Skip to main content Accessibility help
×
Hostname: page-component-8448b6f56d-cfpbc Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-04-19T17:13:02.666Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

16 - Thinking Outside the Head

Cognitive Ecologies and Evolutionary Psychology

from Part IV - Group Living

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  02 March 2020

Lance Workman
Affiliation:
University of South Wales
Will Reader
Affiliation:
Sheffield Hallam University
Jerome H. Barkow
Affiliation:
Dalhousie University, Nova Scotia
Get access

Summary

Humans have colonized virtually every habitat on the planet and have transformed the Earth’s environments to such an extent that there is a strong push to name the current geological epoch the “Anthropocene” (Smith & Zeder, 2013; Steffen et al., 2011). Understanding how humans have achieved such a feat – and how to deal with the unforeseen consequences of these actions – requires a massive, multidisciplinary effort, in which evolutionary-informed analyses of human behavior must surely play a part. Indeed, some researchers claim that an evolutionarily-informed and integrated social science is essential in order to make any headway on this issue (e.g., Mesoudi, 2011). In particular, great claims have been made about the revolutionary promise of evolutionary psychology (EP): how it will transform and unite the social sciences, providing us with a better, more complete explanation of the human mind (e.g., Tooby & Cosmides, 1992, 2005).

Type
Chapter
Information
Publisher: Cambridge University Press
Print publication year: 2020

Access options

Get access to the full version of this content by using one of the access options below. (Log in options will check for institutional or personal access. Content may require purchase if you do not have access.)

References

Adams, F., & Aizawa, K. (2001). The bounds of cognition. Philosophical Psychology, 14(1), 4364.Google Scholar
Adams, F., & Aizawa, K. (2011). The Bounds of Cognition. New York: John Wiley & Sons.Google Scholar
Barkow, J. H. (2006). Introduction: Sometimes the bus does wait. In Barkow, J. H., ed., Missing the Revolution: Darwinism for Social Scientists. Oxford: Oxford University Press, pp. 360.Google Scholar
Barrett, H. C. (2015). The Shape of Thought: How Mental Adaptations Evolve. New York: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Barrett, H. C., & Kurzban, R. (2006). Modularity in cognition: Framing the debate. Psychological Review, 113, 628647.Google Scholar
Barrett, L. (2011). Beyond the Brain: How Body and Environment Shape Animal and Human Minds. Princeton, NJ: Princeton University Press.Google Scholar
Barrett, L. (2012). Why behaviorism isn’t satanism. In Shackelford, T. & Vonk, J., eds., The Oxford Handbook of Comparative Evolutionary Psychology. New York: Oxford University Press, pp. 1738.Google Scholar
Barrett, L., Pollet, T. V., & Stulp, G. (2014). From computers to cultivation: Reconceptualizing evolutionary psychology. Frontiers in Psychology, 5, 867.Google Scholar
Bateson, G. (1960). Minimal requirements for a theory of schizophrenia. AMA Archives of General Psychiatry, 2(5), 477491.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Bennett, M. R., & Hacker, P. M. S. (2003). Philosophical Foundations of Neuroscience. Oxford: Blackwell Publishing.Google Scholar
Brooks, R. A. (1999). Cambrian Intelligence: The Early History of the New AI. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Burke, D. (2014). Why isn’t everyone an evolutionary psychologist? Frontiers in Psychology, 5, 910.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Chemero, A. (2009). Radical Embodied Cognitive Science. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.Google Scholar
Clark, A. (1997). Being There: Putting Brain, Body, and World Together Again. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.Google Scholar
Clark, A. (2001). Reasons, robots and the extended mind. Mind & Language, 16(2), 121145.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Clark, A. (2003). Natural-Born Cyborgs: Minds, Technologies, and the Future of Human Intelligence. Oxford: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Clark, A. (2007). Curing cognitive hiccups: A defense of the extended mind. Journal of Philosophy, 104(4), 163192.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Clark, A. (2008). Supersizing the Mind: Embodiment, Action, and Cognitive Extension. New York: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Clark, A. (2014). Mindware: An Introduction to the Philosophy of Cognitive Science. Oxford: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Clark, A., & Chalmers, D. (1998). The extended mind. Analysis, 58(1), 719.Google Scholar
Cosmides, L. (2016). Evolutionary psychology and the generation of culture. Invited presentation. Big Questions in Evolutionary Science and what they mean for Social-Personality Psychology. Presented at 17th Annual Convention of the Society for Personality and Social Psychology, San Diego, January 28–30.Google Scholar
Cosmides, L., & Tooby, J. (1997). Evolutionary Psychology: A Primer. www.cep.ucsb.edu/primer.html.Google Scholar
Cottingham, J., Stoothoff, R., & Murdoch, D., eds. (1985). The Philosophical Writings of Descartes. Vol. 1. Cambridge, UK: Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
Daston, L., & Galison, P. (2007). Objectivity. New York: Zone Books.Google Scholar
Dewey, J. (1916). Essays in Experimental Logic. Chicago, IL: University of Chicago Press.Google Scholar
Dewey, J. (1981). The Later Works: 1925–1953. Chicago, IL: Southern Illinois University Press.Google Scholar
Fisher, S. E. (2006). Tangled webs: Tracing the connections between genes and cognition. Cognition, 101(2), 270297.Google Scholar
Fodor, J. A. (2008). LOT 2: The Language of Thought Revisited. Oxford: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Geertz, C. (1973). The Interpretation of Cultures: Selected Essays. New York: Basic books.Google Scholar
Gintis, H. (2016). Individuality and Entanglement: The Moral and Material Bases of Social Life. Princeton, NJ: Princeton University Press.Google Scholar
Henrich, J. (2016). The Secret of Our Success: How Culture Is Driving Human Evolution, Domesticating Our Species, and Making Us Smarter. Princeton, NJ: Princeton University Press.Google Scholar
Hurley, S. (2010). Varieties of externalism. In Menary, R. A., ed., The Extended Mind. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press, pp. 101154.Google Scholar
Hutchins, E. (1995). Cognition in the Wild. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.Google Scholar
Hutchins, E. (2008). The role of cultural practices in the emergence of modern human intelligence. Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences, 363(1499), 20112019.Google Scholar
Hutchins, E. (2010). Cognitive ecology. Topics in Cognitive Science, 2(4), 705715.Google Scholar
Hutchins, E. (2011). Enculturating the supersized mind. Philosophical Studies, 152(3), 437446.Google Scholar
Hutto, D. D. (2013). Psychology unified: From folk psychology to radical enactivism. Review of General Psychology, 17(2), 174184.Google Scholar
Hutto, D. D., & Myin, E. (2013). Radicalizing Enactivism: Basic Minds without Content. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.Google Scholar
Jonason, P. K., & Dane, L. K. (2014). How beliefs get in the way of the acceptance of evolutionary psychology. Frontiers in Psychology, 5, 1212.Google Scholar
Jonason, P. K., Jones, A., & Lyons, M. (2013). Creatures of the night: Chronotypes and the Dark Triad traits. Personality and Individual Differences, 55(5), 538541.Google Scholar
Landy, D., & Goldstone, R. L. (2007a). How abstract is symbolic thought? Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 33(4), 720733.Google ScholarPubMed
Landy, D., & Goldstone, R. L. (2007b). The alignment of ordering and space in arithmetic computation. Proceedings of the Cognitive Science Society, 29, 437442.Google Scholar
Landy, D., & Goldstone, R. L. (2010). Proximity and precedence in arithmetic. Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 63, 19531968.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Latour, B. (2005). Reassembling the Social: An Introduction to Actor-Network-Theory. Oxford: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Lock, M. (2013). The Alzheimer Conundrum: Entanglements of Dementia and Aging. Princeton, NJ: Princeton University Press.Google Scholar
Marks, J. M. (2009). Why I Am Not a Scientist: Anthropology and Modern Knowledge. Oakland, CA: University of California Press.Google Scholar
Menary, R. A. (2007). Cognitive Integration: Mind and Cognition Unbounded. London: Palgrave Macmillan.Google Scholar
Menary, R. A. (2010). Cognitive integration and the extended mind. In Menary, R. A., ed., The Extended Mind. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press, pp. 227243.Google Scholar
Mesoudi, A. (2011). Cultural Evolution: How Darwinian Theory Can Explain Human Culture and Synthesize the Social Sciences. Chicago, IL: University of Chicago Press.Google Scholar
Nicholson, J. K., Holmes, E., Kinross, J., et al. (2012). Host–gut microbiota metabolic interactions. Science, 336(6086), 12621267.Google Scholar
Park, J. H. (2007). Persistent misunderstandings of inclusive fitness and kin selection: Their ubiquitous appearance in social psychology textbooks. Evolutionary Psychology, 5(4), 860873.Google Scholar
Payne, S. J., Howes, A., & Reader, W. R. (2001). Adaptively distributing cognition: A decision-making perspective on human–computer interaction. Behaviour & Information Technology, 20(5), 339346.Google Scholar
Pickering, A. (2010). The Mangle of Practice: Time, Agency, and Science. Chicago, IL: University of Chicago Press.Google Scholar
Richerson, P. J., & Boyd, R. (2005). Not by Genes Alone: How Culture Transformed Human Evolution. Chicago, IL: University of Chicago Press.Google Scholar
Ross, A., ed. (1996). Science Wars. Durham, NC: Duke University Press.Google Scholar
Rowlands, M. (1999). The Body in Mind: Understanding Cognitive Processes. Cambridge, UK: Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
Rowlands, M. (2010). The New Science of the Mind: From Extended Mind to Embodied Phenomenology. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Rupert, R. D. (2009). Cognitive Systems and the Extended Mind. Oxford: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Ryle, G. (1949). The Concept of Mind. London: Hutchinson.Google Scholar
Shapin, S. (2010). Never Pure: Historical Studies of Science as if It Was Produced by People with Bodies, Situated in Time, Space, Culture, and Society, and Struggling for Credibility and Authority. Baltimore, MD: Johns Hopkins University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Shapin, S., & Schaffer, S. (1985). Leviathan and the Air-Pump: Hobbes, Boyle, and the Experimental Life. Princeton, NJ: Princeton University Press.Google Scholar
Shapiro, L. (2010). James Bond and the barking dog: Evolution and extended cognition. Philosophy of Science, 77(3), 400418.Google Scholar
Smith, B. D., & Zeder, M. A. (2013). The onset of the Anthropocene. Anthropocene, 4, 813.Google Scholar
Sperber, D. (1994). The modularity of thought and the epidemiology of representations. In Hirschfeld, L. A. & Gelman, S. A., eds., Mapping the Mind: Domain Specificity in Cognition and Culture, Cambridge, UK: Cambridge University Press, pp. 3967.Google Scholar
Sperber, D. (1996). Explaining Culture. Oxford: Blackwell Publishers.Google Scholar
Sprevak, M. (2010). Inference to the hypothesis of extended cognition. Studies in History and Philosophy of Science Part A, 41(4), 353362.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Steffen, W., Grinevald, J., Crutzen, P., & McNeill, J. (2011). The Anthropocene: Conceptual and historical perspectives. Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society of London A: Mathematical, Physical and Engineering Sciences, 369(1938), 842867.Google Scholar
Sterelny, K. (2010). Minds: Extended or scaffolded? Phenomenology and the Cognitive Sciences, 9(4), 465481.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Sussman, R. W. (2014). The Myth of Race: The Troubling Persistence of an Unscientific Idea. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press.Google Scholar
Sutton, J. (2006). Distributed cognition: Domains and dimensions. Pragmatics & Cognition, 14(2), 235247.Google Scholar
Sutton, J. (2010). Exograms and interdisciplinarity: History, the extended mind, and the civilizing process. In Menary, R. A., ed., The Extended Mind. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press, pp. 189225.Google Scholar
Sutton, J. (2015). Remembering as public practice: Wittgenstein, memory, and distributed cognitive ecologies. In Moyal-Sharrock, D., Munz, V., & Coliva, A., eds., Mind, Language, and Action: Proceedings of the 36th Wittgenstein Symposium. Berlin: De Gruyter, pp. 409443.Google Scholar
Thompson, E. (2007). Mind in Life: Biology, Phenomenology, and the Sciences of Mind. Harvard, MA: Harvard University Press.Google Scholar
Tooby, J., & Cosmides, L. (1992). The psychological foundations of culture. In Barkow, J., Cosmides, L., & Tooby, J., eds., The Adapted Mind: Evolutionary Psychology and the Generation of Culture. Oxford: Oxford University Press, pp. 19136.Google Scholar
Tooby, J., & Cosmides, L. (2005). Evolutionary psychology: Conceptual foundations. In Buss, D., ed., The Handbook of Evolutionary Psychology. New York: John Wiley & Sons, pp. 567.Google Scholar
Tribble, E. (2011). Cognition in the Globe: Attention and Memory in Shakespeare’s Theatre. New York: Springer.Google Scholar
Vaesen, K. (2014). Dewey on extended cognition and epistemology. Philosophical Issues, 24(1), 426438.Google Scholar
van Dijk, L. (2016). A horizontal attitude: Gibsonian psychology and an ontology of doing. Unpublished PhD thesis, University of Antwerp.Google Scholar
Varela, F. J., Thompson, E., & Rosch, E. (1992). The Embodied Mind: Cognitive Science and Human Experience. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.Google Scholar
West, S. A., El Mouden, C., & Gardner, A. (2011). Sixteen common misconceptions about the evolution of cooperation in humans. Evolution and Human Behavior, 32(4), 231262.Google Scholar
Wheeler, M., & Clark, A. (2008). Culture, embodiment and genes: Unravelling the triple helix. Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society of London B: Biological Sciences, 363(1509), 35633575.Google Scholar
Wrangham, R. (2009). Catching Fire: How Cooking Made Us Human. New York: Basic Books.Google Scholar

Save book to Kindle

To save this book to your Kindle, first ensure coreplatform@cambridge.org is added to your Approved Personal Document E-mail List under your Personal Document Settings on the Manage Your Content and Devices page of your Amazon account. Then enter the ‘name’ part of your Kindle email address below. Find out more about saving to your Kindle.

Note you can select to save to either the @free.kindle.com or @kindle.com variations. ‘@free.kindle.com’ emails are free but can only be saved to your device when it is connected to wi-fi. ‘@kindle.com’ emails can be delivered even when you are not connected to wi-fi, but note that service fees apply.

Find out more about the Kindle Personal Document Service.

Available formats
×

Save book to Dropbox

To save content items to your account, please confirm that you agree to abide by our usage policies. If this is the first time you use this feature, you will be asked to authorise Cambridge Core to connect with your account. Find out more about saving content to Dropbox.

Available formats
×

Save book to Google Drive

To save content items to your account, please confirm that you agree to abide by our usage policies. If this is the first time you use this feature, you will be asked to authorise Cambridge Core to connect with your account. Find out more about saving content to Google Drive.

Available formats
×