Skip to main content Accessibility help
×
Hostname: page-component-76fb5796d-5g6vh Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-04-25T14:57:16.394Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

4 - Psycholinguistic investigations of phonological structure in ASL

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  22 September 2009

David P. Corina
Affiliation:
Associate Professor of Psychology University of Washington in Seattle, WA
Ursula C. Hildebrandt
Affiliation:
Psychology doctoral student University of Washington in Seattle, WA
Richard P. Meier
Affiliation:
University of Texas, Austin
Kearsy Cormier
Affiliation:
University of Texas, Austin
David Quinto-Pozos
Affiliation:
University of Texas, Austin
Get access

Summary

Introduction

Linguistic categories (e.g. segment, syllable, etc.) have long enabled cogent descriptions of the systematic patterns apparent in spoken languages. Beginning with the seminal work of William Stokoe (1960; 1965), research on the structure of American Sign Language (ASL) has demonstrated that linguistic categories are useful in capturing extant patterns found in a signed language. For example, recognition of a syllable unit permits accounts of morphophonological processes and places constraints on sign forms (Brentari 1990; Perlmutter 1993; Sandler 1993; Corina 1996). Acknowledgment of Movement and Location segments permits descriptions of infixation processes (Liddell and Johnson 1985; Sandler 1986). Feature hierarchies provide accounts of assimilations that are observed in the language and also help to explain those that do not occur (Corina and Sandler 1993). These investigations of linguistic structure have led to a better understanding of both the similarities and differences between signed and spoken language.

Psycholinguists have long sought to understand whether the linguistic categories that are useful for describing patterns in languages are evident in the perception and production of a language. To the extent that behavioral reflexes of these theoretical constructs can be quantified, they are deemed as having a ‘psychological reality’. Psycholinguistic research has been successful in establishing empirical relationships between a subject's behavior and linguistic categories using reaction time and electrophysiological measures.

This chapter describes efforts to use psycholinguistic paradigms to explore the psychological reality of form-based representations in ASL.

Type
Chapter
Information
Publisher: Cambridge University Press
Print publication year: 2002

Access options

Get access to the full version of this content by using one of the access options below. (Log in options will check for institutional or personal access. Content may require purchase if you do not have access.)

References

Ades, Anthony E. 1977. Vowels, consonants, speech, and nonspeech. Psychological Review 84:524–530CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Battison, Robbin. 1978. Lexical borrowing in American Sign Language. Silver Spring, MD: Linstok Press
Blondel, Marion and Christopher Miller. 1998. The relation between poetry and phonology: Movement and rhythm in nursery rhymes in LSF. Paper presented at the 2nd Intersign Workshop, Leiden, The Netherlands, December
Bradley, Lynette L. and Peter, E. Bryant. 1983. Categorizing sounds and learning to read: A causal connection. Nature 301:419–521CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Brentari, Diane. 1990. Licensing in ASL handshape change. In Sign language research: Theoretical issues, ed. Ceil Lucas. Washington, DC: Gallaudet University Press
Brentari, Diane. 1998. A prosodic model of sign language phonology. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press
Cohn, Jim. 1986. The new deaf poetics: Visible poetry. Sign Language Studies 52:263–277CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Corina, David P. 1991. Towards an understanding of the syllable: Evidence from linguistic, psychological, and connectionist investigations of syllable structure. Doctoral dissertation, University of California, San Diego, CA
Corina, David P. 1992. Biological foundations of phonological feature systems: Evidence from American Sign Language. Paper presented to the Linguistics Departmental Colloquium, University of Chicago, IL
Corina, David P. 1993. To branch or not to branch: Underspecification in ASL handshape contours. In Phonetics and Phonology, Vol. 3: Current issues in ASL Phonology, ed. Geoffrey R. Coulter, 63–95. San Diego, CA: Academic Press
Corina, David P. 1996. ASL syllables and prosodic constraints. Lingua 98:73–102CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Corina, David P. 1998. Aphasia in users of signed languages. In Aphasia in atypical populations, ed. Patrick Coppens, Yvan Lebrum, and Anna Baddo, 261–309. Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates
Corina, David P. 2000. Some observations regarding paraphasia and American Sign Language. In The signs of language revisited: An anthology to honor Ursula Bellugi and Edward Klima, ed. Karen Emmorey and Harlan Lane, 493–507. Mahwah, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates
Corina, David P. and Karen Emmorey. 1993. Lexical priming in American Sign Language. Paper presented at the Linguistic Society of America Conference, Philadelphia, PA
Corina, , David, P., Susan, L.McBurney, , Carl, Dodrill, Kevin, Hinshaw, Jim, Brinkley, and George, Ojemann. 1999. Functional roles of Broca's area and SMG: Evidence from cortical stimulation mapping in a deaf signer. NeuroImage 10:570–581CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Corina, David, and Wendy, Sandler. 1993. On the nature of phonological structure in sign language. Phonology 10:165–207CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Cutler, Anne, Brit, Ooijen, Dennis, Norris, and Rosa, Sánchez-Casas. 1996. Speeded detection of vowels: A cross-linguistic study. Perception and Psychophysics 58:807–822CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Cutler, Anne, and Takashi, Otake. 1998. Perception and suprasegmental structure in a non-native dialect. Journal of Phonetics 27:229–253Google Scholar
Elman, Jeffrey L. and James, L. McClelland. 1988. Cognitive penetration of the mechanisms of perception: Compensation for coarticulation of lexically restored phonemes. Journal of Memory and Language 27:143–165CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Emmorey, Karen. 1987. Morphological structure and parsing in the lexicon. Doctoral dissertation, University of California, Los Angeles
Emmorey, Karen and David, Corina 1990. Lexical recognition in sign language: Effects of phonetic structure and morphology. Perceptual and Motor Skills 71:1227–1252CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Goldinger, Stephen D., Paul, A. Luce, David, B. Pisoni, and Joanne, K. Marcario. 1993. Form-based priming in spoken word recognition: The role of competition and bias. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Memory, Learning, and Cognition 18:1211–1238Google Scholar
Hickock, Gregory, Ursula, Bellugi, and Edward, S. Klima. 1998. The neural organization of language: Evidence from sign language aphasia. Trends in Cognitive Science 2:129–136CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Hildebrandt, Ursula C., and David P. Corina. 2002. Phonological similarity in American Sign Language. Language and Cognitive Processes 17(6)
Klima, Edward and Ursula Bellugi. 1979. The Signs of Language. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press
Levelt, Willem J. M., Herbert, Schriefers, Dirk, Vorberg, Antje, S. Meyer. 1991. The time course of lexical access in speech production: A study of picture naming. Psychological Review 98:122–142CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Liberman, Alvin M. 1996. Speech: A special code. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press
Liberman, Alvin M., Franklin, S. Cooper, Donald, P. Shankweiler, and Michael, Studdert-Kennedy. 1967. Perception of the speech code. Psychological Review 74:431–461CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Liddell, Scott K. and Robert E. Johnson. 1985. American Sign Language: The phonological base. Manuscript, Gallaudet University, Washington DC
Lundberg, Ingvar, Ake, Olofsson, and Stig, Wall. 1980. Reading and spelling skills in the first school years predicted from phonemic awareness skills in kindergarten. Scandinavian Journal of Psychology 21:159–173CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Lupker, Stephen J. and Lucia, Colombo. 1994. Inhibitory effects in form priming: Evaluating a phonological competition explanation. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human perception and performance 20:437–451Google Scholar
Mattingly, Ignatius G. and Michael Studdert-Kennedy. 1991. Modularity and the motor theory of speech perception. Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates
Mehler, Jacques, Jean, Y. Dommergues, Uli, Frauenfelder, and Juan, Segui. 1981. The syllable's role in speech segmentation. Journal of Verbal Learning and Verbal Behavior 20:298–305CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Paulesu, Eraldo, Christopher, D. Frith, and Richard, S. J. Frackowiak. 1993. The neural correlates of the verbal component of working memory. Nature 362:342–345CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Perlmutter, David M. 1993. Sonority and syllable structure in American Sign Language. In Phonetics and Phonology, Vol. 3: Current issues in ASL, ed. Geoffrey R. Coulter, 227–261. San Diego, CA: Academic Press
Petersen, Steven E., Peter, T. Fox, Michael, I. Posner, Mark, A. Mintun, and Marcus, E. Raichle. 1989. Positron-emission tomographic studies of the processing of single words. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience 1:153–170CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Poeppel, David. 1996. A critical review of PET studies of phonological processing. Brain and Language 55:317–351CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Poizner, Howard. 1983. Perception of movement in American Sign Language: Effects of linguistic structure and linguistic experience. Perception and Psychophysics 3:215–231CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Poizner, Howard, Edward S. Klima, and Ursula Bellugi. 1987. What the hands reveal about the brain. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press
Poizner, Howard and Harlan Lane. 1978. Discrimination of location in American Sign Language. In Understanding language through sign language research, ed. Patricia Siple, 271–287. San Diego, CA: Academic Press
Rayman, Janice and Eran, Zaidel. 1991. Rhyming and the right hemisphere. Brain and Language 40:89–105CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Repp, Bruno H. 1984. Closure duration and release burst amplitude cues to stop consonant manner and place of articulation. Language and Speech 27:245–254CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Rose, Heidi. 1992. A semiotic analysis of artistic American Sign Language and performance of poetry. Text and Performance Quarterly 12:146–159CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Sandler, Wendy. 1986. The spreading hand autosegment of ASL. Sign Language Studies 15:1–28CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Sandler, Wendy. 1993. Sonority cycle in American Sign Language. Phonology 10:243–279CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Savin, H. B. and Bever, T. G.. 1970. The nonperceptual reality of the phoneme. Journal of Verbal Learning and Verbal Behavior 9:295–302CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Schriefers, Herbert, Antje, S. Meyer, and Willem, J. Levelt. 1990. Exploring the time course of lexical access in language production: Picture/word interference studies. Journal of Memory and Language 29:86–102CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Sergent, Justine, Eric, Zuck, Michel, Levesque, and Brennan, MacDonald. 1992. Positron-emission tomography study of letter and object processing: Empirical findings and methodological considerations. Cerebral Cortex 40:68–80CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Slowiaczek, Louisa M., and Marybeth, Hamburger. 1992. Prelexical facilitation and lexical interference in auditory word recognition. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition 18:1239–1250Google ScholarPubMed
Slowiaczek, Louisa M., Howard, C. Nusbaum, and David, B. Pisoni. 1987. Phonological priming in auditory word recognition. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition 13:64–75Google ScholarPubMed
Stokoe, William C. 1960. Sign language structure: An outline of the visual communication systems of the American deaf. Studies in Linguistics, Occasional Papers 8. Silver Spring, MD: Linstok Press
Stokoe, William C., Dorothy C. Casterline, and Carl G. Croneberg. 1965. A Dictionary of American Sign Language on linguistic principles. Washington, DC: Gallaudet University Press
Stungis, Jim. 1981. Identification and discrimination of handshape in American Sign Language. Perception and Psychophysics 29:261–276CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Valli, Clayton. 1990. The nature of a line in ASL poetry. In SLR87: Papers from the 4th International Symposium on Sign Language Research. Lappeenranta, Finland July, 1987. (International Studies on Sign Language and Communication of the Deaf; 10.) Eds. William H. Edmondson and Fred Karlsson, 171–182. Hamburg: Signum
Ooijen, Brit, Anne, Cutler, and Dennis, Norris. 1992. Detection of vowels and consonants with minimal acoustic variation. Speech Communication 11:101–108Google Scholar
Zattore, Robert J., Ernst, Meyer, Albert, Gjedde, and Alan, C. Evans. 1996. PET studies of phonetic processing of speech: Review, replication, and reanalysis. Cerebral Cortex 6:21–30CrossRefGoogle Scholar

Save book to Kindle

To save this book to your Kindle, first ensure coreplatform@cambridge.org is added to your Approved Personal Document E-mail List under your Personal Document Settings on the Manage Your Content and Devices page of your Amazon account. Then enter the ‘name’ part of your Kindle email address below. Find out more about saving to your Kindle.

Note you can select to save to either the @free.kindle.com or @kindle.com variations. ‘@free.kindle.com’ emails are free but can only be saved to your device when it is connected to wi-fi. ‘@kindle.com’ emails can be delivered even when you are not connected to wi-fi, but note that service fees apply.

Find out more about the Kindle Personal Document Service.

Available formats
×

Save book to Dropbox

To save content items to your account, please confirm that you agree to abide by our usage policies. If this is the first time you use this feature, you will be asked to authorise Cambridge Core to connect with your account. Find out more about saving content to Dropbox.

Available formats
×

Save book to Google Drive

To save content items to your account, please confirm that you agree to abide by our usage policies. If this is the first time you use this feature, you will be asked to authorise Cambridge Core to connect with your account. Find out more about saving content to Google Drive.

Available formats
×