and Behavioral Pediatrics, 17(2), 100–104.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Fewell, R. R. & Deutscher, B. (2004). Contributions of early language and maternal facilitation variables to later language and reading abilities. Journal of Early Intervention, 26(2), 132–145.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Fewell, R. & Wheeden, C. A. (1998). A pilot study of intervention with adolescent mothers and their children: a preliminary examination of child outcomes. Topics in Early Childhood Special Education, 17(4), 18–25.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Gilkerson, L., Hilliard, G. A., Schrag, E., Schonkoff, J. P. (1987). Report accompanying the Education of the Handicapped Act Amendments of 1986 (House Report No. 99–860). Washington, DC: National Center for Clinical Infant Programs.Google Scholar
Girolametto, L. (1988). Improving the social conversational skills of developmentally delayed children: an intervention study. Journal of Hearing and Speech Disorders, 53, 146–157.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Goodman, J. F. (1992). When Slow is Fast Enough: Educating the Delayed Preschool Child. New York: Guilford.Google Scholar
Hemmeter, M. L. & Kaiser, A. P. (1994). Enhanced milieu teaching – effects of parent-implemented language intervention. Journal of Early Intervention, 18(3), 269–289.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Hoff-Ginsberg, E. & Shatz, M. (1982). Linguistic input and the child's acquisition of language. Psychological Bulletin, 92, 3–26.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Kaiser, A. P., Hancock, T. B., Neitfeld, J. P. (2000). The effects of parent-implemented enhanced milieu teaching on the social communication of children who have autism. Early Education and Development, 11(4), 423–446.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Koegel, R. L., Koegel, L. K., Carter, C. M. (1999). Pivotal teaching interactions for children with autism. School Psychology Review, 28(4), 576–594.Google Scholar
Landry, S. H., Smith, K. E., Miller Loncar, C. L., Swank, P. R. (1997). Predicting cognitive-language and social growth curves from early maternal behaviors in children at varying degrees of biological risk. Developmental Psychology, 33(6), 1040–1053.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Landry, S. H., Smith, K. E., Swank, P. R. (2003). The importance of parenting during early childhood for school-age development. Developmental Neuropsychology, 24(2–3), 559–591.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Landry, S. H., Smith, K. E., Swank, P. R. (2006). Responsive parenting: establishing early foundations for social, communication, and independent problem-solving skills. Developmental Psychology, 42(4), 627–642.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Linder, T. W. (1993). Transdisciplinary Play-based Assessment: A Functional Approach to Working with Young Children. Baltimore: Brookes.Google Scholar
Mahoney, G. J. (1988). Maternal communication style with mentally retarded children. American Journal on Mental Retardation, 92, 352–359.Google ScholarPubMed
Mahoney, G. (1992). The Maternal Behavior Rating Scale-Revised. Available from the author, Family Child Learning Center, 143 Northwest Ave. (Bldg A), Tallmadge, Ohio 44278, USA.
Mahoney, G. & Bella, J. (1998). The effects of family-centered early intervention on child and family outcomes. Topics in Early Childhood Special Education, 18(2), 83–94.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Mahoney, G., Boyce, G., Fewell, R., Spiker, D.Wheeden, C. A. (1998). The relationship of parent-child interaction to the effectiveness of early intervention services for at-risk children and children with disabilities. Topics in Early Childhood Special Education, 18(1), 5–17.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Mahoney, G. J., Finger, I., Powell, A. (1985). The relationship between maternal behavioral style to the developmental status of mentally retarded infants. American Journal of Mental Deficiency, 90, 296–302.Google ScholarPubMed
Mahoney, G., Kaiser, A., Girolametto, L., et al. (1999). Parent education in early intervention: a call for a renewed focus. Topics in Early Childhood Special Education, 19(3), 131–140.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Mahoney, G. J., Kim, J. M., Lin, C. S. (2007). The pivotal behavior model of developmental learning. Infants and Young Children, 20(4), 311–325.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Mahoney, G. & MacDonald, J. (2007). Autism and Developmental Delays in Young Children: The Responsive Teaching Curriculum for Parents and Professionals. Austin: PRO-ED.Google Scholar
Mahoney, G. & Perales, F. (2005). A comparison of the impact of relationship-focused intervention on young children with pervasive developmental disorders and other disabilities. Journal of Developmental and Behavioral Pediatrics, 26(2), 77–85.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Mahoney, G., Powell, A., Finger, I. (1986). The maternal behavior rating scale. Topics in Early Childhood Special Education, 6, 44–56.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Mahoney, G., Wheeden, C. A., Perales, F. (2004). Relationship of preschool special education outcomes to instructional practices and parent-child interaction. Research in Developmental Disabilities, 25(6), 493–595.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Masur, E. F. & Turner, M. (2001). Stability and consistency in mothers' and infants' interactive style. Merrill-Palmer Quarterly, 47(1), 100–120.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
McBride, S. L. & Peterson, C. (1997). Home-based intervention with families of children with disabilities: who is doing what? Topics in Early Childhood Special Education, 17(2), 209–233.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
McConachie, H. & Diggle, T. (2007). Parent-implemented early intervention for young children with autism spectrum disorder: a systematic review. Journal of Evaluation of Clinical Practice, 13, 120–129.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
McCollum, J. A. (1984). Social interaction between parents and babies: variation of intervention procedure. Child Care, Health, and Development, 10, 301–315.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
McCollum, J. A. & Hemmeter, M. L. (1997). Parent-child interaction intervention when children have disabilities. In Guralnick, M. J. (ed.), The Effectiveness of Early Intervention, pp. 549–576. Baltimore: Brookes.Google Scholar
Nelson, K. (1973). Structure and strategy in learning to talk. Monograph of the Society for Research in Child Development, 38(1–2 serial), 149.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Peterson, C. A., Luze, G. J.. Eshbaugh, E. M., Jeon, H., Kantz, K. R. (2007). Enhancing parent-child interactions through home visiting: promising practice or unfulfilled promise? Journal of Early Intervention, 29(2), 119–140.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Sameroff, A. J. & Fiese, B. H. (2000). Models of development and developmental risk. In Zeanah, C. H. (ed.), Handbook of Infant Mental Health, pp. 3–13. New York: Guilford.Google Scholar
Seifer, R., Clark, G. N., Sameroff, A. J. (1991). Positive effects of interaction coaching and infants with developmental disabilities and their mothers. American Journal on Mental Retardation, 96, 1–11.Google ScholarPubMed
Shearer, M. S. & Shearer, D. E. (1972). The Portage project: a model of early childhood intervention. In Tjossen, T. J. (ed.), Intervention Strategies for High Risk Infants and Young Children. Baltimore: University Park Press.Google Scholar
Smith, T., Groen, A. D., Wynne, J. W. (2000). Randomized trial of intensive early intervention for children with pervasive developmental disorder. American Journal on Mental Retardation, 105(4), 269–285.2.0.CO;2>CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Sparrow, S., Balla, D., Cicchetti, D. (1984). Vineland Adaptive Behavior Scales. Circle Pines: American Guidance Service.Google Scholar
Stahmer, A. & Gist, K. (2001). The effects of an accelerated parent education program on technique mastery and child outcome. Journal of Positive Behavioral Interventions, 3, 75–82.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Trivette, C. (2003). Influence of caregiver responsiveness on the development of children with or at-risk for developmental disabilities. Bridges, 1(6), 1–13.Google Scholar
Turnbull, A. P., Blue-Banning, M., Turbiville, V. (1999). From parent education to partnership education: a call for a transformed focus – response. Topics in Early Childhood Special Education, 19(3), 164–172.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Ijzendoorn, M. H., Schuengel, C., Bakermans-Kranenburg, M. J. (1999). Disorganized attachment in early childhood: meta-analysis of precursors, concomitants, and sequelae. Development and Psychopathology, 11(2), 225–249.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Velderman, M. K., Bakermans-Kranenburg, M. J., Juffer, F., Ijzendoorn, M. H. (2006). Effects of attachment-based interventions on maternal sensitivity and infant attachment: differential susceptibility of highly reactive infants. Journal of Family Psychology, 20(2), 266–274.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
White, K. R. & Boyce, G. C. (eds.) (1993). Comparative evaluations of early intervention alternatives [Special issue]. Early Education and Development, 4(4).Google Scholar
White, K. R., Taylor, M. J., Moss, V. D. (1992). Does research support claims about the benefits of involving parents in early intervention programs? Review of Educational Research, 62(1), 91–125.CrossRefGoogle Scholar

Save book to Kindle

To save this book to your Kindle, first ensure coreplatform@cambridge.org is added to your Approved Personal Document E-mail List under your Personal Document Settings on the Manage Your Content and Devices page of your Amazon account. Then enter the ‘name’ part of your Kindle email address below. Find out more about saving to your Kindle.

Note you can select to save to either the @free.kindle.com or @kindle.com variations. ‘@free.kindle.com’ emails are free but can only be saved to your device when it is connected to wi-fi. ‘@kindle.com’ emails can be delivered even when you are not connected to wi-fi, but note that service fees apply.

Find out more about the Kindle Personal Document Service.

Available formats
×

Save book to Dropbox

To save content items to your account, please confirm that you agree to abide by our usage policies. If this is the first time you use this feature, you will be asked to authorise Cambridge Core to connect with your account. Find out more about saving content to Dropbox.

Available formats
×

Save book to Google Drive

To save content items to your account, please confirm that you agree to abide by our usage policies. If this is the first time you use this feature, you will be asked to authorise Cambridge Core to connect with your account. Find out more about saving content to Google Drive.

Available formats
×