Skip to main content Accessibility help
×
Hostname: page-component-76fb5796d-22dnz Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-04-25T11:36:33.468Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

7 - Regional Identity and Listener Perception

from Part I - Language Regard: Varied Methods

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  12 January 2018

Betsy E. Evans
Affiliation:
University of Washington
Erica J. Benson
Affiliation:
University of Wisconsin, Eau Claire
James N. Stanford
Affiliation:
Dartmouth College, New Hampshire
Get access

Summary

Image of the first page of this content. For PDF version, please use the ‘Save PDF’ preceeding this image.'
Type
Chapter
Information
Language Regard
Methods, Variation and Change
, pp. 132 - 150
Publisher: Cambridge University Press
Print publication year: 2018

Access options

Get access to the full version of this content by using one of the access options below. (Log in options will check for institutional or personal access. Content may require purchase if you do not have access.)

References

Bates, Douglas, Mächler, Martin, Bolker, Ben, & Walker, Steve. 2015. Fitting linear mixed-effects models using lmer4. Journal of Statistical Software 67(1). 148. doi:http://dx.doi.org/10.18637/jss.v067.i01CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Clopper, Cynthia & Bradlow, Ann R.. 2008. Perception of dialect variation in noise: Intelligibility and classification. Language and Speech 51(3). 175198.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Clopper, Cynthia & Pate, John. 2008. Effects of talker and token variability on perceptual learning of dialect categories. Proceedings of Meetings on Acoustics 5(1). 060002.Google Scholar
Clopper, Cynthia & Tamati, Terrin. 2010. Lexical recognition memory across dialects. Journal of the Acoustical Society of America 127(3). 1956.Google Scholar
Dimov, Svetlin, Katseff, Shira, & Johnson, Keith. 2012. Social and personality variables in compensation for altered auditory feedback. In Solé, Maria-Josep & Recasens, Daniel (eds.). The Initiation of Sound Change: Perception, Production, and Social Factors, 185210. Amsterdam: John Benjamins.Google Scholar
D’Onofrio, Annette. 2015. Persona-based information shapes linguistic perception: Valley Girls and California vowels. Journal of Sociolinguistics 19(2). 241256.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Drager, Katie. 2010. Sociophonetic variation in speech perception. Language and Linguistic Compass 4(7). 473480.Google Scholar
Farrington, Charlie, Kendall, Tyler, & Fridland, Valerie. 2015. The dynamic South: Inherent spectral change in the Southern Vowel Shift. Paper presented at Southeastern Conference on Linguistics (SECOL) 82/Language Variation in the South (LAVIS) IV. Raleigh, North Carolina.Google Scholar
Fridland, Valerie & Kendall, Tyler. 2012. The effect of regional vowel differences on vowel perception and production: Evidence from U.S. vowel shifts. Lingua 122(7). 779793.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Fridland, Valerie 2015. Within-region diversity in the Southern Vowel Shift: Production and perception. In The Scottish Consortium for ICPhS 2015 (ed.), Proceedings of the 18th International Congress of Phonetic Sciences. Glasgow, UK: University of Glasgow. Paper number 500. https://www.internationalphoneticassociation.org/icphs-proceedings/ICPhS2015/Papers/ICPHS0500.pdfGoogle Scholar
Fridland, Valerie, Kendall, Tyler, & Farrington, Charlie. 2014. Durational and spectral differences in American English vowels: Dialect variation within and across regions. Journal of the Acoustical Society of America 136(1). 341349.Google Scholar
Giles, Howard & Ryan, Ellen B. (eds.). 1982. Attitudes towards Language Variation. Social and Applied Contexts. London: Edward Arnold.Google Scholar
Harrington, Jonathan. 2012. The coarticulatory basis of diachronic high back vowel fronting. In Solé, Maria-Josep & Recasens, Daniel (eds.). The Initiation of Sound Change: Perception, Production, and Social Factors, 103122. Amsterdam: John Benjamins.Google Scholar
Hartley, Laura C. 1999. A view from the West: Perceptions of U.S. dialects by Oregon residents. In Preston, Dennis R. (ed.). Handbook of Perceptual Dialectology, Vol. 1, 315332. Amsterdam: John Benjamins.Google Scholar
Hay, Jennifer & Drager, Katie. 2010. Stuffed toys and speech perception. Linguistics 48(4). 865892.Google Scholar
Hay, Jennifer, Nolan, Aaron, & Drager, Katie. 2006. From fush to feesh: Exemplar priming in speech perception. The Linguistic Review, 23. 351–79.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Hay, Jennifer, Warren, Paul, & Drager, Katie. 2006. Factors influencing speech perception in the context of a merger-in-progress. Journal of Phonetics 34(4). 458484.Google Scholar
Kendall, Tyler & Fridland, Valerie. 2012. Variation in the production and perception of mid front vowels in the US Southern Vowel Shift. Journal of Phonetics 40. 289306.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Labov, William. 1991. The three dialects of English. In Eckert, Penelope (ed.). New Ways of Analyzing Variation, 144. New York: Academic Press.Google Scholar
Labov, William. 2001. Principles of Linguistic Change: Social Factors. Boston, MA: Blackwell.Google Scholar
Labov, William, Ash, Sharon, & Boberg, Charles. 2006. The Atlas of North American English: Phonetics, Phonology and Sound Change. A Multimedia Reference Tool. Berlin: Mouton de Gruyter.Google Scholar
Magnuson, James S. & Nusbaum, Howard C.. 2007. Acoustic differences, listener expectations, and the perceptual accommodation of talker variability. Journal of Experimental Psychology 33(2). 391409.Google Scholar
Niedzielski, Nancy A. 1999. The effect of social information on the perception of sociolinguistic variables. Journal of Social Psychology 18. 6285.Google Scholar
Niedzielski, Nancy A. & Preston, Dennis R.. 2000. Folk linguistics. Berlin: Mouton de Gruyter.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Nygaard, Lynne C., Sommers, Mitchell S., & Pisoni, David B.. 1994. Speech perception as a talker-contigent process. Psychological Science 5(1). 4246.Google Scholar
Ohala, John. 1981. The listener as a source of sound change. In Masek, C. S., Laver, John, & Anderson, John (eds.). The Cognitive Representation of Speech, 111122. Amsterdam: North-Holland.Google Scholar
Ohala, John. 1989. Sound change is drawn from a pool of synchronic variation. In Breivik, Leiv Egil & Jahr, Ernst Hakon (eds.). Language Change: Contributions to the Study of Its Causes, 173198. Berlin: Mouton de Gruyer.Google Scholar
Pisoni, David B. 1993. Long-term memory in speech perception: Some new findings on talker variability, speaking rate and perceptual learning. Speech Communication 13. 109125.Google Scholar
Preston, Dennis R. 1989. Perceptual Dialectology. Dordrecht: Foris.Google Scholar
Preston, Dennis R. 1993. Folk dialectology. In Preston, Dennis R. (ed.). American Dialect Research, 333378. Englewood Cliffs, NJ: Prentice Hall.Google Scholar
Preston, Dennis R. 1996. Where the worst English is spoken. In Schneider, Edgar (ed.). Focus on the USA, 297360. Amsterdam: John Benjamins.Google Scholar
Preston, Dennis R. (ed.). 1999. Handbook of Perceptual Dialectology, Vol. 1. Amsterdam: John Benjamins.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Preston, Dennis R. 2013. Language with an attitude. In Chambers, J. K., Trudgill, Peter & Schilling, Natalie (eds.). The Handbook of Language Variation and Change, 2nd edn. 157182. Malden, MA: Wiley-Blackwell.Google Scholar
Remez, Robert E., Philip, E. Rubin, P., Pisoni, David B., & Carrell, Thomas D.. 1981. Speech perception without traditional speech cues. Science 212. 947950.Google Scholar
Shuy, Roger W. & Fasold, Ralph W. (eds.). 1973. Language Attitudes: Current Trends and Prospects. Washington, DC: Georgetown University Press.Google Scholar
Solé, Maria-Josep & Recasens, Daniel (eds.). 2012. The Initiation of Sound Change: Perception, Production, and Social Factors. Amsterdam: John Benjamins.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Sumner, Meghan & Samuel, Arthur. 2009. The effect of experience on the perception and representation of dialect variants. Journal of Memory and Language 60. 487501.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Thomas, Erik. 2002. Sociophonetic applications of speech perception experiments. American Speech 77(2). 115147.Google Scholar

Save book to Kindle

To save this book to your Kindle, first ensure coreplatform@cambridge.org is added to your Approved Personal Document E-mail List under your Personal Document Settings on the Manage Your Content and Devices page of your Amazon account. Then enter the ‘name’ part of your Kindle email address below. Find out more about saving to your Kindle.

Note you can select to save to either the @free.kindle.com or @kindle.com variations. ‘@free.kindle.com’ emails are free but can only be saved to your device when it is connected to wi-fi. ‘@kindle.com’ emails can be delivered even when you are not connected to wi-fi, but note that service fees apply.

Find out more about the Kindle Personal Document Service.

Available formats
×

Save book to Dropbox

To save content items to your account, please confirm that you agree to abide by our usage policies. If this is the first time you use this feature, you will be asked to authorise Cambridge Core to connect with your account. Find out more about saving content to Dropbox.

Available formats
×

Save book to Google Drive

To save content items to your account, please confirm that you agree to abide by our usage policies. If this is the first time you use this feature, you will be asked to authorise Cambridge Core to connect with your account. Find out more about saving content to Google Drive.

Available formats
×