Skip to main content Accessibility help
×
Hostname: page-component-7c8c6479df-p566r Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-03-28T16:12:33.449Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

Emotional expression and health

from Psychology, health and illness

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  18 December 2014

Richard B. Slatcher
Affiliation:
The University of Texas at Austin
James W. Pennebaker
Affiliation:
The University of Texas at Austin
Susan Ayers
Affiliation:
University of Sussex
Andrew Baum
Affiliation:
University of Pittsburgh
Chris McManus
Affiliation:
St Mary's Hospital Medical School
Stanton Newman
Affiliation:
University College and Middlesex School of Medicine
Kenneth Wallston
Affiliation:
Vanderbilt University School of Nursing
John Weinman
Affiliation:
United Medical and Dental Schools of Guy's and St Thomas's
Robert West
Affiliation:
St George's Hospital Medical School, University of London
Get access

Summary

A longstanding puzzle within psychology and psychosomatic medicine concerns the relationship between the expression of emotions and physical health. Descartes and Shakespeare suggested that not expressing powerful emotions could be unhealthy. Similarly, William James (1890) and Franz Alexander (1950) forcefully argued that inhibiting the expression of strong emotions over time could result in physical health problems through basic biological stress-related channels (see ‘Psychoneuroimmunology’ and ‘Psychosomatics’). Despite these early hypotheses, there is still no overwhelming evidence to support the idea that the suppression of emotional expression is unhealthy and, conversely, that the open expression of emotions is beneficial.

Emotional expression has been viewed by our culture somewhat ambivalently. On the one hand, emotional expression is often viewed as rather uncivilized, as ‘giving in’ to passion (King & Emmons, 1990, p. 864). On the other hand, it is assumed that emotions usually should be let out, that the healthy end to an emotional response is emotional expression. This view is especially common in the psychological literature. From Breuer and Freud (1895/1966) to the present (e.g. Cole et al., 1996; Pelletier, 1985) the inherent value of naturally expressing one's thoughts and feelings has been emphasized. Emotional expression is thus viewed as a somewhat unseemly but normal part of everyday life.

While emotional expression is a normative behaviour which is neither good nor bad per se, actively holding back emotion through inhibition may have negative health consequences.

Type
Chapter
Information
Publisher: Cambridge University Press
Print publication year: 2007

Access options

Get access to the full version of this content by using one of the access options below. (Log in options will check for institutional or personal access. Content may require purchase if you do not have access.)

References

Alexander, F. (1950). Psychosomatic Medicine. New York: Norton.
Breuer, J. & Freud, S. (1966). Studies on hysteria. New York: Avon. (Original work published 1895).
Campbell, R. S. & Pennebaker, J. W. (2003). The secret life of pronouns: Flexibility in writing style and physical health. Psychological Science, 14, 60–5.Google Scholar
Cole, S. W., Kemeny, M. E., Taylor, S. E. & Visscher, B. R. (1996). Elevated physical health risk among gay men who conceal their homosexual identity. Health Psychology, 15, 243–51.Google Scholar
Durkheim, E. (1951). Suicide. New York: Free Press.
Frattaroli, J. (2006). Experimental disclosure and its moderators : a meta-analysis. Psychological Bulletin, 132, 823–65.Google Scholar
Frisina, P. G., Borod, J. C. & Lepore, S. J. (2004). A meta-analysis of the effects of written disclosure on the health outcomes of clinical populations. The Journal of Nervous and Mental Disease, 192, 629–34.Google Scholar
Greenberg, M. A., Stone, A. A. & Wortman, C. B. (1996). Health and psychological effects of emotional disclosure: a test of the inhibition-confrontation approach. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 71, 588–602.Google Scholar
Gross, J. J. & Levenson, R. W. (1997). Hiding feelings: the acute effects of inhibiting negative and positive emotion. Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 106, 95–103.Google Scholar
James, W. (1890). The principles of psychology. New York: H. Holt and Co.
Kim, Y. (2004). Effects of expressive writing among Mexican and Korean bilinguals on social,physical, and mental well-being. Unpublished doctoral dissertation, University of Texas, Austin.
King, L. A. & Emmons, R. A. (1990). Conflict over emotional expression: psychological and physical correlates. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 58, 864–77.Google Scholar
Klein, K. & Boals, A. (2001). Expressive writing can increase working memory capacity. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 130, 520–33.Google Scholar
Major, B. & Gramzow, R. (1999). Abortion as stigma: cognitive and emotional implications of concealment. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 77, 735–45.Google Scholar
Meads, C. (2003, October). How effective are emotional disclosure interventions? A systematic review with meta-analyses. Paper given at the 3rd International Conference on The (Non)Expression of Emotions in Health and Disease. Tilburg, NL.
Pennebaker, J. W. (1997). Writing about emotional experiences as a therapeutic process. Psychological Science, 8, 162–6.Google Scholar
Pennebaker, J. W. & Beall, S. K. (1986). Confronting a traumatic event: toward an understanding of inhibition and disease. Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 95, 274–81.Google Scholar
Pennebaker, J. W., Kiecolt-Glaser, J. & Glaser, R. (1988). Disclosure of traumas and immune function: health implications for psychotherapy. Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology, 56, 239–45.Google Scholar
Pennebaker, J. W. & Graybeal, A. (2001). Patterns of natural language use: disclosure, personality, and social integration. Current Directions in Psychological Science, 10, 90–3.Google Scholar
Pelletier, K. R. (1985). Mind as healer, mind as slayer. New York: Delacorte Press.
Petrie, K. J., Fontanilla, I., Thomas, M. G., Booth, R. J. & Pennebaker, J. W. (2004). Effect of written emotional expression on immune function in patients with HIV infection: a randomized trial. Psychosomatic Medicine, 66, 272–5.Google Scholar
Rimé, B. (1995). Mental rumination, social sharing, and the recovery from emotional exposure. In Pennebaker, J. W. (Ed.). Emotion, disclosure, and health (pp. 271–91). Washington, DC: American Psychological Association.
Solano, L., Donati, V., Pecci, F., Persichetti, S. & Colaci, A. (2003). Post-operative course after papilloma resection: effects of written disclosure of the experience in subjects with different alexithymia levels. Psychosomatic Medicine, 65, 477–84.Google Scholar
Smyth, J. M. (1998). Written emotional expression: effect sizes, outcome types, and moderating variables. Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology, 66, 174–84.Google Scholar
Smyth, J. M., Stone, A. A., Hurewitz, A. & Kaell, A. (1999). Effects of writing about stressful experiences on symptom reduction in patients with asthma or rheumatoid arthritis: a randomized trial. Journal of the American Medical Association, 14, 1304–9.Google Scholar
Taylor, L., Wallander, J., Anderson, D., Beasley, P. & Brown, R. (2003). Improving chronic disease utilization, health status, and adjustment in adolescents and young adults with cystic fibrosis. Journal of Clinical Psychology in Medical Settings, 10, 9–16.Google Scholar
Traue, H. C. & Deighton, R. (1999). Inhibition, disclosure, and health: don't simply slash the Gordian knot. Advances in Mind–Body Medicine, 15, 184–93.Google Scholar
Wegner, D. M. (1994). Ironic processes of mental control. Psychological Review, 101, 34–52.Google Scholar

Save book to Kindle

To save this book to your Kindle, first ensure coreplatform@cambridge.org is added to your Approved Personal Document E-mail List under your Personal Document Settings on the Manage Your Content and Devices page of your Amazon account. Then enter the ‘name’ part of your Kindle email address below. Find out more about saving to your Kindle.

Note you can select to save to either the @free.kindle.com or @kindle.com variations. ‘@free.kindle.com’ emails are free but can only be saved to your device when it is connected to wi-fi. ‘@kindle.com’ emails can be delivered even when you are not connected to wi-fi, but note that service fees apply.

Find out more about the Kindle Personal Document Service.

Available formats
×

Save book to Dropbox

To save content items to your account, please confirm that you agree to abide by our usage policies. If this is the first time you use this feature, you will be asked to authorise Cambridge Core to connect with your account. Find out more about saving content to Dropbox.

Available formats
×

Save book to Google Drive

To save content items to your account, please confirm that you agree to abide by our usage policies. If this is the first time you use this feature, you will be asked to authorise Cambridge Core to connect with your account. Find out more about saving content to Google Drive.

Available formats
×