Skip to main content Accessibility help
×
Hostname: page-component-8448b6f56d-c47g7 Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-04-19T08:17:53.381Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

6 - Relationships that support human development

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  22 September 2009

Alan Fogel
Affiliation:
Professor of Psychology University of Utah
Alan Fogel
Affiliation:
University of Utah
Barbara J. King
Affiliation:
College of William and Mary, Virginia
Stuart G. Shanker
Affiliation:
York University, Toronto
Get access

Summary

When Susan was one-and-one-half years old, she had been playing the “lion game” with her mother for the past few months. With a lion puppet on her hand, Susan's mother made the lion roar, tickle, bite, and tease Susan, who seemed delighted to be aroused and frightened. Susan and her mother first concocted this curious blend of happiness and fear, approach and withdrawal, when they discovered tickling games. Susan was only six months old at the time. As her mother loomed in for the tickle, Susan would pull away, turn her body to the side, and at the same time reach out for her mother, look at her, and laugh heartily with her mouth wide open. From early in the first year, simple games create emotional challenges – such as a conflict between approach and withdrawal – that are negotiated in the long-term parent–infant relationship.

Emotions are good for us, a kind of psychological workout. Joy, fear, surprise, and sadness move us internally, shifting our body chemistry and lighting up our brains. Babies are more emotionally alive than most adults: they feel and respond to everything. As people leave infancy behind, however, they learn not to feel as much or as intensely. People who are repeatedly left alone as children, for instance, experience powerful fear and sadness during the separation. Without someone present to whom a child can turn to relieve them, these emotions had to be suppressed because they would be too overwhelming.

Type
Chapter
Information
Human Development in the Twenty-First Century
Visionary Ideas from Systems Scientists
, pp. 57 - 64
Publisher: Cambridge University Press
Print publication year: 2007

Access options

Get access to the full version of this content by using one of the access options below. (Log in options will check for institutional or personal access. Content may require purchase if you do not have access.)

References

Beebe, B., and Lachman, F. (2002). Infancy research and adult treatment: co-constructing interactions. New York: Analytic Press.Google Scholar
Bowlby, J. (1969). Attachment and loss: vol. 1. Attachment. New York: Basic Books.Google Scholar
Buber, M. (1958). I and thou. 2nd edn. (R. G. Smith, trans.). New York: Scribner.Google Scholar
Fogel, A. (1993). Developing through relationships. University of Chicago Press.Google Scholar
Fogel, A. (2001a). Infancy: infant, family and society (4th edn.). Belmont, CA: Wadsworth.Google Scholar
Fogel, A. (2001b). The history (and future) of infancy. In Bremmer, G. and Fogel, A. (eds.), Handbook of infant development. Cambridge: Blackwell Publishers Ltd.Google Scholar
Fogel, A., deKoeyer, I., Bellegamba, F., and Bell, H. (2002). The dialogical self in the first two years of life. Theory and Psychology, 12(2), 191–205.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Greenspan, S., and Weider, S. (1998). The child with special needs: encouraging intellectual and emotional growth. Perseus Publishing.Google Scholar
Panksepp, J. (2001). The long-term psychobiological consequences of infant emotions: prescriptions for the twenty-first century. Infant Mental Health Journal, 22 (1–2), 132–173.3.0.CO;2-9>CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Piaget, J. (1952). The origins of intelligence in children. New York: International Universities Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Schore, A. (2001). Effects of a secure attachment on right brain development, affect regulation, and infant mental health. Infant Mental Health Journal, 22(1–2), 7–66.3.0.CO;2-N>CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Stern, D. N. (1985). The interpersonal world of the infant: a view from psychoanalysis and developmental psychology. New York: Basic Books.Google Scholar
Winnicott, D. (1971). Playing and reality. New York: Basic Books.Google Scholar

Save book to Kindle

To save this book to your Kindle, first ensure coreplatform@cambridge.org is added to your Approved Personal Document E-mail List under your Personal Document Settings on the Manage Your Content and Devices page of your Amazon account. Then enter the ‘name’ part of your Kindle email address below. Find out more about saving to your Kindle.

Note you can select to save to either the @free.kindle.com or @kindle.com variations. ‘@free.kindle.com’ emails are free but can only be saved to your device when it is connected to wi-fi. ‘@kindle.com’ emails can be delivered even when you are not connected to wi-fi, but note that service fees apply.

Find out more about the Kindle Personal Document Service.

Available formats
×

Save book to Dropbox

To save content items to your account, please confirm that you agree to abide by our usage policies. If this is the first time you use this feature, you will be asked to authorise Cambridge Core to connect with your account. Find out more about saving content to Dropbox.

Available formats
×

Save book to Google Drive

To save content items to your account, please confirm that you agree to abide by our usage policies. If this is the first time you use this feature, you will be asked to authorise Cambridge Core to connect with your account. Find out more about saving content to Google Drive.

Available formats
×