Skip to main content Accessibility help
×
Home
  • Cited by 6
  • Print publication year: 2018
  • Online publication date: January 2018

19 - Associative and Controlled Cognition in Divergent Thinking: Theoretical, Experimental, Neuroimaging Evidence, and New Directions

from Part V - Cognitive Control and Executive Functions
Abraham, A. (2014). Creative thinking as orchestrated by semantic processing vs. cognitive control brain networks. Frontiers in Human Neuroscience, 8, 95. http://doi.org/10.3389/fnhum.2014.00095
Abraham, A., Beudt, S., Ott, D. V. M., & Yves von Cramon, D. (2012). Creative cognition and the brain: Dissociations between frontal, parietal–temporal and basal ganglia groups. Brain Research, 1482, 5570. http://doi.org/10.1016/j.brainres.2012.09.007
Abraham, A., Pieritz, K., Thybusch, K., Rutter, B., Kröger, S., Schweckendiek, J., … Hermann, C. (2012). Creativity and the brain: Uncovering the neural signature of conceptual expansion. Neuropsychologia, 50, 19061917. http://doi.org/10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2012.04.015
Acar, S., & Runco, M. A. (2014). Assessing Associative Distance among ideas elicited by tests of divergent thinking. Creativity Research Journal, 26, 229238. http://doi.org/10.1080/10400419.2014.901095
Allen, A. P., & Thomas, K. E. (2011). A dual process account of creative thinking. Creativity Research Journal, 23, 109118. http://doi.org/10.1080/10400419.2011.571183
Andrews-Hanna, J. R., Reidler, J. S., Huang, C., & Buckner, R. L. (2010). Evidence for the default network’s role in spontaneous cognition. Journal of Neurophysiology, 104, 322335. http://doi.org/10.1152/jn.00830.2009
Andrews-Hanna, J. R., Smallwood, J., & Spreng, R. N. (2014). The default network and self-generated thought: Component processes, dynamic control, and clinical relevance: The brain’s default network. Annals of the New York Academy of Sciences, 1316, 2952. http://doi.org/10.1111/nyas.12360
Arden, R., Chavez, R. S., Grazioplene, R., & Jung, R. E. (2010). Neuroimaging creativity: A psychometric view. Behavioural Brain Research, 214, 143156. http://doi.org/10.1016/j.bbr.2010.05.015
Badre, D. (2008). Cognitive control, hierarchy, and the rostro-caudal organization of the frontal lobes. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 12, 193200.
Badre, D., Poldrack, R. A., Paré-Blagoev, E. J., Insler, , , R. Z., & Wagner, A. D. (2005). Dissociable controlled retrieval and generalized selection mechanisms in ventrolateral prefrontal cortex. Neuron, 47, 907918.
Baird, B., Smallwood, J., Mrazek, M. D., Kam, J. W. Y., Franklin, M. S., & Schooler, J. W. (2012). Inspired by distraction: Mind wandering facilitates creative incubation. Psychological Science, 23, 11171122. http://doi.org/10.1177/0956797612446024
Bar, M., Aminoff, E., Mason, M., & Fenske, M. (2007). The units of thought. Hippocampus, 17, 420428.
Baronchelli, A., Ferrer-i-Cancho, R., Pastor-Satorras, R., Chater, N., & Christiansen, M. H. (2013). Networks in cognitive science. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 17, 348360. http://doi.org/10.1016/j.tics.2013.04.010
Beaty, R. E., Benedek, M., Kaufman, B. S., & Silvia, P. J. (2015). Default and executive network coupling supports creative idea production. Scientific Reports, 5, 10964. http://doi.org/10.1038/srep10964
Beaty, R. E., Benedek, M., Silvia, P. J., & Schacter, D. L. (2016). Creative cognition and brain network dynamics. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 20, 8795. http://doi.org/10.1016/j.tics.2015.10.004
Beaty, R. E., Benedek, M., Wilkins, R. W., Jauk, E., Fink, A., Silvia, P. J., … Neubauer, A. C. (2014). Creativity and the default network: A functional connectivity analysis of the creative brain at rest. Neuropsychologia, 64, 9298. http://doi.org/10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2014.09.019
Beaty, R. E., & Silvia, P. J. (2012). Why do ideas get more creative across time? An executive interpretation of the serial order effect in divergent thinking tasks. Psychology of Aesthetics, Creativity, and the Arts, 6, 309319.
Beaty, R. E., & Silvia, P. J. (2014). The role of associative and executive processes in creative cognition. Memory and Cognition, 42, 11861197.
Bechtereva, N. P., Korotkov, A. D., Pakhomov, S. V., Roudas, M. S., Starchenko, M. G., & Medvedev, S. V. (2004). PET study of brain maintenance of verbal creative activity. International Journal of Psychophysiology, 53, 1120.
Beketayev, K., & Runco, M. A. (2016). Scoring divergent thinking tests by computer with a semantics-based algorithm. European Journal of Psychology, 12, 210220.
Bendetowicz, D., Urbanski, M., Aichelburg, C., Levy, R., & Volle, E. (2017). Brain morphometry predicts individual creative potential and the ability to combine remote ideas. Cortex, 86, 216229. doi:10.1016/j.cortex.2016.10.021.
Benedek, M., Franz, F., Heene, M., & Neubauer, A. C. (2012). Differential effects of cognitive inhibition and intelligence on creativity. Personality and Individual Differences, 53, 480485. http://doi.org/10.1016/j.paid.2012.04.014
Benedek, M., Jauk, E., Beaty, R. E., Fink, A., Koschutnig, K., & Neubauer, A. C. (2016). Brain mechanisms associated with internally directed attention and self-generated thought. Scientific Reports, 6, 22959. http://doi.org/10.1038/srep22959
Benedek, M., Jauk, E., Fink, A., Koschutnig, K., Reishofer, G., Ebner, F., & Neubauer, A. C. (2013). To create or to recall? Neural mechanisms underlying the generation of creative new ideas. NeuroImage, 88, 125133. http://doi.org/10.1016/j.neuroimage.2013.11.021
Benedek, M., Könen, T., & Neubauer, A. C. (2012). Associative abilities underlying creativity. Psychology of Aesthetics, Creativity, and the Arts, 6, 273281. http://doi.org/10.1037/a0027059
Benedek, M., Muhlmann, C., Jauk, E., & Neubauer, A. C. (2013). Assessment of divergent thinking by means of the subjective top-scoring method: Effects of the number of top-ideas and time-on-task on reliability and validity. Psychology of Aesthetics, Creativity, and the Arts, 7, 341349. http://doi.org/10.1037/a0033644
Benedek, M., & Neubauer, A. C. (2013). Revisiting Mednick’s model on creativity-related differences in associative hierarchies. Evidence for a common path to uncommon thought. The Journal of Creative Behavior, 47, 273289. http://doi.org/10.1002/jocb.35
Binder, J. R., Desai, R. H., Graves, W. W., & Conant, L. L. (2009). Where ss the semantic system? A critical review and meta-analysis of 120 functional neuroimaging studies. Cerebral Cortex, 19, 27672796. http://doi.org/10.1093/cercor/bhp055
Boccia, M., Piccardi, L., Palermo, L., Nori, R., & Palmiero, M. (2015). Where do bright ideas occur in our brain? Meta-analytic evidence from neuroimaging studies of domain-specific creativity. Frontiers in Psychology, 6, 1195. http://doi.org/10.3389/fpsyg.2015.01195
Borge-Holthoefer, J., & Arenas, A. (2010). Semantic networks: Structure and dynamics. Entropy, 12, 12641302. http://doi.org/10.3390/e12051264
Bossomaier, T., Harré, M., Knittel, , , A., & Snyder, A. (2009). A semantic network approach to the creativity quotien (CQ). Creativity Research Journal, 21, 6471.
Buckner, R. L., Andrews-Hanna, J. R., & Schacter, D. L. (2008). The brain’s default network: Anatomy, function, and relevance to disease. Annals of the New York Academy of Sciences, 1124, 138.
Burgess, P. W., Gonen-Yaacovi, G., & Volle, E. (2011). Functional neuroimaging studies of prospective memory: What have we learnt so far? Neuropsychologia, 49, 22462257. http://doi.org/10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2011.02.014
Carson, S. H., Peterson, J. B., & Higgins, D. M. (2003). Decreased latent inhibition is associated with increased creative achievement in high-functioning individuals. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 85, 499506. http://doi.org/10.1037/0022-3514.85.3.499
Cassotti, M., Agogué, M., Camarda, A., Houdé, O., & Borst, G. (2016). Inhibitory control as a core process of creative problem solving and idea generation from childhood to adulthood: Inhibitory control as a core process of creative problem solving. New Directions for Child and Adolescent Development, 151, 6172. http://doi.org/10.1002/cad.20153
Catani, M., Dell’Acqua, F., & Thiebaut de Schotten, M. (2013). A revised limbic system model for memory, emotion and behaviour. Neuroscience & Biobehavioral Reviews, 37, 17241737. http://doi.org/10.1016/j.neubiorev.2013.07.001
Catani, M., & Thiebaut de Schotten, M. (2012). Atlas of human brain connections. Oxford: Oxford University Press.
Cerruti, C., & Schlaug, G. (2009). Anodal transcranial direct current stimulation of the prefrontal cortex enhances complex verbal associative thought. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 21, 19801987. http://doi.org/10.1162/jocn.2008.21143
Changeux, J.-P., Damasio, A. R., Singer, W., & Christen, Y. (2005). Creation, art and the brain. In Neurobiology of human values (pp. 111). Berlin: Springer-Verlag.
Chavez-Eakle, R. A., Graff-Guerrero, A., García-Reyna, J.-C., Vaugier, , , V., & Cruz-Fuentes, C. (2007). Cerebral blood flow associated with creative performance: A comparative study. NeuroImage, 38, 519528. http://doi.org/10.1016/j.neuroimage.2007.07.059
Chen, A. C., Oathes, D. J., Chang, C., Bradley, T., Zhou, Z.-W., Williams, L. M., … Etkin, A. (2013). Causal interactions between frontoparietal central executive and default-mode networks in humans. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences, 110, 1994419949. http://doi.org/10.1073/pnas.1311772110
Chen, Q., Wei, D., Li, W., Yang, W., Li, H., Lei, Q., … Qiu, J. (2014). Association of creative achievement with cognitive flexibility by a combined voxel-based morphometry and resting-state functional connectivity study. NeuroImage, 102, 474483. http://doi.org/10.1016/j.neuroimage.2014.08.008
Chen, Q.-L., Xu, T., Yang, W.-J., Li, Y.-D., Sun, J.-Z., Wang, K.-C., … Qiu, J. (2015). Individual differences in verbal creative thinking are reflected in the precuneus. Neuropsychologia, 75, 441449. http://doi.org/10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2015.07.001
Chermahini, S. A., & Hommel, B. (2012). More creative through positive mood? Not everyone! Frontiers in Human Neuroscience, 6, 319. http://doi.org/10.3389/fnhum.2012.00319
Christoff, K., Gordon, A. M., Smallwood, J., Smith, R., & Schooler, J. W. (2009). Experience sampling during fMRI reveals default network and executive system contributions to mind wandering. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the USA, 106, 87198724. http://doi.org/10.1073/pnas.0900234106
Christoff, K., Irving, Z. C., Fox, K. C. R., Spreng, R. N., & Andrews-Hanna, J. R. (2016). Mind-wandering as spontaneous thought: A dynamic framework. Nature Reviews Neuroscience, 17(11), 718731.
Chrysikou, E. G., Hamilton, R. H., Coslett, H. B., Datta, A., Bikson, M., & Thomspon-Schill, S. L. (2013). Noninvasive transcranial direct current stimulation over the left prefrontal cortex facilitates cognitive flexibility in tool use. Cognitive Neuroscience, 4, 8189. http://doi.org/10.1080/17588928.2013.768221
Cole, M. W., Reynolds, J. R., Power, J. D., Repovs, G., Anticevic, A., & Braver, T. S. (2013). Multi-task connectivity reveals flexible hubs for adaptive task control. Nature Neuroscience, 16, 13481355. http://doi.org/10.1038/nn.3470
Collins, A. M., & Loftus, E. F. (1975). A spreading-activation theory of semantic processing. Psychological Review, 82, 407428.
Colombo, B., Bartesaghi, N., Simonelli, L., & Antonietti, A. (2015). The combined effects of neurostimulation and priming on creative thinking. A preliminary tDCS study on dorsolateral prefrontal cortex. Frontiers in Human Neuroscience, 9, 403. http://doi.org/10.3389/fnhum.2015.00403
Cousijn, J., Zanolie, K., Munsters, R. J. M., Kleibeuker, S. W., & Crone, E. A. (2014). The relation between resting state connectivity and creativity in adolescents before and after training. PLoS ONE, 9, e105780. http://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0105780
Davelaar, E. J. (2015). Semantic search in the Remote Associates Test. Topics in Cognitive Science, 7, 494512. http://doi.org/10.1111/tops.12146
Davey, J., Thompson, H. E., Hallam, G., Karapanagiotidis, T., Murphy, C., De Caso, I., … Jefferies, E. (2016). Exploring the role of the posterior middle temporal gyrus in semantic cognition: Integration of anterior temporal lobe with goal-oriented cognition. NeuroImage, 137, 165177. http://doi.org/10.1016/j.neuroimage.2016.05.051
De Deyne, S., Kenett, Y. N., Anaki, D., Faust, M., & Navarro, D. (2016). Large-scale network representations of semantics in the mental lexicon. In Jones, M. N. (Ed.), Big data in cognitive science: From methods to insights. Hove: Psychology Press.
De Deyne, S., Navarro, D. J., & Storms, G. (2013). Better explanations of lexical and semantic cognition using networks derived from continued rather than single-word associations. Behavior Research Methods, 45, 480498. http://doi.org/10.3758/s13428-012-0260-7
De Dreu, C. K. W., Nijstad, B. A., Baas, M., Wolsink, I., & Roskes, M. (2012). Working memory benefits creative insight, musical improvisation, and original ideation through maintained task-focused attention. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 38, 656669. http://doi.org/10.1177/0146167211435795
De Neys, W. (2006). Automatic–heuristic and executive–analytic processing during reasoning: Chronometric and dual-task considerations. The Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 59, 10701100. http://doi.org/10.1080/02724980543000123
De Souza, L. C., Guimarães, H. C., Teixeira, , Caramelli, A. L., Levy, P., Dubois, R., , B., & Volle, E. (2014). Frontal lobe neurology and the creative mind. Frontiers in Psychology, 5, 761. http://doi.org/10.3389/fpsyg.2014.00761
De Souza, L. C., Volle, E., Bertoux, M., Czernecki, V., Funkiewiez, A., Allali, G., … Levy, R. (2010). Poor creativity in frontotemporal dementia: A window into the neural bases of the creative mind. Neuropsychologia, 48, 37333742. http://doi.org/10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2010.09.010
De Vico Fallani, F., Richiardi, J., Chavez, M., & Achard, S. (2014). Graph analysis of functional brain networks: Practical issues in translational neuroscience. Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences, 369, 20130521. http://doi.org/10.1098/rstb.2013.0521
Demblon, J., Bahri, M. A., & D’Argembeau, A. (2016). Neural correlates of event clusters in past and future thoughts: How the brain integrates specific episodes with autobiographical knowledge. NeuroImage, 127, 257266. http://doi.org/10.1016/j.neuroimage.2015.11.062
Dietrich, A. (2004). The cognitive neuroscience of creativity. Psychonomic Bulletin & Review, 11, 10111026.
Dietrich, A. (2015). How creativity happens in the brain. Basingstoke: Palgrave Macmillan.
Dietrich, A., & Kanso, R. (2010). A review of EEG, ERP, and neuroimaging studies of creativity and insight. Psychological Bulletin, 136, 822848. http://doi.org/10.1037/a0019749
Dijksterhuis, A., & Meurs, T. (2006). Where creativity resides: The generative power of unconscious thought. Consciousness and Cognition, 15, 135146. http://doi.org/10.1016/j.concog.2005.04.007
Dorfman, L., Martindale, C., Gassimova, V., & Vartanian, O. (2008). Creativity and speed of information processing: A double dissociation involving elementary versus inhibitory cognitive tasks. Personality and Individual Differences, 44, 13821390. http://doi.org/10.1016/j.paid.2007.12.006
Dumas, D., & Dunbar, K. N. (2014). Understanding fluency and originality: A latent variable perspective. Thinking Skills and Creativity, 14, 5667. http://doi.org/10.1016/j.tsc.2014.09.003
Duncan, J. (2010). The multiple-demand (MD) system of the primate brain: Mental programs for intelligent behaviour. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 14, 172179. http://doi.org/10.1016/j.tics.2010.01.004
Durso, F. T., Rea, C. B., & Dayton, T. (1994). Graph-theoretic confirmation of restructuring during insight. Psychological Science, 5, 9497.
Edl, S., Benedek, M., Papousek, I., Weiss, E. M., & Fink, A. (2014). Creativity and the Stroop interference effect. Personality and Individual Differences, 69, 3842. http://doi.org/10.1016/j.paid.2014.05.009
Ellamil, M., Dobson, C., Beeman, M., & Christoff, K. (2011). Evaluative and generative modes of thought during the creative process. NeuroImage, 59, 17831794. http://doi.org/16/j.neuroimage.2011.08.008
Eustache, F., Viard, A., & Desgranges, B. (2016). The MNESIS model: Memory systems and processes, identity and future thinking. Neuropsychologia, 87, 96109. http://doi.org/10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2016.05.006
Evans, J. S. B. T. (2003). In two minds: Dual-process accounts of reasoning. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 7, 454459. http://doi.org/10.1016/j.tics.2003.08.012
Evans, J. S. B. T. (2008). Dual-processing accounts of reasoning, judgment, and social cognition. Annual Review of Psychology, 59, 255278. http://doi.org/10.1146/annurev.psych.59.103006.093629
Evans, J. S. B. T., & Stanovich, K. E. (2013). Dual-process theories of higher cognition: Advancing the debate. Perspectives on Psychological Science, 8, 223241. http://doi.org/10.1177/1745691612460685
Eysenck, H. J. (1993). Creativity and personality: Suggestions for a theory. Psychological Inquiry, 4, 147178. http://doi.org/10.1207/s15327965pli0403_1
Faust, M., & Kenett, Y. N. (2014). Rigidity, chaos and integration: Hemispheric interaction and individual differences in metaphor comprehension. Frontiers in Human Neuroscience, 8, 511. http://doi.org/10.3389/fnhum.2014.00511
Faust, M., & Lavidor, M. (2003). Semantically convergent and semantically divergent priming in the cerebral hemispheres: Lexical decision and semantic judgment. Cognitive Brain Research, 17, 585597. http://doi.org/10.1016/S0926-6410(03)00172-1
Fedorenko, E., Duncan, J., & Kanwisher, N. (2013). Broad domain generality in focal regions of frontal and parietal cortex. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America, 110, 1661616621. http://doi.org/10.1073/pnas.1315235110
Ferrand, L., & Alario, F. X. (1998). Normes d’associations verbales pour 366 noms d’objets concrets. L’Année Psychologique, 98, 659670.
Fink, A., & Benedek, M. (2013). EEG alpha power and creative ideation. Neuroscience & Biobehavioral Reviews, 44, 111123. http://doi.org/10.1016/j.neubiorev.2012.12.002
Fink, A., Koschutnig, K., Hutterer, L., Steiner, E., Benedek, M., Weber, B., … Weiss, E. M. (2013). Gray matter density in relation to different facets of verbal creativity. Brain Structure and Function, 19, 12631269. http://doi.org/10.1007/s00429-013-0564-0
Forster, E. A., & Dunbar, K. N. (2009). Creativity evaluation through latent semantic analysis. In Proceedings of the Annual Conference of the Cognitive Science Society (pp. 602607). Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates.
Forthmann, B., Gerwig, A., Holling, H., Çelik, P., Storme, , , M., & Lubart, T. (2016). The be-creative effect in divergent thinking: The interplay of instruction and object frequency. Intelligence, 57, 2532. http://doi.org/10.1016/j.intell.2016.03.005
Fox, K. C. R., Spreng, R. N., Ellamil, M., Andrews-Hanna, J. R., & Christoff, K. (2015). The wandering brain: Meta-analysis of functional neuroimaging studies of mind-wandering and related spontaneous thought processes. NeuroImage, 111, 611621. http://doi.org/10.1016/j.neuroimage.2015.02.039
Friedman, N. P., & Miyake, A. (2017). Unity and diversity of executive functions: Individual differences as a window on cognitive structure. Cortex, 86, 186204. doi:10.1016/j.cortex.2016.04.023.
Frith, C. D., & Frith, U. (2006). The neural basis of mentalizing. Neuron, 50, 531534.
Gabora, L. (2010). Revenge of the “neurds”: Characterizing creative thought in terms of the structure and dynamics of memory. Creativity Research Journal, 22, 113. http://doi.org/10.1080/10400410903579494
Gabora, L., & Kauffman, S. (2015). Toward an evolutionary-predictive foundation for creativity: Commentary on “Human creativity, evolutionary algorithms, and predictive representations: The mechanics of thought trials” by Arne Dietrich and Hilde Haider, 2014. Psychonomic Bulletin & Review, 23, 632639. http://doi.org/10.3758/s13423-015-0925-1
Gilbert, S. J., Gonen-Yaacovi, G., Benoit, R. G., Volle, E., & Burgess, P. W. (2010). Distinct functional connectivity associated with lateral versus medial rostral prefrontal cortex: A meta-analysis. NeuroImage, 53, 359367. http://doi.org/10.1016/j.neuroimage.2010.07.032
Gilhooly, K. J., Fioratou, E., Anthony, S. H., & Wynn, V. (2007). Divergent thinking: strategies and executive involvement in generating novel uses for familiar objects. British Journal of Psychology, 98, 611625. http://doi.org/10.1348/096317907X173421
Gonen-Yaacovi, G., Souza, L. C. de, Levy, R., Urbanski, M., Josse, G., & Volle, E. (2013). Rostral and caudal prefrontal contribution to creativity: A meta-analysis of functional imaging data. Frontiers in Human Neuroscience, 7, 465. http://doi.org/10.3389/fnhum.2013.00465
Goñi, J., Arrondo, G., Sepulcre, J., Martincorena, I., Mendizábal, N. V. de, Corominas-Murtra, B., … Villoslada, P. (2011). The semantic organization of the animal category: Evidence from semantic verbal fluency and network theory. Cognitive Processing, 12, 183196. http://doi.org/10.1007/s10339-010-0372-x
Goñi, J., Martincorena, , Corominas-Murtra, I., Arrondo, B., Ardanza-Trevijano, G., , S., & Villoslada, P. (2010). Switcher-random-walks: A cognitive-inspired mechanism for network exploration. International Journal of Bifurcation and Chaos, 20, 913922. http://doi.org/10.1142/S0218127410026204
Goulden, N., Khusnulina, A., Davis, N. J., Bracewell, R. M., Bokde, A. L., McNulty, J. P., & Mullins, P. G. (2014). The salience network is responsible for switching between the default mode network and the central executive network: Replication from DCM. NeuroImage, 99, 180190. http://doi.org/10.1016/j.neuroimage.2014.05.052
Green, A. E., Cohen, M. S., Raab, H. A., Yedibalian, C. G., & Gray, J. R. (2015). Frontopolar activity and connectivity support dynamic conscious augmentation of creative state: Neuroimaging Augmented State Creativity. Human Brain Mapping, 36, 923934. http://doi.org/10.1002/hbm.22676
Green, A. E., Kraemer, D. J. M., Fugelsang, J. A., Gray, J. R., & Dunbar, K. N. (2010). Connecting long distance: Semantic distance in analogical reasoning modulates frontopolar cortex activity. Cerebral Cortex, 20, 7076. http://doi.org/10.1093/cercor/bhp081
Green, A. E., Kraemer, D. J. M., Fugelsang, J. A., Gray, J. R., & Dunbar, K. N. (2012). Neural correlates of creativity in analogical reasoning. Journal of Experimental Psychology. Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 38, 264272. http://doi.org/10.1037/a0025764
Greicius, M. D., Supekar, K., Menon, V., & Dougherty, R. F. (2009). Resting-state functional connectivity reflects structural connectivity in the default mode network. Cerebral Cortex, 19, 7278. http://doi.org/10.1093/cercor/bhn059
Gruszka, A., & Necka, E. (2002). Priming and acceptance of close and remote associations by creative and less creative people. Creativity Research Journal, 14, 193205.
Guilford, J. P. (1950). Creativity. American Psychologist, 5, 444454. http://doi.org/10.1037/h0063487
Guilford, J. P. (1967). The nature of human intelligence. New York, NY: McGraw-Hill.
Gupta, N., Jang, Y., Mednick, S. C., & Huber, D. E. (2012). The road not taken: Creative solutions require avoidance of high-frequency responses. Psychological Science, 23, 288294. http://doi.org/10.1177/0956797611429710
Gusnard, D. A., Akbudak, E., Shulman, G. L., & Raichle, M. E. (2001). Medial prefrontal cortex and self-referential mental activity: Relation to a default mode of brain function. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences USA, 98, 42594264.
Hirshorn, E. A., & Thompson-Schill, S. L. (2006). Role of the left inferior frontal gyrus in covert word retrieval: Neural correlates of switching during verbal fluency. Neuropsychologia, 44, 25472557. http://doi.org/10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2006.03.035
Hobeika, L., Diard-Detoeuf, C., Garcin, B., Levy, R., & Volle, E. (2016). General and specialized brain correlates for analogical reasoning: A meta-analysis of functional imaging studies. Human Brain Mapping, 37, 19531969.
Howard-Jones, P. A., Blakemore, S. J., Samuel, E. A., Summers, I. R., & Claxton, G. (2005). Semantic divergence and creative story generation: An fMRI investigation. Brain Research Cognitive Brain Research, 25, 240250.
Jauk, E., Neubauer, A. C., Dunst, B., Fink, A., & Benedek, M. (2015). Gray matter correlates of creative potential: A latent variable voxel-based morphometry study. NeuroImage, 111, 312320. http://doi.org/10.1016/j.neuroimage.2015.02.002
Jones, L. L., & Estes, Z. (2015). Convergent and divergent thinking in verbal analogy. Thinking & Reasoning, 21, 473500. http://doi.org/10.1080/13546783.2015.1036120
Jung, R. E. (2014). Evolution, creativity, intelligence, and madness: “Here Be Dragons.Frontiers in Psychology, 5, 784. doi:10.3389/fpsyg.2014.00784
Jung, R. E., Grazioplene, R., Caprihan, A., Chavez, R. S., & Haier, R. J. (2010). White matter integrity, creativity, and psychopathology: Disentangling constructs with diffusion tensor imaging. PLoS ONE, 5, e9818. http://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0009818
Jung, R. E., & Haier, R. J. (2007). The Parieto-Frontal Integration Theory (P-FIT) of intelligence: Converging neuroimaging evidence. Behavioural and Brain Sciences, 30, 135154.
Jung, R. E., & Haier, R. J. (2013). Creativity and intelligence: Brain networks that link and differentiate the expression of genius. In Vartanian, O., Bristol, A. S., & Kaufman, J. C. (Eds.), The neuroscience of creativity (pp. 233254). Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.
Jung, R. E., Mead, B. S., Carrasco, J., & Flores, R. A. (2013). The structure of creative cognition in the human brain. Frontiers in Human Neuroscience, 7, 330. http://doi.org/10.3389/fnhum.2013.00330
Jung, R. E., Segall, J. M., Bockholt, H. J., Flores, R. A., Smith, S. M., Chavez, R. S., & Haier, R. J. (2010). Neuroanatomy of creativity. Human Brain Mapping, 31, 398409. http://doi.org/10.1002/hbm.20874
Jung, R. E., Wertz, C. J., Meadows, C. A., Ryman, S. G., Vakhtin, A. A., & Flores, R. A. (2015). Quantity yields quality when it comes to creativity: A brain and behavioral test of the equal-odds rule. Frontiers in Psychology, 6, 864. http://doi.org/10.3389/fpsyg.2015.00864
Kahneman, D. (2012). Thinking, fast and slow. London: Penguin.
Kaufman, J. C., & Baer, J. (2012). Beyond new and appropriate: Who decides what is creative? Creativity Research Journal, 24, 8391. http://doi.org/10.1080/10400419.2012.649237
Kenett, Y., & Austerweil, J. (2016). Examining search processes in low and high creative individuals with random walks. In Papafragou, A., Grodner, D., Mirman, D., & Trueswell, J. C. (Eds.), Proceedings of the 38th Annual Meeting of the Cognitive Science Society (pp. 313318). Austin, TX: Cognitive Science Society.
Kenett, Y. N., Anaki, D., & Faust, M. (2014). Investigating the structure of semantic networks in low and high creative persons. Frontiers in Human Neuroscience, 8, 407. http://doi.org/10.3389/fnhum.2014.00407
Kenett, Y. N., Beaty, R. E., Silvia, P. J., Anaki, D., & Faust, M. (2016). Structure and flexibility: Investigating the relation between the structure of the mental lexicon, fluid intelligence, and creative achievement. Psychology of Aesthetics, Creativity, and the Arts. Advance online publication. http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/aca0000056
Kenett, Y. N., Gold, R., & Faust, M. (2016). The hyper-modular associative mind: A computational analysis of associative responses of persons with Asperger Syndrome. Language and Speech, 59, 297317. http://doi.org/10.1177/0023830915589397
Kenett, Y. N., Kenett, D. Y., Ben-Jacob, E., & Faust, M. (2011). Global and local features of semantic networks: Evidence from the Hebrew mental lexicon. PLoS ONE, 6, e23912. http://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0023912
Kim, H. (2010). Dissociating the roles of the default-mode, dorsal, and ventral networks in episodic memory retrieval. NeuroImage, 50, 16481657. http://doi.org/10.1016/j.neuroimage.2010.01.051
Kim, H. (2016). Default network activation during episodic and semantic memory retrieval: A selective meta-analytic comparison. Neuropsychologia, 80, 3546. http://doi.org/10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2015.11.006
Kim, K. (2008). Meta-analyses of the relationship of creative achievement to both IQ and divergent thinking test scores. The Journal of Creative Behavior, 42, 106130.
Kim, K. H. (2006). Can we trust creativity TTSTs? A review of the Torrance Tests of Creative Thinking (TTCT). Creativity Research Journal, 18, 314. http://doi.org/10.1207/s15326934crj1801_2
Koechlin, E., Ody, C., & Kouneiher, F. (2003). The architecture of cognitive control in the human prefrontal cortex. Science, 302, 11811185.
Konishi, S., Hirose, S., Jimura, K., Chikazoe, J., Watanabe, T., Kimura, H. M., & Miyashita, Y. (2010). Medial prefrontal activity during shifting under novel situations. Neuroscience Letters, 484, 182186. http://doi.org/16/j.neulet.2010.08.047
Kozbelt, A., Beghetto, R. A., & Runco, M. A. (2010). Theories of creativity. In Kaufman, J. C., & Sternberg, R. J. (Eds.), The Cambridge handbook of creativity (pp. 2047). Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.
Kühn, S., Ritter, S. M., Müller, B. C., Baaren, , Brass, R. B., , M., & Dijksterhuis, A. (2014). The importance of the default mode network in creativity – A structural MRI study. The Journal of Creative Behavior, 48, 152163.
Kveraga, K., Ghuman, A. S., Kassam, K. S., Aminoff, E. A., Hamalainen, M. S., Chaumon, M., & Bar, M. (2011). Early onset of neural synchronization in the contextual associations network. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences, 108, 33893394. http://doi.org/10.1073/pnas.1013760108
Landauer, T. (1997). A solution to Plato’s problem: The latent semantic analysis theory of acquisition, induction, and representation of knowledge. Psychological Review, 104, 211240.
Lee, C. S., & Therriault, D. J. (2013). The cognitive underpinnings of creative thought: A latent variable analysis exploring the roles of intelligence and working memory in three creative thinking processes. Intelligence, 41, 306320. http://doi.org/10.1016/j.intell.2013.04.008
Lerner, A. J., Ogrocki, P. K., & Thomas, P. J. (2009). Network graph analysis of category fluency testing. Cognitive and Behavioral Neurology, 22, 4552.
Lubart, T., Mouchiroud, C., Tordjman, S., & Zenasni, F. (2015). Psychologie de la créativité (2nd ed.). Paris: Armand Colin.
Maki, W. S., & Buchanan, E. (2008). Latent structure in measures of associative, semantic, and thematic knowledge. Psychonomic Bulletin & Review, 15, 598603.
Martindale, C., & Mines, D. (1975). Creativity and cortical activation during creative, intellectual and eeg feedback tasks. Biological Psychology, 3, 91100. http://doi.org/10.1016/0301-0511(75)90011-3
Marupaka, N., Iyer, L. R., & Minai, A. A. (2012). Connectivity and thought: The influence of semantic network structure in a neurodynamical model of thinking. Neural Networks, 32, 147. http://doi.org/10.1016/j.neunet.2012.02.004
Mason, M. F., Norton, M. I., Van Horn, J. D., Wegner, D. M., Grafton, S. T., & Macrae, C. N. (2007). Wandering minds: The default network and stimulus-independent thought. Science, 315, 393395.
Mayseless, N., Eran, A., & Shamay-Tsoory, S. G. (2014). Generating original ideas: The neural underpinning of originality. NeuroImage, 64, 157168.
Mayseless, N., & Shamay-Tsoory, S. G. (2015). Enhancing verbal creativity: Modulating creativity by altering the balance between right and left inferior frontal gyrus with tDCS. Neuroscience, 291, 167176. http://doi.org/10.1016/j.neuroscience.2015.01.061
McDonald, C. R., Crosson, B., Valenstein, E., & Bowers, D. (2001). Verbal encoding deficits in a patient with a left retrosplenial lesion. Neurocase, 7, 407417. http://doi.org/10.1076/neur.7.5.407.16250
Mednick, M. T., Mednick, S. A., & Jung, C. C. (1964). Continual association as a function of level of creativity and type of verbal stimulus. Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 69, 511515.
Mednick, M. T., Mednick, S. A., & Mednick, E. V. (1964). Incubation of creative performance and specific associative priming. Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 69, 8488.
Mednick, S. A. (1962). The associative basis of the creative process. Psychological Review, 69, 220232.
Mendelsohn, G. A. (1976). Associative and attentional processes in creative performance 1. Journal of Personality, 44(2), 341369. http://doi.org/10.1111/j.1467-6494.1976.tb00127
Mendez, M. F. (2004). Dementia as a window to the neurology of art. Medical Hypotheses, 63, 17.
Merten, T., & Fischer, I. (1999). Creativity, personality and word association responses: Associative behaviour in forty supposedly creative persons. Personality and Individual Differences, 27, 933942. http://doi.org/10.1016/S0191-8869(99)00042-2
Miller, L. A., & Tippett, L. J. (1996). Effects of focal brain lesions on visual problem-solving. Neuropsychologia, 34, 387398. http://doi.org/10.1016/0028-3932(95)00116-6
Mok, L. W. (2014). The interplay between spontaneous and controlled processing in creative cognition. Frontiers in Human Neuroscience, 8, 663. http://doi.org/10.3389/fnhum.2014.00663
Morais, A. S., Olsson, H., & Schooler, L. J. (2013). Mapping the structure of semantic memory. Cognitive Science, 37, 125145. http://doi.org/10.1111/cogs.12013
Nelson, D. L., Dyrdal, G. M., & Goodmon, L. B. (2005). What is preexisting strength? Predicting free association probabilities, similarity ratings, and cued recall probabilities. Psychonomic Bulletin & Review, 12, 711719.
Nijstad, B. A., De Dreu, C. K. W., Rietzschel, E. F., & Baas, M. (2010). The dual pathway to creativity model: Creative ideation as a function of flexibility and persistence. European Review of Social Psychology, 21, 3477.
Noonan, K. A., Jefferies, E., Visser, M., & Lambon Ralph, M. A. (2013). Going beyond inferior prefrontal involvement in semantic control: Evidence for the additional contribution of dorsal angular gyrus and posterior middle temporal cortex. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 25, 18241850.
Northoff, G. (2011). Self and brain: What is self-related processing? Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 15, 186187. http://doi.org/16/j.tics.2011.03.001
Nusbaum, E. C., & Silvia, P. J. (2011). Are intelligence and creativity really so different? Fluid intelligence, executive processes, and strategy use in divergent thinking. Intelligence, 39, 3645.
Perret, E. (1974). The left frontal lobe of man and the suppression of habitual responses in verbal categorical behaviour. Neuropsychologia, 12, 323330.
Plucker, J. A., & Makel, M. C. (2010). Assessment of creativity. In Kaufman, J. C. & Sternberg, R. J. (Eds.), The Cambridge handbook of creativity (pp. 4873). Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.
Plucker, J. A., Qian, M., & Schmalensee, S. L. (2014). Is what you see what you really get? Comparison of scoring techniques in the assessment of real-world divergent thinking. Creativity Research Journal, 26, 135143. http://doi.org/10.1080/10400419.2014.901023
Power, J. D., & Petersen, S. E. (2013). Control-related systems in the human brain. Current Opinion in Neurobiology, 23, 223228.
Prabhakaran, R., Green, A. E., & Gray, J. R. (2013). Thin slices of creativity: Using single-word utterances to assess creative cognition. Behavior Research Methods, 46, 641659. http://doi.org/10.3758/s13428-013-0401-7
Radel, R., Davranche, K., Fournier, M., & Dietrich, A. (2015). The role of (dis)inhibition in creativity: Decreased inhibition improves idea generation. Cognition, 134, 110120. http://doi.org/10.1016/j.cognition.2014.09.001
Raichle, M. E. (2015). The restless brain: How intrinsic activity organizes brain function. Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences, 370, 20140172. http://doi.org/10.1098/rstb.2014.0172
Raichle, M. E., MacLeod, A. M., Snyder, A. Z., Powers, W. J., Gusnard, D. A., & Shulman, G. L. (2001). A default mode of brain function. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences USA, 98, 676682.
Rankin, K. P., Liu, A. A., Howard, S., Slama, H., Hou, C. E., Shuster, K., & Miller, B. L. (2007). A case-controlled study of altered visual art production in Alzheimer’s and FTLD. Cognitive and Behavioral Neurology, 20, 4861. http://doi.org/10.1097/WNN.0b013e31803141dd
Ritter, S. M., & Dijksterhuis, A. (2014). Creativity – The unconscious foundations of the incubation period. Frontiers in Human Neuroscience, 8, 215. http://doi.org/10.3389/fnhum.2014.00215
Ritter, S. M., van Baaren, R. B., & Dijksterhuis, A. (2012). Creativity: The role of unconscious processes in idea generation and idea selection. Thinking Skills and Creativity, 7, 2127. http://doi.org/10.1016/j.tsc.2011.12.002
Rossmann, E., & Fink, A. (2010). Do creative people use shorter associative pathways? Personality and Individual Differences, 49, 891895. http://doi.org/10.1016/j.paid.2010.07.025
Runco, M. A. (2010). Divergent thinking, creativity, and ideation. In Kaufman, J. C. & Sternberg, R. J. (Eds.), The Cambridge handbook of creativity. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.
Runco, M. A., & Acar, S. (2012). Divergent thinking as an indicator of creative potential. Creativity Research Journal, 24, 6675. http://doi.org/10.1080/10400419.2012.652929
Schacter, D. L., Addis, D. R., & Buckner, R. L. (2007). Remembering the past to imagine the future: The prospective brain. Nature Reviews Neuroscience, 8, 657661. http://doi.org/10.1038/nrn2213
Schilling, M. A. (2005). A “small-world” network model of cognitive insight. Creativity Research Journal, 17, 131154.
Schott, G. D. (2012). Pictures as a neurological tool: Lessons from enhanced and emergent artistry in brain disease. Brain, 135, 19471963. http://doi.org/10.1093/brain/awr314
Seeley, W. W., Menon, V., Schatzberg, A. F., Keller, J., Glover, G. H., Kenna, H., … Greicius, M. D. (2007). Dissociable intrinsic connectivity networks for salience processing and executive control. The Journal of Neuroscience, 27, 23492356. http://doi.org/10.1523/JNEUROSCI.5587-06.2007
Shah, C., Erhard, K., Ortheil, H.-J., Kaza, E., Kessler, C., & Lotze, M. (2013). Neural correlates of creative writing: An fMRI study. Human Brain Mapping, 34, 10881101. http://doi.org/10.1002/hbm.21493
Shallice, T., Fletcher, P., Frith, C. D., Grasby, P., Frackowiak, R. S., & Dolan, R. J. (1994). Brain regions associated with acquisition and retrieval of verbal episodic memory. Nature, 368, 633635. http://doi.org/10.1038/368633a0
Shamay-Tsoory, S. G., Adler, N., Aharon-Peretz, J., Perry, D., & Mayseless, N. (2011). The origins of originality: The neural bases of creative thinking and originality. Neuropsychologia, 49, 178185. http://doi.org/10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2010.11.020
Shapira-Lichter, I., Oren, N., Jacob, Y., Gruberger, M., & Hendler, T. (2013). Portraying the unique contribution of the default mode network to internally driven mnemonic processes. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences USA, 110, 49504955. http://doi.org/10.1073/pnas.1209888110
Silvia, P. J. (2008a). Creativity and intelligence revisited: A latent variable analysis of Wallach and Kogan (1965). Creativity Research Journal, 20, 3439. http://doi.org/10.1080/10400410701841807
Silvia, P. J. (2008b). Discernment and creativity: How well can people identify their most creative ideas? Psychology of Aesthetics, Creativity, and the Arts, 2, 139146. http://doi.org/10.1037/1931-3896.2.3.139
Silvia, P. J., Beaty, R. E., & Nusbaum, E. C. (2013). Verbal fluency and creativity: General and specific contributions of broad retrieval ability (Gr) factors to divergent thinking. Intelligence, 41, 328340. http://doi.org/10.1016/j.intell.2013.05.004
Silvia, P. J., Nusbaum, E. C., & Beaty, R. E. (2015). Old or new? Evaluating the old/new scoring method for divergent thinking tasks. The Journal of Creative Behavior. http://doi.org/10.1002/jocb.101
Simonton, D. K. (2015). On praising convergent thinking: Creativity as blind variation and Sslective retention. Creativity Research Journal, 27, 262270. http://doi.org/10.1080/10400419.2015.1063877
Singer, J. L. (1993). Experimental studies of ongoing conscious experience. In Bock, G. R., & Marsh, J. (Eds.), Ciba Foundation Symposium 174: Experimental and Theoretical Studies of consciousness. Chichester: John Wiley and Sons.
Sio, U. N., & Ormerod, T. C. (2009). Does incubation enhance problem solving? A meta-analytic review. Psychological Bulletin, 135, 94120. http://doi.org/10.1037/a0014212
Smallwood, J., & Schooler, J. W. (2015). The science of mind wandering: Empirically navigating the stream of consciousness. Annual Review of Psychology, 66, 487518. http://doi.org/10.1146/annurev-psych-010814-015331
Smith, K. A., Huber, D. E., & Vul, E. (2013). Multiply-constrained semantic search in the Remote Associates Test. Cognition, 128, 6475. http://doi.org/10.1016/j.cognition.2013.03.001
Smith, S. M., Fox, P. T., Miller, K. L., Glahn, D. C., Fox, P. M., Mackay, C. E., … Beckmann, C. F. (2009). Correspondence of the brain’s functional architecture during activation and rest. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences USA, 106, 1304013045. http://doi.org/10.1073/pnas.0905267106
Snyder, A., Mitchel, D., Bossomaier, T., & Pallier, G. (2004). The Creativity Quotient, an objective scoring of ideational fluency. Creativity Research Journal, 16, 415420.
Sowden, P., Pringle, A., & Gabora, L. (2015). The shifting sands of creative thinking: Connections to dual process theory and implications for creativity training. Thinking and Reasoning, 21, 4060.
Spreng, R. N., DuPre, E., Selarka, D., Garcia, J., Gojkovic, S., Mildner, J., … & Turner, G. R. (2014). Goal-congruent default network activity facilitates cognitive control. The Journal of Neuroscience, 34, 1410814114. http://doi.org/10.1523/JNEUROSCI.2815-14.2014
Spreng, R. N., Stevens, W. D., Chamberlain, J. P., Gilmore, A. W., & Schacter, D. L. (2010). Default network activity, coupled with the frontoparietal control network, supports goal-directed cognition. NeuroImage, 53, 303317. http://doi.org/16/j.neuroimage.2010.06.016
Sridharan, D., Levitin, D. J., & Menon, V. (2008). A critical role for the right frontoinsular cortex in switching between central-executive and default-mode networks. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences USA, 105, 1256912574. http://doi.org/10.1073/pnas.0800005105
Steyvers, M., Shiffrin, R. M., & Nelson, D. L. (2004). Word association spaces for predicting semantic similarity effects in episodic memory. In Healy, A. F. (Ed.), Experimental cognitive psychology and its applications (pp. 237249). Washington, DC: American Psychological Association.
Takeuchi, H., Taki, Y., Hashizume, H., Sassa, Y., Nagase, T., Nouchi, R., & Kawashima, R. (2011). Failing to deactivate: The association between brain activity during a working memory task and creativity. NeuroImage, 55, 681687. http://doi.org/10.1016/j.neuroimage.2010.11.052
Takeuchi, H., Taki, Y., Hashizume, H., Sassa, Y., Nagase, T., Nouchi, R., & Kawashima, R. (2012). The association between resting functional connectivity and creativity. Cerebral Cortex, 22, 29212929. http://doi.org/10.1093/cercor/bhr371
Takeuchi, H., Taki, Y., Sassa, Y., Hashizume, H., Sekiguchi, A., Fukushima, A., & Kawashima, R. (2010a). Regional gray matter volume of dopaminergic system associate with creativity: Evidence from voxel-based morphometry. NeuroImage, 51, 578585. http://doi.org/10.1016/j.neuroimage.2010.02.078
Takeuchi, H., Taki, Y., Sassa, Y., Hashizume, H., Sekiguchi, A., Fukushima, A., & Kawashima, R. (2010b). White matter structures associated with creativity: Evidence from diffusion tensor imaging. NeuroImage, 51, 1118. http://doi.org/10.1016/j.neuroimage.2010.02.035
Thompson-Schill, S. L., & Botvinick, M. M. (2006). Resolving conflict: A response to Martin and Cheng (2006). Psychonomic Bulletin and Review, 13, 402408.
Torrance, E. P. (2004). Un résumé historique du développement des tests de pensée créative de Torrance. Revue Européenne de Psychologie Appliquée/European Review of Applied Psychology, 54, 5763. http://doi.org/10.1016/j.erap.2004.01.003
Troyer, A. K. (2000). Normative data for clustering and switching on verbal fluency tasks. Journal of Clinical and Experimental Neuropsychology, 22, 370378. http://doi.org/10.1076/1380–3395(200006)22:3;1-V;FT370
Troyer, A. K., Moscovitch, M., & Winocur, G. (1997). Clustering and switching as two components of verbal fluency: Evidence from younger and older healthy adults. Neuropsychology, 11, 138146.
Troyer, A. K., Moscovitch, M., Winocur, G., Alexander, M. P., & Stuss, D. (1998). Clustering and switching on verbal fluency: The effects of focal frontal- and temporal-lobe lesions. Neuropsychologia, 36, 499504. http://doi.org/10.1016/S0028-3932(97)00152-8
Unsworth, N., Spillers, G. J., & Brewer, G. A. (2011). Variation in verbal fluency: A latent variable analysis of clustering, switching, and overall performance. The Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 64, 447466. http://doi.org/10.1080/17470218.2010.505292
Urbanski, M., Bréchemier, M., Garcin, B., Bendetowicz, D., Foulon, C., Thiebaut de Schotten, M., … Volle, E. (2016). Reasoning by analogy requires the left frontal pole: Lesion-deficit mapping and clinical implications. Brain, 139, 17831799.
Varga, A. L., & Hamburger, K. (2014). Beyond type 1 vs. type 2 processing: The tri-dimensional way. Cognitive Science, 5, 993. http://doi.org/10.3389/fpsyg.2014.00993
Vartanian, O. (2009). Variable attention facilitates creative problem solving. Psychology of Aesthetics, Creativity, and the Arts, 3, 5759.
Vartanian, O., Martindale, C., & Kwiatkowski, J. (2007). Creative potential, attention, and speed of information processing. Personality and Individual Differences, 43, 14701480. http://doi.org/10.1016/j.paid.2007.04.027
Vartanian, O., Martindale, C., & Matthews, J. (2009). Divergent thinking ability is related to faster relatedness judgments. Psychology of Aesthetics, Creativity, and the Arts, 3, 99103. http://doi.org/10.1037/a0013106
Vincent, J. L., Kahn, I., Snyder, A. Z., Raichle, M. E., & Buckner, R. L. (2008). Evidence for a frontoparietal control system revealed by intrinsic functional connectivity. Journal of Neurophysiology, 100, 33283342. http://doi.org/10.1152/jn.90355.2008
Volle, E., Kinkingnéhun, S., Pochon, J.-B., Mondon, K., Thiebaut de Schotten, M., Seassau, M., … Levy, R. (2008). The functional architecture of the left posterior and lateral prefrontal cortex in humans. Cerebral Cortex, 18, 24602469. http://doi.org/10.1093/cercor/bhn010
Volle, E., Levy, R., & Burgess, P. W. (2013). A new era for lesion-behavior mapping of prefrontal functions. In Stuss, D. T. & Knight, R. T. (Eds.), Principles of frontal lobe function (pp. 500523). New York, NY: Oxford University Press.
Ward, T. B. (2007). Creative cognition as a window on creativity. Methods, 42, 2837.
Ward, T. B., & Kolomyts, Y. (2010). Cognition and creativity. In Kaufman, J. C., & Sternberg, R. J. (Eds.), The Cambridge handbook of creativity (pp. 93112). Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.
Wei, D., Yang, J., Li, W., Wang, K., Zhang, Q., & Qiu, J. (2014). Increased resting functional connectivity of the medial prefrontal cortex in creativity by means of cognitive stimulation. Cortex, 51, 92102. http://doi.org/10.1016/j.cortex.2013.09.004
Wei, T., Liang, X., He, Y., Zang, Y., Han, Z., Caramazza, A., & Bi, Y. (2012). Predicting conceptual processing capacity from spontaneous neuronal activity of the left middle temporal gyrus. The Journal of Neuroscience, 32, 481489. http://doi.org/10.1523/JNEUROSCI.1953-11.2012
Wiggins, G., & Bhattacharya, J. (2014). Mind the gap: An attempt to bridge computational and neuroscientific approaches to study creativity. Frontiers in Human Neuroscience, 8, 540.
Wirth, M., Jann, K., Dierks, T., Federspiel, A., Wiest, R., & Horn, H. (2011). Semantic memory involvement in the default mode network: A functional neuroimaging study using independent component analysis. NeuroImage, 54, 30573066. http://doi.org/10.1016/j.neuroimage.2010.10.039
Woolgar, A., Parr, A., Cusack, R., Thompson, R., Nimmo-Smith, I., Torralva, T., … Duncan, J. (2010). Fluid intelligence loss linked to restricted regions of damage within frontal and parietal cortex. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences USA, 107, 1489914902. http://doi.org/10.1073/pnas.1007928107
Wu, X., Yang, W., Tong, D., Sun, J., Chen, Q., Wei, D., … Qiu, J. (2015). A meta-analysis of neuroimaging studies on divergent thinking using activation likelihood estimation. Human Brain Mapping, 36, 27032718. http://doi.org/10.1002/hbm.22801
Yaniv, I., & Meyer, D. E. (1987). Activation and metacognition of inaccessible stored information: Potential bases for incubation effects in problem solving. Journal of Experimental Psychology. Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 13, 187205.
Zabelina, D. L., & Andrews-Hanna, J. R. (2016). Dynamic network interactions supporting internally-oriented cognition. Current Opinion in Neurobiology, 40, 8693.
Zabelina, D. L., & Robinson, M. D. (2010). Child’s play: Facilitating the originality of creative output by a priming manipulation. Psychology of Aesthetics, Creativity, and the Arts, 4, 5765. http://doi.org/10.1037/a0015644
Zeng, L., Proctor, R. W., & Salvendy, G. (2011). Can traditional divergent thinking tests be trusted in measuring and predicting real-world creativity? Creativity Research Journal, 23, 2437.
Zhong, C.-B., Dijksterhuis, A., & Galinsky, A. D. (2008). The merits of unconscious thought in creativity. Psychological Science, 19, 912918. http://doi.org/10.1111/j.1467-9280.2008.02176.x
Zhu, F., Zhang, Q., & Qiu, J. (2013). Relating inter-individual differences in verbal creative thinking to cerebral structures: An optimal voxel-based morphometry study. PLoS ONE, 8, e79272. http://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0079272
Zmigrod, S., Zmigrod, L., & Hommel, B. (2015). Zooming into creativity: Individual differences in attentional global–local biases are linked to creative thinking. Frontiers in Psychology, 6, 1647. http://doi.org/10.3389/fpsyg.2015.01647