Skip to main content Accessibility help
×
Hostname: page-component-7c8c6479df-8mjnm Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-03-28T15:37:15.570Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

Part IV - Group Living

The Evolution of Social and Moral Behavior

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  02 March 2020

Lance Workman
Affiliation:
University of South Wales
Will Reader
Affiliation:
Sheffield Hallam University
Jerome H. Barkow
Affiliation:
Dalhousie University, Nova Scotia
Get access

Summary

Image of the first page of this content. For PDF version, please use the ‘Save PDF’ preceeding this image.'
Type
Chapter
Information
Publisher: Cambridge University Press
Print publication year: 2020

Access options

Get access to the full version of this content by using one of the access options below. (Log in options will check for institutional or personal access. Content may require purchase if you do not have access.)

References

Reference

Darwin, C. (1871). The Descent of Man, and Selection in Relation to Sex. London: John Murray.Google Scholar
Hamilton, W. D. (1964). The genetical evolution of social behaviour. II. Journal of Theoretical Biology, 7(1), 1752.Google Scholar
Read, L. (1958). I, Pencil. Atlanta, GA: Foundation for Economic Education.Google Scholar
Trivers, R. L. (1971). The evolution of reciprocal altruism. Quarterly Review of Biology, 46(1), 3557.CrossRefGoogle Scholar

References

Abbot, P., Abe, J., Alcock, J., et al. (2011). Inclusive fitness theory and eusociality. Nature, 471(7339), E1E4.Google Scholar
Ackerman, J. M., Kenrick, D. T., & Schaller, M. (2007). Is friendship akin to kinship? Evolution and Human Behavior, 28, 365374.Google Scholar
Allen, A. P., Dinan, T. G., Clarke, G., & Cryan, J. F. (2017). A psychology of the human brain–gut–microbiome axis. Social and Personality Psychology Compass, 11(4). e12309.Google Scholar
Andersson, M. (1984). The evolution of eusociality. Annual Review of Ecology and Systematics, 15, 165189.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Andreoni, J., & Petrie, R. (2004). Public goods experiments without confidentiality: A glimpse into fund-raising. Journal of Public Economics, 88(7), 16051623.Google Scholar
Ariely, D., Bracha, A., & Meier, S. (2009). Doing good or doing well? Image motivation and monetary incentives in behaving prosocially. American Economic Review, 99(1), 544555.Google Scholar
Arnocky, S., Piché, T., Albert, G., Ouellette, D., & Barclay, P. (2016). Altruism predicts mating success in humans. British Journal of Psychology, 108, 416435.Google Scholar
Arnold, W. (1990a). The evolution of marmot sociality: I. Why disperse late? Behavioral Ecology and Sociobiology, 27(4), 229237.Google Scholar
Arnold, W. (1990b). The evolution of marmot sociality: II. Costs and benefits of joint hibernation. Behavioral Ecology and Sociobiology, 27(4), 239246.Google Scholar
Ashworth, T. (2000). Trench Warfare, 1914–1918: The Live and Let Live System, 2nd ed. Basingstoke: Pan Macmillan.Google Scholar
Axelrod, R. (1984). The Evolution of Co-operation. New York: Basic Books.Google Scholar
Axelrod, R., & Hamilton, W. D. (1981). The evolution of cooperation. Science, 211(4489), 13901396.Google Scholar
Barclay, P. (2010). Altruism as a courtship display: Some effects of third-party generosity on audience perceptions. British Journal of Psychology, 101(1), 123135.Google Scholar
Barclay, P. (2011). Competitive helping increases with the size of biological markets and invades defection. Journal of Theoretical Biology, 281, 4755.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Barclay, P. (2013). Strategies for cooperation in biological markets, especially for humans. Evolution and Human Behavior, 34(3), 164175.Google Scholar
Barclay, P. (2016). Biological markets and the effects of partner choice on cooperation and friendship. Current Opinion in Psychology, 7, 3338.Google Scholar
Baum, W. M., Richerson, P. J., Efferson, C. M., & Paciotti, B. M. (2004). Cultural evolution in laboratory micro-societies including traditions of rule-giving and rule-following. Evolution and Human Behavior, 25, 305326.Google Scholar
Bell, D. C., & Richard, A. J. (2000). Caregiving: The forgotten element in attachment. Psychological Inquiry, 11, 6983.Google Scholar
Bell, G. (2008). Selection: The Mechanism of Evolution, 2nd ed. Oxford: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Bercik, P., Verdu, E. F., Foster, J. A., et al. (2010). Chronic gastrointestinal inflammation induces anxiety-like behavior and alters central nervous system biochemistry in mice. Gastroenterology, 139(6), 21022112.Google Scholar
Bercik, P., Park, A. J., Sinclair, D., et al. (2011). The anxiolytic effect of Bifidobacterium longum NCC3001 involves vagal pathways for gut–brain communication. Neurogastroenterology & Motility, 23(12), 11321139.Google Scholar
Berte, N. A. (1988). K’ekchi’ horticultural labor exchange: Productive and reproductive implications. In Betzig, L., Borgerhofl Mulder, M., & Turke, P., eds., Human Reproductive Behavior: A Darwinian Perspective. Cambridge, UK: Cambridge University Press, pp. 8395.Google Scholar
Bhogal, M. S. (2017). Physical Attractiveness, Altruism and Fairness in a Game-Theoretic Framework. Unpublished doctoral dissertation, University of Wolverhampton.Google Scholar
Blumstein, D. T., Steinmetz, J., Armitage, K. B., & Daniel, J. C. (1997). Alarm calling in yellow-bellied marmots: II. The importance of direct fitness. Animal Behaviour, 53(1), 173184.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Bowles, S. (2009). Did warfare among ancestral hunter–gatherers affect the evolution of human social behaviors? Science, 324(5932), 12931298.Google Scholar
Boyd, R., & Richerson, P. J. (1982). Cultural transmission and the evolution of cooperative behavior. Human Ecology, 10(3), 325351.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Boyd, R., & Richerson, P. J. (1985). Culture and the Evolutionary Process. Chicago, IL: University of Chicago Press.Google Scholar
Boyd, R., & Richerson, P. J. (2005). The Origin and Evolution of Cultures. Oxford: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Bravo, J. A., Forsythe, P., Chew, M. V., et al. (2011). Ingestion of Lactobacillus strain regulates emotional behavior and central GABA receptor expression in a mouse via the vagus nerve. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences, 108(38), 1605016055.Google Scholar
Bressan, P., Colarelli, S. M., & Cavalieri, M. B. (2009). Biologically costly altruism depends on emotional closeness among step but not half or full sibling. Evolutionary Psychology, 7(1), 118132.Google Scholar
Brown, S. L., & Brown, R. M. (2006). Selective investment theory: Recasting the functional significance of close relationships. Psychological Inquiry, 17, 129.Google Scholar
Browning, L. E., Patrick, S. C., Rollins, L. A., Griffith, S. C., & Russell, A. F. (2012). Kin selection, not group augmentation, predicts helping in an obligate cooperatively breeding bird. Proceedings of the Royal Society of London B: Biological Sciences, 279(1743), 38613869.Google Scholar
Burda, H., Honeycutt, R. L., Begall, S., Locker-Grütjen, O., & Scharff, A. (2000). Are naked and common mole-rats eusocial and if so, why? Behavioral Ecology and Sociobiology, 47(5), 293303.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Burnstein, E., Crandall, C., & Kitayama, S. (1994). Some neo-Darwinian decision rules for altruism: Weighing cues for inclusive fitness as a function of the biological importance of the decision. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 67(5), 773789.Google Scholar
Carter, G., & Wilkinson, G. (2013). Does food sharing in vampire bats demonstrate reciprocity? Communicative & integrative biology, 6(6), e25783.Google Scholar
Carpenter, J., & Myers, C. K. (2010). Why volunteer? Evidence on the role of altruism, image, and incentives. Journal of Public Economics, 94(11), 911920.Google Scholar
Cheney, D. L., & Seyfarth, R. M. (1985). Vervet monkey alarm calls: Manipulation through shared information? Behaviour, 94(1), 150166.Google Scholar
Chiang, Y. S. (2010). Self-interested partner selection can lead to the emergence of fairness. Evolution and Human Behavior, 31(4), 265270.Google Scholar
Cialdini, R. B., Brown, S. L., Lewis, B. P., Luce, C., & Neuberg, S. L. (1997). Reinterpreting the empathy–altruism relationship: When one into one equals oneness. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 73(3), 481494.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Clark, M. S., & Mills, J. (1993). The difference between communal and exchange relationships: What it is and is not. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 19(6), 684691.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Clutton-Brock, T. (2009). Cooperation between non-kin in animal societies. Nature, 462(7269), 5157.Google Scholar
Colquhoun, L., Davies, J., & Workman, L. (2017). Kith or kin – Who do we favour? The relationship between emotional closeness and genetic relatedness in altruistic willingness to help. Presented at British Psychological Society Annual Conference.Google Scholar
Crittenden, A. N., & Marlowe, F. W. (2008). Allomaternal care among the Hadza of Tanzania. Human Nature, 19(3), 249262.Google Scholar
Curry, O., Roberts, S. G., & Dunbar, R. I. (2013). Altruism in social networks: Evidence for a “kinship premium.” British Journal of Psychology, 104(2), 283295.Google Scholar
Cutts, C., & Speakman, J. (1994). Energy savings in formation flight of pink-footed geese. Journal of Experimental Biology, 189(1), 251261.Google Scholar
Danielsbacka, M., & Tanskanen, A. O. (2012). Adolescent grandchildren’s perceptions of grandparents’ involvement in UK: An interpretation from life course and evolutionary theory perspective. European Journal of Ageing, 9, 329341.Google Scholar
Danielsbacka, M., Tanskanen, A. O., Jokela, M., & Rotkirch, A. (2011). Grandparental child care in Europe: Evidence for preferential investment in more certain kin. Evolutionary Psychology, 9, 324.Google Scholar
Darwin, C. (1859). On The Origin of Species by Means of Natural Selection, or Preservation of Favoured Races in the Struggle for Life. London: John Murray.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Davies, B. (2014). Thomas Aquinas’s Summa Theologiae: A Guide and Commentary. Oxford: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Dawkins, R. (2016). The Selfish Gene. 4th ed. Oxford: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
DeNault, L. K., & McFarlane, D. A. (1995). Reciprocal altruism between male vampire bats, Desmodus rotundus. Animal Behaviour, 49(3), 855856.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Deulofeu, J. (2014). Prisioneros con dilemas y estrategias dominantes: Teoría de juegos. [Prisoners with dilemmas and dominant strategies: Game theory], trans. London: Windmill Books and Vespa Design, RBA Collectibles.Google Scholar
Diamond, J. (2012). The World Until Yesterday: What Can We Learn from Traditional Societies? London: Penguin UK.Google Scholar
Dierkes, P., Heg, D., Taborsky, M., Skubic, E., & Achmann, R. (2005). Genetic relatedness in groups is sex‐specific and declines with age of helpers in a cooperatively breeding cichlid. Ecology Letters, 8(9), 968975.Google Scholar
Dobson, A. P. (1988). The population biology of parasite-induced changes in host behavior. Quarterly Review of Biology, 63(2), 139165.Google Scholar
Duffy, J. E., Morrison, C. L., & Ríos, R. (2000). Multiple origins of eusociality among sponge-dwelling shrimps (Synalpheus). Evolution, 54(2), 503516.Google Scholar
Dunbar, R. I., Clark, A., & Hurst, N. L. (1995). Conflict and cooperation among the Vikings: Contingent behavioral decisions. Ethology and Sociobiology, 16(3), 233246.Google Scholar
Efferson, C., Lalive, R., Richerson, P. J., McElreath, R., & Lubell, M. (2008). Conformists and mavericks: The empirics of frequency-dependent cultural transmission. Evolution and Human Behavior, 29(1), 5664.Google Scholar
Eisenberg, A. R. (1988). Grandchildren’s perspectives on relationships with grandparents: The influence of gender across generations. Sex Roles, 19, 205217.Google Scholar
Emlen, S. T., & Wrege, P. H. (1988). The role of kinship in helping decisions among white-fronted bee-eaters. Behavioral Ecology and Sociobiology, 23(5), 305315.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Essock-Vitale, S. M., & McGuire, M. T. (1980). Predictions derived from the theories of kin selection and reciprocation assessed by anthropological data. Ethology and Sociobiology, 1(3), 233243.Google Scholar
Essock-Vitale, S. M., & McGuire, M. T. (1985). Women’s lives viewed from an evolutionary perspective. II. Patterns of helping. Ethology and Sociobiology, 6(3), 155173.Google Scholar
Euler, H. A., & Weitzel, B. (1996). Discriminative grandparental solicitude as reproductive strategy. Human Nature, 7, 3959.Google Scholar
Feder, H. M. (1966). Cleaning symbioses in the marine environment. In Henry, S. M., ed., Associations of Microorganisms, Plants and Marine Organisms, Vol. 1. Cambridge, MA: Academic Press, pp. 327380.Google Scholar
Feldman, M., & Cavalli-Sforna, L. (1976). Cultural and biological evolutionary processes, selection for a trait under complex transmission. Theoretical Population Biology, 9, 238259.Google Scholar
Filiz-Ozbay, E., & Ozbay, E. Y. (2014). Effect of an audience in public goods provision. Experimental Economics, 17(2), 200214.Google Scholar
Fisher, R. A. (1930). The Genetical Theory of Natural Selection. Wotton-under-Edge: Clarendon Press.Google Scholar
Fortin, E. L. (1996). Classical Christianity and the Political Order: Reflections on the Theologico-political Problem. Lanham, MD: Rowman & Littlefield.Google Scholar
Frank, R. H. (1993). The strategic role of the emotions reconciling over-and undersocialized accounts of behavior. Rationality and Society, 5(2), 160184.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Gaulin, S. J., & Schlegel, A. (1980). Paternal confidence and paternal investment: A cross cultural test of a sociobiological hypothesis. Ethology and Sociobiology, 1(4), 301309.Google Scholar
Goehler, L. E., Park, S. M., Opitz, N., Lyte, M., & Gaykema, R. P. (2008). Campylobacter jejuni infection increases anxiety-like behavior in the holeboard: Possible anatomical substrates for viscerosensory modulation of exploratory behavior. Brain, Behavior, and Immunity, 22(3), 354366.Google Scholar
Gorrell, J. C., McAdam, A. G., Coltman, D. W., Humphries, M. M., & Boutin, S. (2010). Adopting kin enhances inclusive fitness in asocial red squirrels. Nature Communications, 1, 22.Google Scholar
Grafen, A. (1990). Biological signals as handicaps. Journal of Theoretical Biology, 144, 517546.Google Scholar
Grayson, D. K. (1993). Differential mortality and the Donner Party disaster. Evolutionary Anthropology: Issues, News, and Reviews, 2(5), 151159.Google Scholar
Grey, P. B., & Brogdon, E. (2017). Do step- and biological grandparents show differences in investment and emotional closeness with their grandchildren? Evolutionary Psychology, 15(1), 19.Google Scholar
Griskevicius, V., Tybur, J. M., & Van den Bergh, B. (2010). Going green to be seen: Status, reputation, and conspicuous conservation. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 98, 392404.Google Scholar
Gross, M. R., & Shine, R. (1981). Parental care and mode of fertilization in ectothermic vertebrates. Evolution, 35(4), 775793.Google Scholar
Gurven, M., Allen-Arave, W., Hill, K., & Hurtado, A. M. (2001). Reservation food sharing among the Ache of Paraguay. Human Nature, 12(4), 273297.Google Scholar
Haldane, J. B. S. (1932). The Causes of Evolution, 3rd ed. Harlow: Longmans, Green & Co.Google Scholar
Haldane, J. B. S. (1955). Population genetics. New Biology, 18, 3451.Google Scholar
Hames, R. (1987). Garden labor exchange among the Ye’kwana. Ethology and Sociobiology, 8(4), 259284.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Hamilton, W. D. (1964a). The genetical evolution of social behaviour I. Journal of Theoretical Biology, 7(1), 116.Google Scholar
Hamilton, W. D. (1964b). The genetical evolution of social behaviour II. Journal of Theoretical Biology, 7(1), 1752.Google Scholar
Hawkes, K. (1991). Showing off: Tests of an hypothesis about men’s foraging goals. Ethology and Sociobiology, 12(1), 2954.Google Scholar
He, W. (2005). Introduction: Kinship and family in international context. International Journal of Sociology and Social Policy, 25(3), 18.Google Scholar
Henrich, J., & McElreath, R. (2007). Dual-inheritance theory: The evolution of human cultural capacities and cultural evolution. In Dunbar, R. I. M. & Barrett, L., eds., Oxford Handbook of Evolutionary Psychology. Oxford: Oxford University Press, pp. 555570.Google Scholar
Hill, K., & Hurtado, A. M. (1996). Ache Life History: The Ecology and Demography of a Foraging People. London: Aldine.Google Scholar
Honeycutt, R. L. (1992). Naked mole-rats. American Scientist, 80(1), 4353.Google Scholar
Hughes, W. O., Oldroyd, B. P., Beekman, M., & Ratnieks, F. L. (2008). Ancestral monogamy shows kin selection is key to the evolution of eusociality. Science, 320(5880), 12131216.Google Scholar
Johnson, L. L., & Boyce, M. S. (1991). Female choice of males with low parasite loads in sage grouse. In Loye, J. E. & Zuk, M., eds., Bird Parasite Interactions: Ecology, Evolution, and Behavior. Oxford: Oxford University Press, pp. 377388.Google Scholar
Karlan, D., & McConnell, M. A. (2014). Hey look at me: The effect of giving circles on giving. Journal of Economic Behavior & Organization, 106, 402412.Google Scholar
Kobayashi, Y., & Yamamura, N. (2007). Evolution of signal emission by uninfested plants to help nearby infested relatives. Evolutionary Ecology, 21(3), 281294.Google Scholar
Korchmaros, J. D., & Kenny, D. A. (2001). Emotional closeness as a mediator of the effect of genetic relatedness on altruism. Psychological Science, 12(3), 262265.Google Scholar
Korchmaros, J. D., & Kenny, D. A. (2006). An evolutionary and close-relationship model of helping. Journal of Social and Personal Relationships, 23(1), 2143.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Kruger, D. J. (2003). Evolution and altruism: Combining psychological mediators with naturally selected tendencies. Evolution and Human Behavior, 24(2), 118125.Google Scholar
Lacetera, N., & Macis, M. (2010a). Do all material incentives for pro-social activities backfire? The response to cash and non-cash incentives for blood donations. Journal of Economic Psychology, 31(4), 738748.Google Scholar
Lacetera, N., & Macis, M. (2010b). Social image concerns and prosocial behavior: Field evidence from a nonlinear incentive scheme. Journal of Economic Behavior & Organization, 76(2), 225237.Google Scholar
Laham, S. M., Gonsalkorale, K., & von Hippel, W. (2005). Darwinian grandparenting: Preferential investment in more certain kin. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 31, 6372.Google Scholar
Laland, K. N., & Brown, G. R. (2011). Sense and Nonsense: Evolutionary Perspectives on Human Behaviour. Oxford: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Lee, R. B. (1979). The !Kung San: Men, Women, and Work in a Foraging Society. Cambridge, UK: Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
Lee, T. R., Mancini, J. A., & Maxwell, J. W. (1990). Sibling relationships in adulthood: Contact patterns and motivations. Journal of Marriage and the Family, 52(2), 431440.Google Scholar
Lehmann, L., Rousset, F., Roze, D., & Keller, L. (2007). Strong reciprocity or strong ferocity? A population genetic view of the evolution of altruistic punishment. American Naturalist, 170(1), 2136.Google Scholar
Lehmann, L., Feldman, M. W., & Foster, K. R. (2008). Cultural transmission can inhibit the evolution of altruistic helping. American Naturalist, 172(1), 1224.Google Scholar
Lentz, B. F., & Laband, D. N. (1989). Why so many children of doctors become doctors: Nepotism vs. human capital transfers. Journal of Human Resources, 24(3), 396413.Google Scholar
Lewin-Epstein, O., Aharonov, R., & Hadany, L. (2017). Microbes can help explain the evolution of host altruism. Nature Communications, 8, 14040.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Linardi, S., & McConnell, M. (2011). No excuses for good behavior. Journal of Public Economics, 95(5–6), 445454.Google Scholar
Littlefield, C. H., & Rushton, J. P. (1986). When a child dies: The sociobiology of bereavement. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 51(4), 797802.Google Scholar
Low, B. S., & Heinen, J. T. (1993). Population, resources, and environment: Implications of human behavioral ecology for conservation. Population and Environment, 15(1), 741.Google Scholar
Madsen, E. A., Tunney, R. J., Fieldman, G., et al. (2007). Kinship and altruism: A cross‐cultural experimental study. British Journal of Psychology, 98(2), 339359.Google Scholar
Marler, P. (1955). Characteristics of some animal calls. Nature, 176, 68.Google Scholar
Marler, P. (1957). Specific distinctiveness in the communication signals of birds. Behaviour, 11(1), 1338.Google Scholar
Maynard Smith, J. (1964). Group selection and kin selection. Nature, 201, 11451147.Google Scholar
McCullough, J. M., & Barton, E. Y. (1991). Relatedness and mortality risk during a crisis year: Plymouth colony, 1620–1621. Ethology and Sociobiology, 12(3), 195209.Google Scholar
McCullough, J. M., Heath, K. M., & Fields, J. D. (2006). Culling the cousins: Kingship, kinship, and competition in mid-millennial England. History of the Family, 11(1), 5966.Google Scholar
McElreath, R., Lubell, M., Richerson, P. J., et al. (2005). Applying evolutionary models to the laboratory study of social learning. Evolution and Human Behavior, 26(6), 483508.Google Scholar
Milius, S. (1998). The science of EEEEEK!: What a squeak can tell researchers about life, society, and all that. Science News, 154(11), 174175.Google Scholar
Mills, J., & Clark, M. S. (1994). Communal and exchange relationships: Controversies and research. In Erber, R. & Gilmour, R., eds., Theoretical Frameworks for Personal Relationships. Mahwah, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum, pp. 2942.Google Scholar
Mongkolsamrit, S., Kobmoo, N., Tasanathai, K., et al. (2012). Life cycle, host range and temporal variation of Ophiocordyceps unilateralis/Hirsutella formicarum on Formicine ants. Journal of Invertebrate Pathology, 111(3), 217224.Google Scholar
Moore, J. (1984). The evolution of reciprocal sharing. Ethology and Sociobiology, 5(1), 514.Google Scholar
Morris, K., & Goodall, J (1977). Competition for meat between chimpanzees and baboons of the Gombe National Park. Folia Primatologica. 28, 109121.Google Scholar
Murphy, G. P., & Dudley, S. A. (2009). Kin recognition: Competition and cooperation in Impatiens (Balsaminaceae). American Journal of Botany, 96(11), 19901996.Google Scholar
Neyer, F. J., & Lang, F. R. (2003). Blood is thicker than water: Kinship orientation across adulthood. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 84(2), 310321.Google Scholar
Nishida, T., Uehara, S., & Nyundo, R (1979). Predatory behavior among wild chimpanzees of the Mahale Mountains. Primates, 20, 120.Google Scholar
Nowak, M., & Sigmund, K. (2005) Evolution of indirect reciprocity. Nature, 437, 12911298.Google Scholar
Nowak, M. A., Tarnita, C. E., & Wilson, E. O. (2010). The evolution of eusociality. Nature, 466(7310), 10571062.Google Scholar
Owens, D., & Owens, M. (1984). Helping behaviour in brown hyenas. Nature, 308(5962), 843845.Google Scholar
Packer, C. (1977). Reciprocal altruism in olive baboons. Nature, 265, 441443.Google Scholar
Panchanathan, K., & Boyd, R. (2004). Indirect reciprocity can stabilize cooperation without the second-order free rider problem. Nature, 432(7016), 499502.Google Scholar
Pashos, A., & McBurney, D. H. (2008). Kin relationships and the caregiving biases of grandparents, aunts and uncles: A two generational questionnaire study. Human Nature, 19, 311330.Google Scholar
Pashos, A., Schwarz, S., & Bjorklund, D. F. (2016). Kin investment by step-grandparents – More than expected. Evolutionary Psychology, 14(1), 111.Google Scholar
Pollet, T. V., Roberts, S. G., & Dunbar, R. I. (2013). Going that extra mile: Individuals travel further to maintain face-to-face contact with highly related kin than with less related kin. PLoS ONE, 8(1), e53929.Google Scholar
Portin, P. (2015). A comparison of biological and cultural evolution. Journal of Genetics, 94(1), 155168.Google Scholar
Portugal, S. J., Hubel, T. Y., Fritz, J., et al. (2014). Upwash exploitation and downwash avoidance by flap phasing in ibis formation flight. Nature, 505(7483), 399402.Google Scholar
Refardt, D., Bergmiller, T., & Kümmerli, R. (2013). Altruism can evolve when relatedness is low: Evidence from bacteria committing suicide upon phage infection. Proceedings of the Royal Society of London B: Biological Sciences, 280(1759), 20123035.Google Scholar
Rege, M., & Telle, K. (2004). The impact of social approval and framing on cooperation in public good situations. Journal of Public Economics, 88(7), 16251644.Google Scholar
Richerson, P. J., & Boyd, R. (2005). Not by Genes Alone: How Culture Transformed Human Biology. Chicago, IL: University of Chicago Press.Google Scholar
Richerson, P. J., Baldini, R., Bell, A. V., et al. (2016). Cultural group selection plays an essential role in explaining human cooperation: A sketch of the evidence. Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 39, 168.Google Scholar
Riss, D. C., & Busse, C. D (1977). Fifty-day observation of a free-ranging adult male chimpanzee. Folia Primatologica. 28, 283297.Google Scholar
Rudebeck, G. (1950). The choice of prey and modes of hunting of predatory birds with special reference to their selective effect. Oikos, 2(1), 6588.Google Scholar
Rudebeck, G. (1951). The choice of prey and modes of hunting of predatory birds with special reference to their selective effect. Oikos, 3(2), 200231.Google Scholar
Seyfarth, R. M., & Cheney, D. L. (1984). Grooming, alliances, and reciprocal altruism in vervet monkeys. Nature, 308, 541543.Google Scholar
Shavit, Y., Fischer, C. S., & Koresh, Y. (1994). Kin and nonkin under collective threat: Israeli networks during the gulf war. Social Forces, 72(4), 11971215.Google Scholar
Singer, P. (1981). The Expanding Circle: Ethics and Sociobiology. New York: Farrar, Straus and Giroux.Google Scholar
Smith, M. S., Kish, B. J., & Crawford, C. B. (1987). Inheritance of wealth as human kin investment. Ethology and Sociobiology, 8(3), 171182.Google Scholar
Sober, E., & Wilson, D. S. (1998). Unto Others: The Evolution and Psychology of Unselfish Behavior. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press.Google Scholar
Soler, M., Soler, J. J., Martinez, J. G., & Moller, A. P. (1995). Magpie host manipulation by great spotted cuckoos: Evidence for an avian mafia? Evolution, 49, 770775.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Soltis, J., Boyd, R., & Richerson, P. J. (1995). Can group-functional behaviors evolve by cultural group selection?: An empirical test. Current Anthropology, 36(3), 473494.Google Scholar
Sprankel, H., Richarz, K., Ludwig, H., & Rott, R. (1978). Behavior alterations in tree shrews (Tupaia glis, Diard 1820) induced by Borna disease virus. Medical Microbiology and Immunology, 165(1), 118.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Stewart-Williams, S. (2007). Altruism among kin vs. nonkin: Effects of cost of help and reciprocal exchange. Evolution and Human Behavior, 28(3), 193198.Google Scholar
Stewart-Williams, S. (2018). The Ape That Understood the Universe: How the Mind and Culture Evolve. Cambridge, UK: Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
Storey, S., & Workman, L. (2013). The effects of temperature priming on cooperation in the iterated prisoner’s dilemma. Evolutionary Psychology, 11(1), 5267.Google Scholar
Sutton, M. (1982). Nationalism, Positivism, and Catholicism. The Politics of Charles Maurras and French Catholics 1890–1914. Cambridge, UK: Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
Tinbergen, L. (1960). The Dynamics of Insect and Bird Populations in Pine Woods. Leiden: Brill Archive.Google Scholar
Trivers, R. L. (1971). The evolution of reciprocal altruism. Quarterly Review of Biology, 46, 3557.Google Scholar
Trivers, R. L. (1985). Social Evolution. San Francisco, CA: Benjamin/Cummings.Google Scholar
Trivers, R. L. (2011). The Folly of Fools: The Logic of Deceit and Self-Deception in Human Life. New York: Basic Books.Google Scholar
Voelkl, B., Portugal, S. J., Unsöld, M., et al. (2015). Matching times of leading and following suggest cooperation through direct reciprocity during V-formation flight in ibis. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences, 112(7), 21152120.Google Scholar
Waibel, M., Keller, L., & Floreano, D. (2009). Genetic team composition and level of selection in the evolution of cooperation. IEEE Transactions on Evolutionary Computation, 13(3), 648660.Google Scholar
Waibel, M., Floreano, D., & Keller, L. (2011). A quantitative test of Hamilton’s rule for the evolution of altruism. PLoS Biology, 9(5), e1000615.Google Scholar
Waynforth, D. (2011). Mate choice and sexual selection. In Swami, V., ed., Evolutionary Psychology: A Critical Introduction. Hoboken, NJ: Wiley-Blackwell, pp. 107130.Google Scholar
Weimerskirch, H., Martin, J., Clerquin, Y., Alexandre, P., & Jiraskova, S. (2001). Energy saving in flight formation. Nature, 413(6857), 697698.Google Scholar
West, S. A., Griffin, A. S., & Gardner, A. (2007a). Evolutionary explanations for cooperation. Current Biology, 17(16), R661R672.Google Scholar
West, S. A., Griffin, A. S., & Gardner, A. (2007b). Social semantics: Altruism, cooperation, mutualism, strong reciprocity and group selection. Journal of Evolutionary Biology, 20(2), 415432.Google Scholar
West, S. A., El Mouden, C., & Gardner, A. (2011). Sixteen common misconceptions about the evolution of cooperation in humans. Evolution and Human Behavior, 32(4), 231262.Google Scholar
Wheeler, B. C. (2008). Selfish or altruistic? An analysis of alarm call function in wild capuchin monkeys, Cebus apella nigritus. Animal Behaviour, 76(5), 14651475.Google Scholar
Wilkinson, G. S. (1984). Reciprocal food sharing in the vampire bat. Nature, 308(5955), 181184.Google Scholar
Wilson, D. S. (2007). Evolution for Everyone: How Darwin’s Theory Can Change the Way We Think About Our Lives. New York: Delacorte Press.Google Scholar
Wilson, D. S., & Brown, J. M. (1994). Poecilochirus carabi: Behavioral and life-history adaptations to different hosts and the consequences of geographical shifts in host communities. In Houck, M. A., ed., Mites: Ecological and Evolutionary Analyses of Life-History Patterns. Berlin: Springer, pp. 122.Google Scholar
Wilson, D. S., & Sober, E. (1994). Reintroducing group selection to the human behavioral sciences. Behavioural and Brain Sciences, 17, 585654.Google Scholar
Wilson, D. S., & Wilson, E. O. (2007). Rethinking the theoretical foundation of sociobiology. Quarterly Review of Biology, 82(4), 327348.Google Scholar
Wilson, E. O. (2005). Kin selection as the key to altruism: Its rise and fall. Social Research, 72(1), 159166.Google Scholar
Wilson, E. O. (2012). The Social Conquest of Earth. New York: Norton.Google Scholar
Woolfenden, G. E. (1975). Florida scrub jay helpers at the nest. The Auk, 92(1), 115.Google Scholar
Workman, L., & Reader, W. (2014). Evolutionary Psychology, 3rd ed. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
Workman, L., & Reader, W. (2015). Evolution and Behavior. Abingdon: Routledge.Google Scholar
Wynne-Edwards, V. C. (1962). Animal Dispersion: In Relation to Social Behaviour. Edinburgh: Oliver and Boyd.Google Scholar
Zahavi, A. (1975). Mate selection: A selection for a handicap. Journal of Theoretical Biology, 53, 205214.Google Scholar
Zahavi, A., & Zahavi, A. (1997). The Handicap Principle: A Missing Piece of Darwin’s Puzzle. Oxford: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Ziker, J. (2005). Food sharing at meals: Kinship, reciprocity, and clustering in the Taimyr autonomous Okrug, northern Russia. Human Nature, 16, 178211.Google Scholar

References

Alexander, R. D. (1987). The Biology of Moral Systems. New York: Aldine de Gruyter.Google Scholar
Bandura, A. (1991). Social cognitive theory of moral thought and action. In Kurtines, W. M. & Gewirtz, J. L., eds., Handbook of Moral Behavior and Development, Vol. 1. Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum, pp. 54104.Google Scholar
Batson, C. D. (1991). The Altruism Question: Toward a Social-Psychological Answer. Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.Google Scholar
Boehm, C. (2000). Conflict and the evolution of social control. Journal of Consciousness Studies, 7, 79101.Google Scholar
Boehm, C. (2012). Moral Origins: The Evolution of Virtue Altruism and Shame. New York: Basic Books.Google Scholar
Brosnan, S. F., & de Waal, F. B. (2003). Monkeys reject unequal pay. Nature, 425, 297299.Google Scholar
Brown, S. L., & Brown, M. (2006). Selective investment theory: Recasting the functional significance of close relationships. Psychological Inquiry, 17, 3059.Google Scholar
Burnham, T., & Phelan, J. (2000). Mean Genes. New York: Perseus Publishing.Google Scholar
Buss, D. (2004). Evolutionary Psychology: The New Science of the Mind, 2nd ed. Boston, MA: Allyn & Bacon.Google Scholar
Cummins, D. (2005). Dominance, status, and social hierarchies. In Buss, D., ed., The Handbook of Evolutionary Psychology. Hoboken, NJ: John Wiley & Sons, pp. 676697.Google Scholar
Darwin, C. (1871). The Descent of Man, and Selection in Relation to Sex. London: John Murray.Google Scholar
Dawkins, R. (1989). The Selfish Gene. Oxford: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Denton, K. K., & Krebs, D. L. (2017). Rational and emotional sources of moral decision-making: An evolutionary-developmental account. Evolutionary Psychological Science, 3, 7285.Google Scholar
de Waal, F. B. M. (2008). Putting the altruism back in altruism. Annual Review of Psychology, 59, 279300.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
de Waal, F. B. M., & Brosnan, S. F. (2006). Simple and complex reciprocity in animals. In Kappeler, P. M. & van Schaik, C. P., eds., Cooperation in Primates and Humans. New York: Springer-Verlag, pp. 85105.Google Scholar
Fessler, D. M. T., & Haley, K. J. (2003). The strategy of affect: Emotions in human cooperation. In Hammerstein, P., ed., Genetic and Cultural Evolution of Cooperation. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press, pp. 736.Google Scholar
Greene, J. D. (2013). Moral Tribes: Emotion, Reason, and the Gap between Us and Them. New York: Penguin Press.Google Scholar
Haidt, J. (2001). The emotional dog and its rational tail: A social intuitionist approach to moral judgment. Psychological Review, 108, 814834.Google Scholar
Haidt, J., & Joseph, C. (2004). Intuitive ethics: How innately prepared intuitions generate culturally variable virtues. Daedalus, 133, 5566.Google Scholar
Hamilton, W. D. (1964). The evolution of social behavior. Journal of Theoretical Biology, 7, 152.Google Scholar
Haslam, N. (1997). Four grammars for primate social relations. In Simpson, J. A. & Kenrick, D. T., eds., Evolutionary Social Psychology. Mahwah, NJ: Erlbaum, pp. 297316.Google Scholar
Hauser, M. D. (2006). Moral Minds: How Nature Designed Our Universal Sense of Right and Wrong. New York: HarperCollins Publishers.Google Scholar
Henrich, J. (2016). The Secret of Our Success. Princeton, NJ: Princeton University Press.Google Scholar
Janicki, M. (2004). Beyond sociobiology: A kinder and gentler evolutionary view of human nature. In Crawford, C. & Salmon, C., eds., Evolutionary Psychology: Public Policy and Personal Decisions. Mahwah, NJ: Erlbaum, pp. 5172.Google Scholar
Kohlberg, L. (1984). Essays in Moral Development: The Psychology of Moral Development, Vol. 2. New York: Harper & Row.Google Scholar
Krebs, D. L. (2011). The Origins of Morality. New York: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Krebs, D. L., & Denton, K. (2006). Explanatory limitations of cognitive-developmental approaches to morality. Psychological Review, 113, 672675.Google Scholar
McCullough, M. E. (2008). Beyond Revenge: The Evolution of the Forgiveness Instinct. San Francisco, CA: Jossey-Bass.Google Scholar
McCullough, M. E., Kimeldorf, M. B., & Cohen, A. D. (2008). An adaptation for altruism? The social causes, social effects and social evolution of gratitude. Current Directions in Psychological Science, 17, 281285.Google Scholar
Miller, G. F. (2007). The sexual selection of moral virtues. Quarterly Review of Biology, 82, 97125.Google Scholar
Moll, J., di Oliveira-Sourza, R., Zahn, R., & Grafman, J. (2008). The cognitive neuroscience of moral emotions. In Sinnott-Armstrong, W., ed., Moral Psychology: The Neuroscience of Morality: Emotion, Brain Disorders, and Development. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press, pp. 118.Google Scholar
Nowak, M. (2006). Five rules for the evolution of cooperation. Science, 314, 15601563.Google Scholar
Nowak, M. A., Tarnita, C. T., & Wilson, E. O. (2010). The evolution of eusociality. Nature, 466, 10571062.Google Scholar
Pinker, S. (2008). The moral instinct. New York Times, January 13.Google Scholar
Richerson, P. J., & Boyd, R. (2001). The evolution of subjective commitment: A tribal instincts hypothesis. In Nesse, R., ed., Evolution and the Capacity for Commitment. New York: Russell Sage Foundation, pp. 186219.Google Scholar
Richerson, P. J., & Boyd, R. (2005). Not by Genes Alone: How Culture Transformed Human Evolution. Chicago, IL: University of Chicago Press.Google Scholar
Rozin, P., Haidt, J., & McCauley, C. R. (2000). Disgust. In Lewis, M. & Haviland-Jones, J. M., eds., Handbook of Emotions, 2nd ed. New York: Guilford Press, pp. 637653.Google Scholar
Schnall, S., Haidt, J., Clore, G. L., & Jordan, A. H. (2008). Disgust as embodied moral judgment. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 34, 10961109.Google Scholar
Sober, E., & Wilson, D. S. (1998). Unto Others: The Evolution and Psychology of Unselfish Behavior. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press.Google Scholar
Tajfel, H., & Turner, J. C. (1985). The social identity theory of intergroup behavior. In Worchel, S. & Austin, W. G., eds., Psychology of Intergroup Relations. Chicago, IL: Nelson-Hall, pp. 724.Google Scholar
Thompson, R., Meyer, S., & McGinley, M. (2006). Understanding values in relationships: The development of conscience. In Killen, M. & Smetana, J., eds., Handbook of Moral Development. Mahwah, NJ: Erlbaum, pp. 267298.Google Scholar
Tooby, J., & Cosmides, L. (1996). Friendship and the banker’s paradox: Other pathways to the evolution of adaptations for altruism. Proceedings of the British Academy, 88, 119143.Google Scholar
Trivers, R. L. (1971). The evolution of reciprocal altruism. Quarterly Review of Biology, 46, 3557.Google Scholar
Trivers, R. (1985). Social Evolution. Menlo Park, CA: Benjamin Cummings.Google Scholar
Trivers, R. (2006). Reciprocal altruism: 30 years later. In Kappeler, P. M. & Van Schaik, C. P., eds., Cooperation in Primates and Humans. New York: Springer-Verlag, pp. 6784.Google Scholar
Wilson, J. Q. (1993). The Moral Sense. New York: The Free Press.Google Scholar
Wright, R. (1994). The Moral Animal. New York: Pantheon Books.Google Scholar
Wynne-Edwards, V. C. (1962). Animal Dispersion in Relation to Social Behavior. Edinburgh: Oliver and Boyd.Google Scholar

References

Alexander, R. D. (1987). The Biology of Moral Systems. New York: Aldine de Gruyter.Google Scholar
Anderson, J. R., Bucher, B., Chijiiwa, H., et al. (2017). Third-party social evaluations of humans by monkeys and dogs. Neuroscience and Biobehavioral Reviews, 82, 95109.Google Scholar
Arvan, M. (2013). Bad news for conservatives? Moral judgments and the Dark Triad personality traits: A correlational study. Neuroethics, 6(2), 307318.Google Scholar
Bartholomew, J. (2015). The awful rise of “virtue signalling”. Spectator, April 18.Google Scholar
Bernhard, H., Fischbacher, U., & Fehr, E. (2006). Parochial altruism in humans. Nature, 442(7105), 912915.Google Scholar
Bowles, S., & Choi, J. K. (2003). The first property rights revolution. Presented at: “Workshop on the Co-evolution of Behaviors and Institutions,” Santa Fe Institute.Google Scholar
Bowles, S., Choi, J. K., & Hopfensitz, A. (2003). The co-evolution of individual behaviours and social institutions. Journal of Theoretical Biology, 223(2), 135147.Google Scholar
Brown, R. (1986). The Nature of Social Laws: Machiavelli to Mill. Cambridge, UK: Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
Chagnon, N. (2012). The Yanomamo. Scarborough, ON: Nelson Education.Google Scholar
Choi, J. K., & Bowles, S. (2007). The coevolution of parochial altruism and war. Science, 318(5850), 636640.Google Scholar
Curry, O. S. (2016). Morality as cooperation: A problem-centred approach. In Shackelford, T. K. & Hansen, R. D., eds., The Evolution of Morality. New York: Springer International, pp. 2751.Google Scholar
Curry, O. S., Mullins, D. A., & Whitehouse, H. (2019). Is it good to cooperate? Testing the theory of morality-as-cooperation in 60 societies. Current Anthropology, 60, 1.Google Scholar
Cushman, F., & Greene, J. D. (2012). Finding faults: How moral dilemmas illuminate cognitive structure. Social Neuroscience, 7(3), 269279.Google Scholar
de Waal, F. B. M. (1996). Good Natured: The Origins of Right and Wrong in Humans and Other Animals. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press.Google Scholar
DeScioli, P., & Kurzban, R. (2009). Mysteries of morality. Cognition, 112(2), 281299.Google Scholar
DeScioli, P., & Kurzban, R. (2013). A solution to the mysteries of morality. Psychological bulletin, 139(2), 477496.Google Scholar
DeScioli, P., Gilbert, S. S., & Kurzban, R. (2012). Indelible victims and persistent punishers in moral cognition. Psychological Inquiry, 23(2), 143149.Google Scholar
Dinesen, P. T., & Sønderskov, K. M. (2015). Ethnic diversity and social trust: Evidence from the micro-context. American Sociological Review, 80(3) 550573.Google Scholar
Dunbar, R. I. (1992). Neocortex size as a constraint on group size in primates. Journal of Human Evolution, 22(6), 469493.Google Scholar
Dunbar, R. I., & Spoors, M. (1995). Social networks, support cliques, and kinship. Human Nature, 6(3), 273290.Google Scholar
Fehr, E., & Fischbacher, U. (2004). Third-party punishment and social norms. Evolution and Human Behavior, 25, 6387.Google Scholar
Fiske, A. P. (1992). The four elementary forms of sociality. Psychological Review, 99(4), 689723.Google Scholar
Gavrilets, S. (2015). Collective action and the collaborative brain. Journal of the Royal Society Interface, 12(102), 20141067.Google Scholar
Gollwitzer, M., & Keller, L. (2010). What you did only matters if you are one of us: Offenders’ group membership moderates the effect of criminal history on punishment severity. Social Psychology, 41(1), 2026.Google Scholar
Gray, K., Young, L., & Waytz, A. (2012). Mind perception is the essence of morality. Psychological Inquiry, 23(2), 101124.Google Scholar
Haidt, J. (2012). The Righteous Mind: Why Good People Are Divided by Religion and Politics. New York: Pantheon.Google Scholar
Haidt, J., Koller, S. H., & Dias, M. G. (1993). Affect, culture, and morality, or is it wrong to eat your dog? Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 65(4), 613628.Google Scholar
Hardy, I. C. (1997). Possible factors influencing vertebrate sex ratios: An introductory overview. Applied Animal Behaviour Science, 51(3–4), 217241.Google Scholar
Hauser, M. (2006). Moral Minds: How Nature Designed Our Universal Sense of Right and Wrong. New York: Ecco/HarperCollins Publishers.Google Scholar
Hill, K., & Hurtado, A. M. (2009). Cooperative breeding in South American hunter–gatherers. Proceedings of the Royal Society of London B: Biological Sciences, 276, rspb20091061.Google Scholar
Hrdy, S. B. (1979). Infanticide among animals: a review, classification, and examination of the implications for the reproductive strategies of females. Ethology and Sociobiology, 1(1), 1340.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Hrdy, S. B. (2011). Mothers and Others. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press.Google Scholar
Iyer, A., Jetten, J., & Haslam, S. A. (2012). Sugaring o’er the devil: Moral superiority and group identification help individuals downplay the implications of ingroup rule-breaking. European Journal of Social Psychology, 42(2), 141149.Google Scholar
Jetten, J., & Hornsey, M. J. (2011). The many faces of rebels. In Jetten, J. & Hornsey, M. J., eds., Rebels in Groups: Dissent, Deviance, Difference, and Defiance. New York: Blackwell, pp. 113.Google Scholar
Jonason, P. K., Lyons, M., Bethell, E. J., & Ross, R. (2013). Different routes to limited empathy in the sexes: Examining the links between the Dark Triad and empathy. Personality and Individual Differences, 54(5), 572576.Google Scholar
Kant, I. (1999). Critique of Pure Reason. Cambridge, UK: Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
Kelly, R. C. (2000). Warless Societies and the Origin of War. Ann Arbor: University of Michigan Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Krebs, D. L. (2011). The Origins of Morality: An Evolutionary Account. New York: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Krebs, J. R., & Davies, N. B., eds. (2009). Behavioral Ecology: An Evolutionary Approach. Hoboken, NJ: John Wiley & Sons.Google Scholar
Kroeber, A. L. (1917). The superorganic. American Anthropologist, 19(2), 163213.Google Scholar
Langergraber, K., Schubert, G., Rowney, C., et al. (2011). Genetic differentiation and the evolution of cooperation in chimpanzees and humans. Proceedings of the Royal Society of London B: Biological Sciences, 278, rspb20102592.Google Scholar
Legare, C. H., & Watson-Jones, R. E. (2015). The evolution and ontogeny of ritual. In Buss, D. M., ed., The Handbook of Evolutionary Psychology. Hoboken, NJ: Wiley, pp. 119.Google Scholar
Marques, J. M., & Paez, D. (1994). The “black sheep effect”: Social categorization, rejection of ingroup deviates, and perception of group variability. European Review of Social Psychology, 5(1), 3768.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Marques, J. M., Robalo, E. M., & Rocha, S. A. (1992). Ingroup bias and the “black sheep” effect: Assessing the impact of social identification and perceived variability on group judgements. European Journal of Social Psychology, 22(4), 331352.Google Scholar
McAndrew, F. T. (2002). New evolutionary perspectives on altruism: Multilevel-selection and costly-signaling theories. Current Directions in Psychological Science, 11(2), 7982.Google Scholar
McPherson, M., Smith-Lovin, L., & Cook, J. M. (2001). Birds of a feather: Homophily in social networks. Annual Review of Sociology, 27, 415444.Google Scholar
Meer, T. V. D., & Tolsma, J. (2014). Ethnic diversity and its effects on social cohesion. Annual Review of Sociology, 40, 459478.Google Scholar
Nitzschner, M., Melis, A. P., Kaminski, J., & Tomasello, M. (2012). Dogs (Canis familiaris) evaluate humans on the basis of direct experiences only. PLoS ONE, 7(10), e46880.Google Scholar
Putnam, R. D. (2007). E pluribus unum: Diversity and community in the twenty‐first century the 2006 Johan Skytte Prize Lecture. Scandinavian Political Studies, 30(2), 137174.Google Scholar
Rebers, S., & Koopmans, R. (2012). Altruistic punishment and between-group competition. Human Nature, 23(2), 173190.Google Scholar
Ridley, M. (1996). The Origins of Virtue. London: Penguin Books.Google Scholar
Schaller, M., & Duncan, L. A. (2007). The behavioral immune system: Its evolution and social psychological implications. In Forgas, J. P., Haselton, M. G., & von Hippel, W. Evolution and the Social Mind: Evolutionary Psychology and Social Cognition. New York: Psychology Press, pp. 293307.Google Scholar
Sherif, M. (1958). Superordinate goals in the reduction of intergroup conflict. American Journal of Sociology, 63, 349356.Google Scholar
Sloan-Wilson, D., Sloan, M., & Price, M. (n.d.). Is there a universal morality? Introduction and overview of responses. This View of Life. The Evolution Institute. https://evolution-institute.org/is-there-a-universal-morality-introduction-and-overview-of-responses.Google Scholar
Tomasello, M., Melis, A. P., Tennie, C., et al. (2012). Two key steps in the evolution of human cooperation: The interdependence hypothesis. Current Anthropology, 53(6), 673692.Google Scholar
Trivers, R. L. (1971). The evolution of reciprocal altruism. Quarterly Review of Biology, 46(1), 3557.Google Scholar
Valdesolo, P., & DeSteno, D. (2007). Moral hypocrisy. Psychological Science, 18(8), 689690.Google Scholar
van der Meer, T. (2015). Care is required when making assertions about the relationship between diversity and social cohesion. Democratic Audit Blog. March 9. www.democraticaudit.com/2015/03/09/care-is-required-when-making-assertions-about-the-relationship-between-diversity-and-social-cohesion.Google Scholar
van Prooijen, J. W. (2006). Retributive reactions to suspected offenders: The importance of social categorizations and guilt probability. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 32(6), 715726.Google Scholar
Wrangham, R. W. (1987). African apes: The significance of African apes for reconstructing human social evolution. In Kinzey, W. G., ed., Evolution of Human Behavior: Primate Models. New York: SUNY Press, pp. 5171.Google Scholar
Wright, R. (1994). The Moral Animal. New York: Pantheon.Google Scholar

References

Alexander, R. D. (1974). The evolution of social behavior. Annual Review of Ecology and Systematic, 5, 325383.Google Scholar
Alexander, R. D. (1979). Darwinism and Human Affairs. Seattle, WA: University of Washington Press.Google Scholar
Anthrobase (2009). Segmentary Lineage/Segmentary Opposition. www.anthrobase.com/Dic/eng/def/segmentary-lineage.htm.Google Scholar
Bohannan, L. (1958). Political aspects of Tiv social organization. In Middleton, J. & Tait, D., eds., Tribes without Rulers: Studies in African Segmentary Systems. London: Routledge & Kegan Paul, pp. 3366.Google Scholar
Chapais, B. (2008). Primeval Kinship: How Pair-Bonding Gave Birth to Human Society. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press.Google Scholar
Coe, K., Palmer, A. L., Palmer, C. T., & DeVito, C. L. (2010). Culture, altruism, and conflict between ancestors and descendants. Structure and Dynamics, 4(3), 117.Google Scholar
Cronk, L. (1999). That Complex Whole: Culture and the Evolution of Human Behavior. Boulder, CO: Westview Press.Google Scholar
Dawkins, R. (1982). The Extended Phenotype. Oxford: W. H. Freeman.Google Scholar
Durkheim, E. (1961/1912). The Elementary Forms of the Religious Life. New York: Collier.Google Scholar
Emlen, S. T., Wrege, P. H., & Demong, N. J. (1995). Making decisions in the family: An evolutionary perspective. American Scientist, 83(2), 148157.Google Scholar
Evans-Pritchard, E. E. (1940). The Nuer. London: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Flinn, M. V. (2011). Evolutionary anthropology of the human family. In Salmon, C. & Shackelford, T., eds., Evolutionary Family Psychology. New York: Oxford University Press, pp. 1232.Google Scholar
Fortes, M. (1969). Kinship and the Social Order: The Legacy of Lewis Henry Morgan. Chicago, IL: Aldine.Google Scholar
Fox, R. (1967). Kinship and Marriage. Harmondsworth: Penguin.Google Scholar
Geary, D. C., & Flinn, M. V. (2001). Evolution of human parental behavior and the human family. Parenting: Science and Practice, 1(1 & 2), 561.Google Scholar
Geary, D. C., Bailey, D. H., & Oxford, J. (2011). Reflections on the human family. In Salmon, C. & Shackelford, T., eds., Evolutionary Family Psychology. New York: Oxford University Press, pp. 365385.Google Scholar
Goodale, J. C. (1962). Marriage contracts among the Tiwi. Ethnology, 1(4), 452466.Google Scholar
Goodall, J. (1986). The Chimpanzees of Gombe: Patterns of Behavior. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press.Google Scholar
Gorelik, B. K., Shackelford, T. K., & Salmon, C. A. (2011). Between conflict and cooperation: New horizons in the evolutionary science of the human family. In Salmon, C. & Shackelford, T., eds., Evolutionary Family Psychology. New York: Oxford University Press, pp. 386398.Google Scholar
Hepper, P. (2011). Kin recognition. In Salmon, C. & Shackelford, T., eds., Evolutionary Family Psychology. New York: Oxford University Press, pp. 211229.Google Scholar
Hrdy, S. B. (2005). Mother Nature: A History of Mothers, Infants, and Natural Selection. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press.Google Scholar
Hughes, A. L. (1988). Evolution and Human Kinship. New York: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Huntington, G. E., & Hostetler, J. A. (2002). The Hutterites in North America. Mason, OH: Cengage Learning.Google Scholar
Keesing, R. (1975). Kin Groups and Social Structure. New York: Holt, Rinehart and Winston.Google Scholar
Kramer, K., & Greaves, R. (2011). Postmarital residence and bilateral kin associations among hunter–gatherers: Pumé foragers living in the best of both worlds. Human Nature, 22, 4163.Google Scholar
Miller, G. (2000). The Mating Mind: How Sexual Choice Shaped the Evolution of Human Nature. New York: Random House.Google Scholar
Mock, D. W. (2011). The evolution of relationships in nonhuman families. In Salmon, C. & Shackelford, T., eds., Evolutionary Family Psychology. New York: Oxford University Press, pp. 5162.Google Scholar
Murdock, G. P. (1949). Social Structure. New York: The MacMillan Company.Google Scholar
Palagi, E., & Norscia, I. (2013). Bonobos protect and console friends and kin. PLoS ONE, 8(11), e79290.Google Scholar
Palmer, C. T., & Begley, R. O. (2015). Costly signaling theory, sexual selection, and the influence of ancestors on religious behavior. In Slone, J., ed., The Attraction of Religion: Connecting Religion, Sex, and Evolution. London: Bloomsbury Press, pp. 93107.Google Scholar
Palmer, C. T., Begley, R. O., Coe, K., & Steadman, L. B. (2013). Moral elevation and traditions: Ancestral encouragement of altruism through ritual and myth. Journal of Ritual Studies, 27(2), 8396.Google Scholar
Palmer, C. T., & Coe, K. (2010). Parenting, courtship, Disneyland and the human brain. International Journal of Tourism Anthropology, 1(1), 114.Google Scholar
Palmer, C. T., Coe, K., & Steadman, L. B. (2016). Reconceptualizing the human social niche: How it came to exist and how it is changing. Current Anthropology, 57(S13), S181S191.Google Scholar
Palmer, C. T., & Palmer, A. L. (2016). Why traditional ethical codes prescribing self-sacrifice are a puzzle to evolutionary theory: The example of Besa. Journal of the Association for the Study of Ethical Behavior/Evolutionary Biology in Literature, 12(1), 4050.Google Scholar
Palmer, C. T., & Steadman, L. B. (1997). Human kinship as a descendant-leaving strategy: A solution to an evolutionary puzzle. Journal of Social and Evolutionary Systems, 20(1), 3951.Google Scholar
Palmer, C. T., Steadman, L. B., & Coe, K. (2006). More kin: An effect of the tradition of marriage. Structure and Dynamics, 1(2), 116.Google Scholar
Quirko, H. N. (2011). Fictive kinship and induced altruism. In Salmon, C. & Shackelford, T., eds., Evolutionary Family Psychology. New York: Oxford University Press, pp. 310328.Google Scholar
Rollins, B. Y., Belue, R. Z., & Francis, L. A. (2010). The beneficial effect of family meals on obesity differs by race, sex, and household education: the national survey of children’s health, 2003–2004. Journal of the American Dietetic Association, 110(9), 13351339.Google Scholar
Sahlins, M. (1961). The segmentary lineage: An organization of predatory expansion. American Anthropologist, 63(2), 322345.Google Scholar
Salmon, C. A., & Malcolm, J. (2011). Parent–offspring conflict. In Salmon, C. & Shackelford, T., eds., Evolutionary Family Psychology. New York: Oxford University Press, pp. 8396.Google Scholar
Smaldino, P. E., Newson, L., Schank, J. C., & Richerson, P. J. (2013). Simulating the evolution of the human family: Cooperative breeding increases in harsh environments. PLoS ONE, 8(11), e80753.Google Scholar
Steadman, L. B., & Palmer, C. T. (1995). Religion as an identifiable traditional behavior subject to natural selection. Journal of Social and Evolutionary Systems, 18(2), 149164.Google Scholar
Steadman, L. B., Palmer, C. T., & Tilley, C. F. (1996). The universality of ancestor worship. Ethnology, 35, 6376.Google Scholar
Stevens, J. M. G., Vervaecke, H., & Van Elsacker, L. (2007). The bonobo’s adaptive potential: Social relations under captive conditions. In Furuichi, T. & Thompson, J., eds., The Bonobos: Behavior, Ecology, and Conservation. New York: Springer, pp. 1938.Google Scholar
Trivers, R. L. (1974). Parent–offspring conflict. American Zoologist, 14(1), 249264.Google Scholar
Turner, V. (1968). The Drums of Affliction: A Study of Religious Processes among the Ndembu of Zambia. Oxford: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
van den Berghe, P. L., & Barash, D. P. (1977). Inclusive fitness and human family structure. American Anthropologist, 79, 809823.Google Scholar
Voland, E., & Voland, R. (1995). Parent–offspring conflict, the extended phenotype, and the evolution of conscience. Journal of Social and Evolutionary Systems, 18(4), 397412.Google Scholar
Vonnegut, K. (1976). Slapstick, or Lonesome No More! New York: Delacorte Press.Google Scholar
West, S. A., El Mouden, C., & Gardner, A. (2011). Sixteen common misconceptions about the evolution of cooperation in humans. Evolution and Human Behavior, 32, 231262.Google Scholar
West-Eberhard, M. J. (1975). The evolution of social behavior by kin selection. Quarterly Review of Biology, 50, 133.Google Scholar
Wrangham, R. W. (1980). An ecological model of female-bonded primate groups. Behaviour, 75, 262300.Google Scholar
Wright, R. (1994). The Moral Animal: Why We Are the Way We Are. New York: Vintage.Google Scholar

References

Anderson, R. M., & May, R. M. (1991). Infectious Disease of Humans: Dynamics and Control. Oxford: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Billing, J., & Sherman, P. W. (1998). Antimicrobial functions of spices: Why some like it hot. Quarterly Review of Biology, 73, 349.Google Scholar
Blute, M. (2010). Darwinian Sociocultural Evolution: Solutions to Dilemmas in Cultural and Social Theory. Cambridge, UK: Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
Brown, G. D. A., Fincher, C. L., & Walasek, L. (2016). Personality, parasites, political attitudes, and cooperation: A model of how infection prevalence influences openness and social group formation. Topics in Cognitive Science, 8, 98117.Google Scholar
Cashdan, E. (2001). Ethnic diversity and its environmental determinants: Effects on climate, pathogens, and habitat diversity. American Anthropology, 103, 968991.Google Scholar
Cashdan, E., & Steele, M. (2013). Pathogen prevalence, group bias, and collectivism in the standard cross-cultural sample. Human Nature, 24, 5975.Google Scholar
Chiao, J. Y., & Blizinsky, K. D. (2010). Culture–gene coevolution of individualism–collectivism and the serotonin transporter gene. Proceedings of the Royal Society B, 277, 529537.Google Scholar
Clay, R., Terrizzi, J. A. Jr., & Shook, N. J. (2012). Individual differences in the behavioral immune system and the emergence of cultural systems. Journal of Social Psychology, 43, 174184.Google Scholar
Corby-Harris, V., & Promislow, D. E. L. (2008). Host ecology shapes geographical variation for resistance to bacterial infection in Drosophila melanogaster. Journal of Animal Ecology, 77, 768776.Google Scholar
Curtis, V. A. (2007). Dirt, disgust and disease: A natural history of hygiene. Journal of Epidemiology and Community Health, 61, 660664.Google Scholar
Curtis, V., Aunger, R., & Rabie, T. (2004). Evidence that disgust evolved to protect from risk of disease. Proceedings of the Royal Society B, 271, 1731.Google Scholar
Curtis, V. A., de Barra, M., & Aunger, H. (2011). Disgust as an adaptive system for disease avoidance behaviour. Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society B, 366, 389401.Google Scholar
de Barra, M., DeBruine, L., Jones, B., & Curtis, V. A. (2013). Illness in childhood predicts face preferences in adulthood. Evolution and Human Behavior, 34, 384389.Google Scholar
DeBruine, L. M., Little, A. C., & Jones, B. C. (2012). Extending parasite-stress theory to variation in human mate preferences. Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 35, 8687.Google Scholar
Denic, S., & Nicholls, M. G. (2007). Genetic benefits of consanguinity through selection of genotypes protective against malaria. Human Biology, 79, 145158.Google Scholar
Denic, S., Nagelkerke, N., & Agarwal, M. M. (2008a). Consanguineous marriages and endemic malaria: Can inbreeding increase population fitness? Malaria Journal, 7, 150.Google Scholar
Denic, S., Nagelkerke, N., & Agarwal, M. M. (2008b). Consanguineous marriages: Do genetic benefits outweigh its costs in populations with alpha(+)-thalassemia, hemoglobin S, and malaria? Evolution and Human Behavior, 29, 364369.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Diamond, J. (1998). Guns, Germs and Steel: The Fates of Human Societies. New York: W.W. Norton.Google Scholar
Dionne, M., Miller, K. M., Dodson, J. J., Caron, F., & Bernatchez, L. (2007). Clinical variation in MHC diversity with temperature: Evidence for the role of host–pathogen interaction on local adaptation in Atlantic salmon. Evolution, 61, 21542164.Google Scholar
Dobson, A. P., & Carper, E. R. (1996). Infectious diseases and human population history. BioScience, 46, 115126.Google Scholar
Duncan, L. A., Schaller, M., & Park, J. H. (2009). Perceived vulnerability to disease: Development and validation of a 15-item self-report instrument. Personality and Individual Differences, 47, 541546.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Durham, W. H. (1991). Coevolution: Genes, Culture and Human Diversity. Stanford, CA: Stanford University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Ewald, P. W. (1994). Evolution of Infectious Disease. New York: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Faulkner, J., Schaller, M., Park, J. H., & Duncan, L.A. (2004). Evolved disease-avoidance mechanisms and contemporary xenophobic attitudes. Group Processes and Intergroup Relations, 7, 333353.Google Scholar
Fessler, D. M. T., Eng, S. J., & Navarrete, C. D. (2005). Elevated disgust sensitivity in the first trimester of pregnancy: Evidence supporting the compensatory prophylaxis hypothesis. Evolution and Human Behavior, 26, 344351.Google Scholar
Fincher, C. L., & Thornhill, R. (2008a). A parasite-driven wedge: Infectious diseases may explain language and other biodiversity. Oikos, 117, 12891297.Google Scholar
Fincher, C. L., & Thornhill, R. (2008b). Assortative sociality, limited dispersal, infectious disease and the genesis of the global pattern of religion diversity. Proceedings of the Royal Society of London, Biological Sciences, 275, 25872594.Google Scholar
Fincher, C. L., & Thornhill, R. (2012a). Parasite-stress promotes in-group assortative sociality: The cases of strong family ties and heightened religiosity. Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 35, 6179.Google Scholar
Fincher, C. L., & Thornhill, R. (2012b). The parasite-stress theory may be a general theory of culture and sociality response. Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 35, 99119.Google Scholar
Fincher, C. L., Thornhill, R., Murray, D. R., & Schaller, M. (2008). Pathogen prevalence predicts human cross-cultural variability in individualism/collectivism. Proceedings of the Royal Society of London, Biological Sciences, 275, 12791285.Google Scholar
Fumagalli, M., Sironi, M., Pozzoli, U., et al. (2011). Signatures of environmental genetic adaptation pinpoint pathogens as the main selective pressure through human evolution. PLoS Genetics, 7, e1002355.Google Scholar
Gangestad, S. W., & Buss, D. M. (1993). Pathogen prevalence and human mate preference. Ethology and Sociobiology, 14, 8996.Google Scholar
Gangestad, S. W., Haselton, M. G., & Buss, D. M. (2006). Evolutionary foundations of cultural variation: Evoked culture and mate preferences. Psychological Inquiry, 17, 7595.Google Scholar
Good, C. M. (1972). Salt, trade, and disease: Aspects of development in Africa’s northern Great Lakes region. International Journal of African Historical Studies, 5, 543586.Google Scholar
Guernier, V., Hochberg, M. E., & Guegan, J. (2004). Ecology drives the worldwide distribution of human diseases. PLoS Biology, 2, e141.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Gurven, M., Allen-Arave, W., Hill, K., & Hurtado, M. (2000). “It’s a Wonderful Life”: Signaling generosity among the Ache of Paraguay. Evolution and Human Behavior, 21, 263282.Google Scholar
Hamilton, W. D., & Zuk, M. (1982). Heritable true fitness and bright birds: A role for parasites? Science, 218, 284387.Google Scholar
Hoben, A. D. (2011). An evolutionary investigation of consanguineous marriages. Unpublished doctoral dissertation, University of Groningen.Google Scholar
Hoben, A. D., Buunk, A. P., Fincher, C. L., & Thornhill, R. (2010). On the adaptive origins and maladaptive consequences of human inbreeding: Parasite prevalence, immune functioning, and consanguineous marriage. Evolutionary Psychology, 8, 658676.Google Scholar
Inbar, Y., Pizarro, D. A., Iyer, R., & Raidt, J. (2012). Disgust sensitivity, political conservatism, and voting. Social Psychological and Personality Science, 5, 537544.Google Scholar
Jones, B. C., Feinberg, D. R., Watkins, C. D., et al. (2013a). Pathogen disgust predicts women’s preferences for masculinity in men’s voices, faces, and bodies. Behavioral Ecology, 24, 373379.Google Scholar
Jones, B. C., Fincher, C. L., Welling, L. L. M., et al. (2013b). Salivary cortisol and pathogen disgust predict men’s preferences for feminine shape cues in women’s faces. Biological Psychology, 92, 233240.Google Scholar
Kaltz, O., Gandon, S., Michalakis, Y., & Shykoff, J. A. (1999). Local maladaptation in the anther-smut fungus Microbotryum violaceum to its host plant Silene latifolia: Evidence from a cross-inoculation experiment. Evolution, 53, 395407.Google Scholar
Kouznetsova, D., Stevenson, R. J., Oaten, M. J., & Case, T. I. (2012). Disease-avoidant behaviour and its consequences. Psychology and Health, 27, 491506.Google Scholar
Lee, A. J., & Zietsch, B. P. (2011). Experimental evidence that women’s mate preferences are directly influenced by cues of pathogen prevalence and resource scarcity. Biology Letters, 7, 892895.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Letendre, K., Fincher, C. L., & Thornhill, R. (2010). Does infectious disease cause global variation in the frequency of intrastate armed conflict and civil war? Biological Reviews, 85, 669683.Google Scholar
Letendre, K., Fincher, C. L., & Thornhill, R. (2012). Infectious disease, collectivism, and warfare. In Shackelford, T. & Weekes-Shackelford, V., eds., The Oxford Handbook on Evolutionary Perspectives on Violence, Homicide, and Warfare. New York: Oxford University Press, pp. 351371.Google Scholar
Little, A. C., DeBruine, L. M., & Jones, B. C. (2010). Exposure to visual cues of pathogen contagion changes preferences for masculinity and symmetry in opposite-sex faces. Proceedings of the Royal Society of London B, 278, 20322039.Google Scholar
Loker, E. S. (2012). Macroevolutionary immunology: A role for immunity in the diversification of animal life. Frontiers in Immunology, 3, 25.Google Scholar
Lopez, A. D., Mathers, C. D., Ezzati, M., Jamieson, D. T., & Murray, C. J. (2006). Global and regional burden of disease and risk factors, 2001: Systematic analysis of population health data. Lancet, 367, 17471757.Google Scholar
Low, B. S. (1990). Marriage systems and pathogen stress in human societies. American Zoologist, 30, 325339.Google Scholar
MacMurray, J., Comings, D. E., & Napolioni, V. (2014). The gene–immune–behavioral pathway: Gamma-interferon (1FN-γ) simultaneously coordinates susceptibility to infectious disease and harm avoidance behaviors. Brain, Behavior, and Immunity, 35, 169175.Google Scholar
McNeill, W. H. (1998). Plagues and Peoples. Harpswell, ME: Anchor.Google Scholar
Miller, E. N., Fadl, M., Mohamed, H. S., et al. (2007). Y chromosome lineage- and village-specific genes on chromosomes 1p22 and 6q27 control visceral leishmaniasis in Sudan. PLoS Genetics, 3, 679688.Google Scholar
Miller, S. L., & Maner, J. K. (2011). Sick body, vigilant mind: The biological immune system activates the behavioral immune system. Psychological Science, 22, 14671471.Google Scholar
Moore, F. R, Coetzee, V., Contreras-Garduño, J., et al. (2013). Cross-cultural variation in women’s preferences for cues to sex- and stress-hormones in the male face. Biology Letters, 9, 20130050.Google Scholar
Mortensen, C. R., Becker, D. V., Ackerman, J. M., Neuberg, S. L., & Kenrick, D. T. (2010). Infection breeds reticence: The effects of disease salience on self-perceptions of personality and behavioral avoidance tendencies. Psychological Science, 21, 440447.Google Scholar
Murray, D. R., & Schaller, M. (2010). Historical prevalence of infectious diseases within 230 geopolitical regions: A tool for investigating origins of culture. Journal of Cross-Cultural Psychology, 41, 99108.Google Scholar
Murray, D. R., & Schaller, M. (2012). Threat(s) and conformity deconstructed: Perceived threat of infectious disease and its implications for conformist attitudes and behavior. European Journal of Social Psychology, 42, 180188.Google Scholar
Murray, D. R., Trudeau, R., & Schaller, M. (2011). On the origins of cultural differences in conformity: Four tests of the pathogen prevalence hypothesis. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 37, 318329.Google Scholar
Murray, D. R., Schaller, M., & Suedfeld, P. (2013). Pathogens and politics: Further evidence that parasite prevalence predicts authoritarianism. PLoS ONE, 8, e62275.Google Scholar
Napolioni, V., Murray, D. R., Cominngs, D. E., et al. (2014). Interaction between infectious diseases and personality traits: ACP1* C as a potential mediator. Infection, Genetics and Evolution, 26, 267273.Google Scholar
Navarrete, C. D., & Fessler, D. M. T. (2006). Disease avoidance and ethnocentrism: The effects of disease vulnerability and disgust sensitivity on intergroup attitudes. Evolution and Human Behavior, 27, 270282.Google Scholar
Navarrete, C. D., Fessler, D. M. T., & Eng, S. J. (2007). Elevated ethnocentrism in the first trimester of pregnancy. Evolution and Human Behavior, 28, 6065.Google Scholar
Norenzayan, A., & Shariff, A. F. (2008). The origin and evolution of religious prosociality. Science, 322, 5862.Google Scholar
Oaten, M., Stevenson, R. J., & Case, T. I. (2009). Disgust as a disease-avoidance mechanism. Psychological Bulletin, 135, 303321.Google Scholar
Park, J. H., & Schaller, M. (2005). Does attitude similarity serve as a heuristic cue for kinship? Evidence of an implicit cognitive association. Evolution and Human Behavior, 26, 158170.Google Scholar
Pitchappan, R. M. (2002). Castes, migration, immunogenetics and infectious diseases in south India. Community Genetics, 5, 157161.Google Scholar
Prokop, P., & Fačovičová, J. (2011). Preferences for spicy foods and disgust of ectoparasites are associated with reported health in humans. Psihologija, 44, 281293.Google Scholar
Prugnolle, F., Manica, A., Charpentier, M., et al. (2005). Pathogen-driven selection and worldwide HLA class I diversity. Current Biology, 15, 10221027.Google Scholar
Reid, S. A., Zhang, J., Anderson, G. L., et al. (2012). Parasite primes make foreign-accented English sound more distant to people who are disgusted by pathogens (but not by sex or morality). Evolution and Human Behavior, 33, 471478.Google Scholar
Ridley, M. (1993). The Red Queen: Sex and the Evolution of Human Nature. New York: Macmillan Publishing Company.Google Scholar
Rougeron, V., De Meeus, T., Hide, M., et al. (2009). Extreme inbreeding in Leishmania braziliensis. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences, 106, 1022410229.Google Scholar
Ryan, S., Oaten, M., Stevenson, R. J., & Case, T. I. (2012). Facial disfigurement is treated like an infectious disease. Evolution and Human Behavior, 33, 639646.Google Scholar
Schaller, M. (2006). Parasites, behavioral defenses, and the social psychological mechanisms through which cultures are evoked. Psychological Inquiry, 17, 96101.Google Scholar
Schaller, M., & Duncan, L. (2007). The behavioral immune system: Its evolution and social psychological implications. In Forges, J. P., Haselton, M. G., & Von Hippel, W., eds., Evolution and the Social Mind: Evolutionary Psychology and Social Cognition. New York: Psychology Press, pp. 293307.Google Scholar
Schaller, M., & Murray, D. (2008). Pathogens, personality, and culture: Disease prevalence predicts worldwide variability in sociosexuality, extraversion, and openness to experience. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 95, 212221.Google Scholar
Schaller, M., & Neuberg, S. L. (2008). Intergroup prejudices and intergroup conflicts. In Crawford, C. & Krebs, D. L., eds., Foundations of Evolutionary Psychology. New York: Erlbaum, pp. 399412.Google Scholar
Schaller, M., Miller, G. E., Gervais, W. M., Yager, S., & Chen, E. (2010). Mere visual perception of other people’s disease symptoms facilitates a more aggressive immune response. Psychological Science, 21, 649652.Google Scholar
Schnall, S., Haidt, J., Clore, G.L., & Jordan, A. H. (2008). Disgust as embodied moral judgment. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 34, 10961109.Google Scholar
Sherman, P. W., & Billing, J. (1999). Darwinian gastronomy: Why we use spices. BioScience, 49, 453463.Google Scholar
Smith, K. F., Sax, D. F., Gaines, S. D., Guernier, V., & Guégan, J. F. (2007). Globalization of human infectious disease. Ecology, 88, 19031910.Google Scholar
Stevenson, R. J., Case, T. I., & Oaten, M. J. (2009). Frequency and recency of infection and their relationship with disgust and contamination sensitivity. Evolution and Human Behavior, 30, 363368.Google Scholar
Stevenson, R. J., Hodgson, D., Oaten, M. J., Barouei, J., & Case, T. I. (2011). The effect of disgust on oral immune function. Psychophysiology, 48, 900907.Google Scholar
Sugiyama, L. S. (2004). Illness, injury, and disability among Shiwiar forager–horticulturalists: Implications of human life history. American Journal of Physical Anthropology, 123, 371389.Google Scholar
Sugiyama, L. S., & Sugiyama, M. S. (2003). Social roles, prestige, and health risk: Social niche specialization as a risk-buffering strategy. Human Nature, 14, 165190.Google Scholar
Terrizzi, J. A. Jr., Shook, N. J., & Ventis, W. L. (2010). Disgust: A predictor of social conservatism and prejudicial attitudes toward homosexuals. Personality and Individual Differences, 49, 587592.Google Scholar
Terrizzi, J. A. Jr., Shook, N. J., & Ventis, W. L. (2012). Religious conservatism: an evolutionarily evoked disease-avoidance strategy. Religion, Brain and Behavior, 2, 105120.Google Scholar
Terrizzi, J. A. Jr., Shook, N. J., & McDaniel, M. A. (2013). The behavioral immune system and social conservatism: A meta-analysis. Evolution and Human Behavior, 34, 99108.Google Scholar
Thompson, J. N. (2005). The Geographic Mosaic of Coevolution. Chicago, IL: University of Chicago Press.Google Scholar
Thornhill, R., & Fincher, C. L. (2011). Parasite stress promotes homicide and child maltreatment. Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society: Biological Sciences, 366, 34663477.Google Scholar
Thornhill, R., & Fincher, C. L. (2013a). Commentary on Hackman, J., & Hruschka, D. (2013). Fast life histories, not pathogens, account for state-level variation in homicide, child maltreatment, and family ties in the U.S. Evolution and Human Behavior, 34, 314315.Google Scholar
Thornhill, R., & Fincher, C. L. (2013b). The parasite-driven-wedge model of parapatric speciation. Journal of Zoology, 291, 2333.Google Scholar
Thornhill, R., & Fincher, C. L. (2014). The Parasite-Stress Theory of Values and Sociality: Infectious Disease, History and Human Values Worldwide. New York: Springer.Google Scholar
Thornhill, R., & Gangestad, S. W. (2008). The Evolutionary Biology of Human Female Sexuality. New York: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Thornhill, R., Fincher, C. L., & Aran, D. (2009). Parasites, democratization, and the liberalization of values across contemporary countries. Biological Reviews, 84, 113131.Google Scholar
Thornhill, R., Fincher, C. L., Murray, D. R., & Schaller, M. (2010). Zoonotic and non-zoonotic diseases in relation to human personality and societal values: Support for the parasite-stress model. Evolutionary Psychology, 8, 151169.Google Scholar
Tinsley, M. C., Blanford, S., & Jiggins, F. M. (2006). Genetic variation in Drosophila melanogaster pathogen susceptibility. Parasitology, 132, 767773.Google Scholar
Van Valen, L. (1973). A new evolutionary law. Evolutionary Theory, 1, 130.Google Scholar
Volk, A. A., & Atkinson, J. A. (2013). Infant and child death in the human environment of evolutionary adaptation. Evolution and Human Behavior, 34, 182192.Google Scholar
Way, B. M., & Lieberman, M. D. (2010). Is there a genetic contribution to cultural differences? Collectivism, individualism and genetic markers of social sensitivity. Social Cognitive and Affective Neuroscience, 5, 203211.Google Scholar
Welling, L. L. M., Conway, C. A., DeBruine, L. M., & Jones, B. C. (2007). Perceived vulnerability to disease is positively related to the strength of preferences for apparent health in faces. Journal of Evolutionary Psychology, 5, 131139.Google Scholar
Wolfe, N. D., Dunavan, C. P., & Diamond, J. (2007). Origins of major human infectious diseases. Nature, 447, 279283.Google Scholar
Wu, B., & Chang, L. (2012). The social impact of pathogen threat: How disease salience influences conformity. Personality and Individual Differences, 53, 5054.Google Scholar
Young, S. G., Savvo, D. F., & Hugenberg, K. (2011). Vulnerability to disease is associated with a domain-specific preference for symmetrical faces relative to symmetrical non-face stimuli. European Journal of Social Psychology, 41, 558563.Google Scholar

References

Adams, D. B. (1981). Motivational systems of social behavior in male rats and monkeys: Are they homologous? Aggressive Behavior, 7, 518.Google Scholar
Adler, N. E., Epel, E. S., Castellazzo, G., & Ickovics, J. R. (2000). Relationship of subjective and objective social status with psychological and physiological functioning: Preliminary data in healthy, white women. Health Psychology, 19, 586592.Google Scholar
Al-Shawaf, L., Conroy-Beam, D., Asao, K., & Buss, D. M. (2015). Human emotions: An evolutionary psychological perspective. Emotion Review, 8, 173186.Google Scholar
Anderson, C., & Berdahl, J. L. (2002). The experience of power: Examining the effects of power on approach and inhibition tendencies. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 83, 13621377.Google Scholar
Ashton-James, C. E., & Tracy, J. L. (2012). Pride and prejudice: How feelings about the self influence judgments of others. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 38, 466476.Google Scholar
Babcock, M. K., & Sabini, J. (1990). On differentiating embarrassment from shame. European Journal of Social Psychology, 20, 151169.Google Scholar
Bales, R. F. (1950). Interaction Process Analysis: A Method for the Study of Small Groups. Reading, MA: Addison-Wesley.Google Scholar
Barkow, J. H. (1975). Prestige and culture: A biosocial interpretation. Current Anthropology, 16, 553572.Google Scholar
Barkow, J. H. (1989). Darwin, Sex, and Status. Toronto: University of Toronto Press.Google Scholar
Bartlett, M. Y., & DeSteno, D. (2006). Gratitude and prosocial behavior: Helping when it costs you. Psychological Science, 17, 319325.Google Scholar
Baumeister, R. F., & Leary, M. R. (1995). The need to belong: Desire for interpersonal attachments as a fundamental human motivation. Psychological Bulletin, 117, 497529.Google Scholar
Baumeister, R. F., Stillwell, A. M., & Heatherton, T. F. (1994). Guilt: An interpersonal approach. Psychological Bulletin, 115, 243267.Google Scholar
Beall, A. T., & Tracy, J. L. (2014). The puzzling attractiveness of male shame. Evolutionary Psychology, 13, 2947.Google Scholar
Beall, A. T., & Tracy, J. L. (2017). Emotivational psychology: How distinct emotions facilitate fundamental motives. Social and Personality Psychology Compass, 11, e12303.Google Scholar
Belsky, J., Domitrovich, C. E., & Crnic, K. (1997). Temperament and parenting antecedents of individual differences in 3-year-old pride and shame reactions. Child Development, 68, 456466.Google Scholar
Berger, J., Rosenholtz, S. J., & Zelditch, M. (1980). Status organizing processes. Annual Review of Sociology, 6, 479508.Google Scholar
Boehm, C. (1993). Egalitarian society and reverse dominance hierarchy. Current Anthropology, 34, 227254.Google Scholar
Boiger, M., Mesquita, B., Uchida, Y., & Barrett, L. F. (2013). Condoned or condemned the situational affordance of anger and shame in the United States and Japan. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 39, 540553.Google Scholar
Buss, D. M. (2008). Human nature and individual differences: Evolution of human personality. In John, O. P., Robins, R. W., & Pervin, L. A., eds., Handbook of Personality: Theory and Research, 3rd ed. New York: Guilford Press, pp. 2960.Google Scholar
Campos, J. J. (1995). Foreword. In Tangney, J. P. & Fischer, K. W., eds., Self-Conscious Emotions: The Psychology of Shame, Guilt, Embarrassment, and Pride. New York: Guilford Press, pp. ixxi.Google Scholar
Campos, J. J., Barrett, K. C., Lamb, M. E., Goldsmith, H. H., & Stenberg, C. (1983). Socioemotional development. In Haith, M. M. & Campos, J. J., eds., Handbook of Child Psychology (Vol. II): Infancy and Developmental Psychobiology. New York: Wiley, pp. 783915.Google Scholar
Carver, C. S., & Scheier, M. F. (1990). Origins and functions of positive and negative affect: A control-process view. Psychological Review, 97, 1935.Google Scholar
Carver, C. S., Sinclair, S., & Johnson, S. L. (2010). Authentic and hubristic pride: Differential relations to aspects of goal regulation, affect, and self-control. Journal of Research in Personality, 44, 698703.Google Scholar
Cheng, J. T., Tracy, J. L., & Henrich, J. (2010). Pride, personality, and the evolutionary foundations of human social status. Evolution and Human Behavior, 31, 334347.Google Scholar
Cheng, J. T., Tracy, J. L., Foulsham, T., Kingstone, A., & Henrich, J. (2013). Two ways to the top: Evidence that dominance and prestige are distinct yet viable avenues to social rank and influence. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 104, 103.Google Scholar
Cohen, D. (2003). The American national conversation about (everything but) shame. Social Research, 70, 10751108.Google Scholar
Cosmides, L., & Tooby, J. (2000). Evolutionary psychology and the emotions. Handbook of Emotions, 2, 91115.Google Scholar
Cowlishaw, G., & Dunbar, R. I. (1991). Dominance rank and mating success in male primates. Animal Behaviour, 41, 10451056.Google Scholar
Davidson, R. J. (2001). The neural circuitry of emotion and affective style: Prefrontal cortex and amygdala contributions. Social Science Information, 40, 1137.Google Scholar
de Hooge, I. E., Zeelenberg, M., & Breugelmans, S. M. (2007). Moral sentiments and cooperation: Differential influences of shame and guilt, Cognition and Emotion, 21, 10251042.Google Scholar
de Hooge, I. E., Zeelenberg, M., & Breugelmans, S. M. (2011). A functionalist account of shame-induced behavior. Cognition and Emotion, 25, 939946.Google Scholar
de Waal, F. (1989). Peacemaking among Primates. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press.Google Scholar
Deaner, R. O., Khera, A. V., & Platt, M. L. (2005). Monkeys pay per view: Adaptive valuation of social images by rhesus macaques. Current Biology, 15, 543548.Google Scholar
Dickerson, S. S., Kemeny, M. E., Aziz, N., Kim, K. H., & Fahey, J. L. (2004). Immunological effects of induced shame and guilt. Psychosomatic Medicine, 66, 124131.Google Scholar
Dickerson, S. S., Mycek, P. J., & Zaldivar, F. (2008). Negative social evaluation – but not mere social presence – elicits cortisol responses in the laboratory. Health Psychology, 27, 116121.Google Scholar
Dickerson, S. S., Gruenewald, T. L., & Kemeny, M. E. (2009). Psychobiological responses to social self threat: Functional or detrimental? Self and Identity, 8, 270285.Google Scholar
Eibl-Eisenfeldt, I. (1989). Human Ethology. New York: Aldine de Gruyter.Google Scholar
Ekman, P. (1992). An argument for basic emotions. Cognition and Emotion, 6, 169200.Google Scholar
Ekman, P., & Cordaro, D. (2011). What is meant by calling emotions basic? Emotion Review, 3, 364370.Google Scholar
Ekman, P., Sorenson, E. R., & Friesen, W. V. (1969). Pan-cultural elements in facial displays of emotion. Science, 164, 8688.Google Scholar
Ekman, P., Levenson, R. W., & Friesen, W. V. (1983). Autonomic nervous system activity distinguishes among emotions. Science, 221, 12081210.Google Scholar
Ekman, P., Friesen, W. V., O’Sullivan, M., et al. (1987). Universals and cultural differences in the judgment of facial expressions of emotion. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 53, 712717.Google Scholar
Elfenbein, H. A., & Ambady, N. (2002). On the universality and cultural specificity of emotion recognition: A meta-analysis. Psychological Bulletin, 128, 203235.Google Scholar
Ellis, L. (1995). Dominance and reproductive success among nonhuman animals: A cross-species comparison. Ethology and Sociobiology, 16, 257333.Google Scholar
Ellsworth, P. C., & Smith, C. A. (1988). Shades of joy: Patterns of appraisal differentiating pleasant emotions. Cognition and Emotion, 2, 301331.Google Scholar
Fessler, D. M. T. (2004). Shame in two cultures: Implications for evolutionary approaches. Journal of Cognition and Culture, 4, 207262.Google Scholar
Fessler, D. M. T. (2007). From appeasement to conformity: Evolutionary and cultural perspectives on shame, competition, and cooperation. In Tracy, J. L., Robins, R. W., & Tangney, J. P., eds., The Self-Conscious Emotions: Theory and Research. New York: Guilford Press, pp. 174193.Google Scholar
Fischer, J., Fischer, P., Englich, B., Aydin, N., & Frey, D. (2011). Empower my decisions: The effects of power gestures on confirmatory information processing. Journal of Experimental Social Psychology, 47, 11461154.Google Scholar
Fischer, K. W., & Tangney, J. P. (1995). Self-conscious emotions and the affect revolution: Framework and overview. In Tangney, J. P. & Fischer, K. W., eds., Self-Conscious Emotions: The Psychology of Shame, Guilt, Embarrassment, and Pride. New York: Guilford Press, pp. 324.Google Scholar
Fredrickson, B. L. (1998). What good are positive emotions? Review of General Psychology, 2, 300319.Google Scholar
Fried, M. H. (1967). The Evolution of Political Society: An Essay in Political Anthropology. New York: Random House.Google Scholar
Gangestad, S. W., & Simpson, J. A. (2000). The evolution of human mating: Trade-offs and strategic pluralism. Behavioral and brain sciences, 23, 573587.Google Scholar
Gilbert, P. (1997). The evolution of social attractiveness and its role in shame, humiliation, guilt and therapy. British Journal of Medical Psychology, 70, 113147.Google Scholar
Gilbert, P. (2007). The evolution of shame as a marker for relationship security: A biopsychosocial approach. In Tracy, J. L., Robins, R. W., & Tangney, J. P., eds., The Self-Conscious Emotions: Theory and Research. New York: Guilford Press, pp. 283309.Google Scholar
Giner-Sorolla, R., Castano, E., Espinosa, P., & Brown, R. (2008). Shame expressions reduce recipient’s insult from outgroup reparations. Journal of Experimental Social Psychology, 44, 519526.Google Scholar
Ginsburg, H. J. (1980). Playground as laboratory: Naturalistic studies of appeasement, altruism, and the omega child. In Omark, D. R., Strayer, F. F., & Freeman, D. G., eds., Dominance Relations: An Ethological View of Human Conflict and Social Interaction. New York: Garland, pp. 341357.Google Scholar
Graham, S., & Weiner, B. (1986). From an attributional theory of emotion to developmental psychology: A round-trip ticket? Social Cognition, 4, 152179.Google Scholar
Greenwald, A. G., McGhee, D. E., & Schwartz, J. L. K. (1998). Measuring individual differences in implicit cognition: The implicit association test. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 74, 14641480.Google Scholar
Gruber, J., & Johnson, S. L. (2009). Positive emotional traits and ambitious goals among people at risk for mania: The need for specificity. International Journal of Cognitive Therapy, 2, 179190.Google Scholar
Gruber, J., Culver, J. L., Johnson, S. L., et al. (2009). Do positive emotions predict symptomatic change in bipolar disorder? Bipolar Disorders, 11, 330336.Google Scholar
Gruber, J., Oveis, C., Keltner, D., & Johnson, S. L. (2011). A discrete emotions approach to positive emotion disturbance in depression. Cognition and Emotion, 25, 4052.Google Scholar
Haidt, J., & Keltner, D. (1999). Culture and facial expression: Open-ended methods find more expressions and a gradient of recognition. Cognition and Emotion, 13, 225266.Google Scholar
Harris, P. L., Olthof, T., Terwogt, M. M., & Hardman, C. E. (1987). Children’s knowledge of the situations that provoke emotion. International Journal of Behavioral Development, 10, 319343.Google Scholar
Hart, D., & Karmel, M. P. (1996). Self-awareness and self-knowledge in humans, apes, and monkeys. In Russon, A. E., Bard, K. A., & Parker, S. T., eds., Reaching into Thought: The Minds of the Great Apes. New York: Cambridge University Press, pp. 325347.Google Scholar
Hart, D., & Matsuba, M. K. (2007). The development of pride and moral life. In Tracy, J. L., Robins, R. W., & Tangney, J. P., eds., The Self-Conscious Emotions: Theory and Research. New York: Guilford Press, pp. 114133.Google Scholar
Harter, S. (1983). Developmental perspective on the self-system. In Hetherington, E. M., ed., Mussen, P. H., series ed., Handbook of Child Psychology: Vol 4. Socialization, Personality, and Social Development, 4th ed. New York: Wiley, pp. 275385.Google Scholar
Heaven, P. C. L., Ciarrochi, J., & Leeson, P. (2009). The longitudinal links between shame and increasing hostility during adolescence. Personality and Individual Differences, 47, 841844.Google Scholar
Heine, S. J., Lehman, D. R., Markus, H. R., & Kitayama, S. (1999). Is there a universal need for positive self-regard? Psychological Review, 106, 766794.Google Scholar
Henrich, J., & Boyd, R. (1998). The evolution of conformist transmission and between-group differences. Evolution and Human Behavior, 19, 215242.Google Scholar
Henrich, J., & Gil-White, F. J. (2001). The evolution of prestige: Freely conferred deference as a mechanism for enhancing the benefits of cultural transmission. Evolution and Human Behavior, 22, 165196.Google Scholar
Hill, J. (1984). Prestige and reproductive success in man. Ethology and Sociobiology, 5, 7795.Google Scholar
Hill, K., & Hurtado, A. M. (1989). Hunter–gatherers of the New World. American Scientist, 77, 436443.Google Scholar
Huang, L., Galinsky, A. D., Gruenfeld, D. H., & Guillory, L. E. (2010). Powerful postures versus powerful roles: Which is the proximate correlate of thought and behavior? Psychological Science, 22, 95102.Google Scholar
Izard, C. E. (1971). The Face of Emotion. East Norwalk, CT: Appleton-Century-Crofts.Google Scholar
Jacquet, J. (2011). Is shame necessary? In Brockman, M., ed., Future Science: Essays from the Cutting Edge. New York: Vintage Books, pp. 128140.Google Scholar
Jacquet, J., Hauert, C., Traulsen, A., & Milinski, M. (2011). Shame and honor drive cooperation. Biology Letters, 7, 899901.Google Scholar
Johnson-Laird, P. N., & Oatley, K. (1989). The language of emotions: Analysis of a semantic field. Cognition and Emotion, 3, 81123.Google Scholar
Keltner, D. (1995). Signs of appeasement: Evidence for the distinct displays of embarrassment, amusement, and shame. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 61, 441454.Google Scholar
Keltner, D., & Buswell, B. N. (1997). Embarrassment: Its distinct form and appeasement functions. Psychological Bulletin, 122, 250270.Google Scholar
Keltner, D., Young, R. C., & Buswell, B. N. (1997). Appeasement in human emotion, social practice, and personality. Aggressive Behavior, 23, 359374.Google Scholar
Keltner, D., Haidt, J., & Shiota, M. N. (2006). Social functionalism and the evolution of emotions. In Schaller, M., Simpson, J. A., & Kenrick, D. T., eds., Evolution and Social Psychology. Madison, CT: Psychosocial Press, pp. 115142.Google Scholar
Kitayama, S., Mesquita, B., & Karasawa, M. (2006). Cultural affordances and emotional experience: Socially engaging and disengaging emotions in Japan and the United States. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 91, 890903.Google Scholar
Kummer, H. (1968). Social Organization of Hamadryas Baboons. Chicago, IL: University of Chicago Press.Google Scholar
Lagattuta, K. H., & Thompson, R. A. (2007). The development of self-conscious emotions: Cognitive processes and social influences. In Tracy, J. L., Robins, R. W., & Tangney, J. P., eds., The Self-Conscious Emotions: Theory and Research. New York: Guilford Press, pp. 91113.Google Scholar
Lancaster, J. B. (1976). Primate Behavior and the Emergence of Human Culture. New York: Holt.Google Scholar
Larsen, R. J. (2000). Toward a science of mood regulation. Psychological Inquiry, 11, 129141.Google Scholar
Leary, M. R., Tambor, E. S., Terdal, S. K., & Downs, D. L. (1995). Self-esteem as an interpersonal monitor: The sociometer hypothesis. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 68, 518530.Google Scholar
LeDoux, J. E. (1996). The Emotional Brain: The Mysterious Underpinnings of Emotional Life. New York: Simon & Schuster.Google Scholar
Lee, R. B. (1979). The !Kung San: Men, Women and Work in a Foraging Society. Cambridge, UK: Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
Leith, K. P., & Baumeister, R. F. (1998). Empathy, shame, guilt, and narratives of interpersonal conflicts: Guilt-prone people are better at perspective taking. Journal of Personality, 66, 137.Google Scholar
Leresche, L. A. (1976). Dyadic play in Hamadryas baboons. Behaviour, 57, 190205.Google Scholar
Levenson, R. W. (2011). Basic emotion questions. Emotion Review, 3, 379386.Google Scholar
Lewis, M. (2000). Self-conscious emotions: Embarrassment, pride, shame, and guilt. In Lewis, M. & Haviland-Jones, J. M., eds., Handbook of Emotions, 2nd ed. New York: Guilford Press, pp. 623636.Google Scholar
Lewis, M., Alessandri, S. M., & Sullivan, M. W. (1992). Differences in shame and pride as a function of children’s gender and task difficulty. Child Development, 63, 630638.Google Scholar
Markus, H. R., & Kitayama, S. (1991). Culture and the self: Implications for cognition, emotion, and motivation. Psychological Review, 98, 224253.Google Scholar
Martens, J. P., & Tracy, J. L. (2013). The emotional origins of a social learning bias does the pride expression cue copying? Social Psychological and Personality Science, 4, 492499.Google Scholar
Martens, J. P., Tracy, J. L., Cheng, J. T., Parr, L. A., & Price, S. (2010). Do the chimpanzee bluff display and human pride expression share evolutionary origins? Poster presented at the 33rd Society for Personality and Social Psychology Pre-Conference on Evolutionary Psychology, Las Vegas, NV.Google Scholar
Martens, J. P., Tracy, J. L., & Shariff, A. F. (2012). Status signals: Adaptive benefits of displaying and observing the nonverbal expressions of pride and shame. Cognition and Emotion, 26, 390406.Google Scholar
Maslow, A. H. (1936). The role of dominance in the social and sexual behavior of infra-human primates: IV. The determination of hierarchy in pairs and in a group. Pedagogical Seminary and Journal of Genetic Psychology, 49, 161198.Google Scholar
Mazur, A. (1983). Hormones, aggression, and dominance in humans. In Svare, B. B, ed., Hormones and Aggressive Behavior. New York: Plenum Press, pp. 563576.Google Scholar
Mazur, A., & Lamb, T. A. (1980). Testosterone, status, and mood in human males. Hormones and Behavior, 14, 236246.Google Scholar
Mehta, P. H., & Josephs, R. A. (2010). Testosterone and cortisol jointly regulate dominance: Evidence for a dual-hormone hypothesis. Hormones and Behavior, 58, 898906.Google Scholar
Mesquita, B., & Boiger, M. (2014). Emotions in context: A sociodynamic model of emotions. Emotion Review, 6, 298302.Google Scholar
Mesquita, B., & Karasawa, M. (2004). Self-conscious emotions as dynamic cultural processes. Psychological Inquiry, 15, 161166.Google Scholar
Miller, P. J., Wang, S.-H., Sandel, T., & Cho, G. E. (2002). Self-esteem as folk theory: A comparison of European American and Taiwanese mothers’ beliefs. Parenting: Science and Practice, 2, 209239.Google Scholar
Morrison, D., & Gilbert, P. (2001). Social rank, shame and anger in primary and secondary psychopaths. Journal of Forensic Psychiatry, 12, 330356.Google Scholar
Nelson, N. L., & Russell, J. A. (2011). When dynamic, the head and face alone can express pride. Emotion, 11, 990993.Google Scholar
Nesse, R. M. (1991). What good is feeling bad? Sciences, 31, 3037.Google Scholar
Nesse, R. M., & Ellsworth, P. C. (2009). Evolution, emotions, and emotional disorders. American Psychologist, 64, 129139.Google Scholar
Newman, M. L., Sellers, J. G., & Josephs, R. A. (2005). Testosterone, cognition, and social status. Hormones and Behavior, 47, 205211.Google Scholar
Nussbaum, M. C. (2006). Hiding from Humanity: Disgust, Shame, and the Law. Princeton, NJ: Princeton University Press.Google Scholar
Orth, U., Robins, R. W., & Soto, C. J. (2008). Tracking the trajectory of shame, guilt, and pride across the lifespan. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 99, 10611071.Google Scholar
Panksepp, J. (1998). Affective Neuroscience: The Foundations of Human and Animal Emotions. New York: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Panksepp, J., & Watt, D. (2011). What is basic about basic emotions? Lasting lessons from affective neuroscience. Emotion Review, 3, 387396.Google Scholar
Proeve, M. J., & Howells, K. (2006). Effects of remorse and shame and criminal justice experience on judgements about a sex offender. Psychology, Crime and Law, 12, 145161.Google Scholar
Randles, D., & Tracy, J. L. (2013). Nonverbal displays of shame predict relapse and declining health in recovering alcoholics. Clinical Psychological Science, 1, 149155.Google Scholar
Randles, D., & Tracy, J. L. (2015). Shame. In Segal, R. & Stuckrad, K., eds., Vocabulary for the Study of Religion. Leiden: Brill, pp. 339343.Google Scholar
Retzinger, S. M. (1987). Resentment and laughter: Video studies of the shame–rage spiral. In Lewis, H. B., ed., The Role of Shame in Symptom Formation. Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates, pp. 151181.Google Scholar
Roberts, B. W., Wood, D., & Caspi, A. (2008). The development of personality traits in adulthood. In John, O. P., Robins, R. W., & Pervin, L. A., eds., Handbook of Personality: Theory and Research. New York: Guilford Press, pp. 375398.Google Scholar
Robinson, R., Roberts, W. L., Strayer, J., & Koopman, R. (2007). Empathy and emotional responsiveness in delinquent and non-delinquent adolescents. Social Development, 16, 555579.Google Scholar
Rose, R. M., Gordon, T. P., & Bernstein, T. S. (1972). Plasma testosterone levels in the male rhesus: Influences of sexual and social stimuli. Science, 178, 643645.Google Scholar
Rosenberg, E. L., & Ekman, P. (1994). Coherence between expressive and experiential systems in emotion. Cognition and Emotion, 8, 201229.Google Scholar
Sauter, D. A., & Scott, S. K. (2007). More than one kind of happiness: Can we recognize vocal expressions of different positive states? Motivation and Emotion, 31, 192199.Google Scholar
Schaller, G. E. (1963). The Mountain Gorilla: Ecology and Behavior. Chicago, IL: University of Chicago Press.Google Scholar
Shariff, A. F., & Tracy, J. L. (2009). Knowing who’s boss: Implicit perceptions of status from the nonverbal expression of pride. Emotion, 9, 631639.Google Scholar
Shariff, A. F., & Tracy, J. L. (2011). What are emotion expressions for? Current Directions in Psychological Science, 20, 395399.Google Scholar
Shariff, A. F., Tracy, J. L., Cheng, J. T., & Henrich, J. (2010). Further thoughts on the evolution of pride’s two facets: A response to Clark. Emotion Review, 2, 399400.Google Scholar
Shariff, A. F., Tracy, J. L., & Markusoff, J. L. (2012). (Implicitly) judging a book by its cover: The power of pride and shame expressions in shaping judgments of social status. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 38, 11781193.Google Scholar
Shaver, P., Schwartz, J., Kirson, D., & O’Connor, C. (1987). Emotion knowledge: Further exploration of a prototype approach. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 52, 10611086.Google Scholar
Silk, J. B. (1987). Correlates of agonistic and competitive interactions in pregnant baboons. American Journal of Primatology, 12, 479495.Google Scholar
Simon, H. A. (1967). Motivational and emotional controls of cognition. Psychological Review, 74, 2939.Google Scholar
Simon-Thomas, E. R., Keltner, D. J., Sauter, D., Sinicropi-Yao, L., & Abramson, A. (2009). The voice conveys specific emotions: Evidence from vocal burst displays. Emotion, 9, 838846.Google Scholar
Stipek, D. (1995). The development of pride and shame in toddlers. In Tangney, J. P. & Fischer, K. W., eds., Self-Conscious Emotions: The Psychology of Shame, Guilt, Embarrassment, and Pride. New York: Guilford Press, pp. 237254.Google Scholar
Stipek, D. (1998). Differences between Americans and Chinese in the circumstances evoking pride, shame and guilt. Journal of Cross-Culture Psychology, 29, 616629.Google Scholar
Stipek, D., Recchia, S., & McClintic, S. (1992). Self-evaluation in young children. Monographs of the Society for Research in Child Development, 57, 100.Google Scholar
Strayer, F. F., & Strayer, J. (1976). An ethological analysis of social agonism and dominance relations among preschool children. Child Development, 47, 980989.Google Scholar
Susskind, J. M., & Anderson, A. K. (2008). Facial expression form and function. Communicative and Integrative Biology, 1, 148149.Google Scholar
Takahashi, H., Matsuura, M., Koeda, M., et al. (2008). Brain activations during judgments of positive self-conscious emotion and positive basic emotion: Pride and joy. Cerebral Cortex, 18, 898903.Google Scholar
Takahashi, H., Yahata, N., Koeda, M., et al. (2004). Brain activation associated with evaluative processes of guilt and embarrassment: An fMRI study. NeuroImage, 23, 967974.Google Scholar
Tangney, J. P. (1990). Assessing individual differences in proneness to shame and guilt: Development of the Self-Conscious Affect and Attribution Inventory. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 59, 102111.Google Scholar
Tangney, J. P., & Dearing, R. (2002). Shame and Guilt in Interpersonal Relationships. New York: Guilford Press.Google Scholar
Tangney, J. P., & Fisher, K. W., eds. (1995). Self-Conscious Emotions: The Psychology of Shame, Guilt, Embarrassment, and Pride. New York: Guilford Press.Google Scholar
Tangney, J. P., Stuewig, J., & Mashek, D. J. (2007). Moral emotions and moral behavior. Annual Review of Psychology, 58, 345372.Google Scholar
Tangney, J. P., & Tracy, J. L. (2012). Self-conscious emotions. In Leary, M. & Tangney, J. P., eds., Handbook of Self and Identity, 2nd ed. New York: Guilford Press, pp. 446478.Google Scholar
Tangney, J. P., Wagner, P., & Gramzow, R. (1989). The Test of Self-Conscious Affect. Fairfax, VA: George Mason University.Google Scholar
Tangney, J. P., Wagner, P. E., Hill-Barlow, D., Marschall, D. E., & Gramzow, R. (1996). Relation of shame and guilt to constructive versus destructive responses to anger across the lifespan. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 70, 797809.Google Scholar
Thompson, R. A. (1989). Causal attributions and children’s emotional understanding. In Saarni, C. & Harris, P. L., eds., Children’s Understanding of Emotion. New York: Cambridge University Press, pp. 117150.Google Scholar
Tiedens, L., Ellsworth, P. C., & Mesquita, B. (2000). Sentimental stereotypes: Emotional expectations for high- and low-status group members. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 26, 560575.Google Scholar
Tomkins, S. S. (1962). Affect Imagery Consciousness: The Positive Affects, Vol. 1. New York: Springer.Google Scholar
Tracy, J. L. (2016). Take Pride: Why the Deadliest Sin Holds the Secret to Human Success. Boston, MA: Houghton Mifflin Harcourt.Google Scholar
Tracy, J. L., & Beall, A. (2011). Happy guys finish last: The impact of emotional expressions on sexual attraction. Emotion, 11, 13791387.Google Scholar
Tracy, J. L., Cheng, J. T., Robins, R. W., & Trzesniewski, K. H. (2009). Authentic and hubristic pride: The affective core of self-esteem and narcissism. Self and Identity, 8, 196213.Google Scholar
Tracy, J. L., & Matsumoto, D. (2008). The spontaneous display of pride and shame: Evidence for biologically innate nonverbal displays. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences, 105, 1165511660.Google Scholar
Tracy, J. L., & Prehn, C. (2012). The use of contextual knowledge to differentiate hubristic and authentic pride from a single non-verbal expression. Cognition and Emotion, 26, 1424.Google Scholar
Tracy, J. L., & Randles, D. (2011). Four models of basic emotions: A review of Ekman and Cordaro, Izard, Levenson, and Paksepp and Watt. Emotion Review, 3, 397405.Google Scholar
Tracy, J. L., & Robins, R. W. (2004a). Putting the self into self-conscious emotions: A theoretical model. Psychological Inquiry, 15, 103125.Google Scholar
Tracy, J. L., & Robins, R. W. (2004b). Show your pride: Evidence for a discrete emotion expression. Psychological Science, 15, 194197.Google Scholar
Tracy, J. L., & Robins, R. W. (2007a). The psychological structure of pride: A tale of two facets. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 92, 506525.Google Scholar
Tracy, J. L., & Robins, R. W. (2007b). Emerging insights into the nature and function of pride. Current Directions in Psychological Science, 16, 147150.Google Scholar
Tracy, J. L., & Robins, R. W. (2007c). The prototypical pride expression: Development of a nonverbal behavioral coding system. Emotion, 7, 789801.Google Scholar
Tracy, J. L., & Robins, R. W. (2008a). The nonverbal expression of pride: Evidence for crosscultural recognition. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 94, 516530.Google Scholar
Tracy, J. L., & Robins, R. W. (2008b). The automaticity of emotion recognition. Emotion, 7, 789801.Google Scholar
Tracy, J. L., Robins, R. W., & Lagattuta, K. H. (2005). Can children recognize the pride expression? Emotion, 5, 251257.Google Scholar
Tracy, J. L., Robins, R. W., & Schriber, R. A. (2009). Development of a FACS-verified set of basic and self-conscious emotion expressions. Emotion, 9, 554559.Google Scholar
Tracy, J. L., Shariff, A. F., & Cheng, J. T. (2010). A naturalist’s view of pride. Emotion Review, 2, 163177.Google Scholar
Tracy, J. L., Shariff, A. F., Zhao, W., & Henrich, J. (2013). Cross-cultural evidence that the pride expression is a universal automatic status signal. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 142, 163180.Google Scholar
van Hooff, J. A. R. A. M. (1973). A structural analysis of the social behaviour of a semi-captive group of chimpanzees. In von Cranach, M. & Vine, I., eds., Social Communication and Movement: Studies of Interaction and Expression in Man and Chimpanzee. New York: Academic Press, pp. 75162.Google Scholar
Van Vugt, M., Hogan, R., & Kaiser, R. (2008). Leadership, followership, and evolution: Some lessons from the past. American Psychologist, 63, 182196.Google Scholar
Verbeke, W., Belschak, F., & Bagozzi, R. P. (2004). The adaptive consequences of pride in personal selling. Journal of the Academy of Marketing Science, 32, 386402.Google Scholar
Watson, D., Clark, L. A., & Tellegen, A. (1988). Development and validation of brief measures of positive and negative affect: The PANAS Scales. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 54, 10631070.Google Scholar
Weidman, A. C., Cheng, J. T., & Tracy, J. L. (2018). The psychological structure of humility. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 114, 153178.Google Scholar
Weidman, A. C., Tracy, J. L., & Elliot, A. J. (2016). The benefits of following your pride: Authentic pride promotes achievement. Journal of Personality, 84, 607622.Google Scholar
Weiner, B. (1985). An attributional theory of achievement motivation and emotion. Psychological Review, 92, 548573.Google Scholar
Weisfeld, G. E., & Beresford, J. M. (1982). Erectness of posture as an indicator of dominance or success in humans. Motivation and Emotion, 6, 113131.Google Scholar
Williams, L. A., & DeSteno, D. (2008). Pride and perseverance: The motivational role of pride. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 94, 10071017.Google Scholar
Williams, L. A., & DeSteno, D. (2009). Pride: Adaptive social emotion or seventh sin? Psychological Science, 20, 284288.Google Scholar
Yang, M. L., Yang, C. C., & Chiou, W. B. (2010). When guilt leads to other orientation and shame leads to egocentric self-focus: effects of differential priming of negative affects on perspective taking. Social Behavior and Personality, 38, 605614.Google Scholar
Zahavi, A., & Zahavi, A. (1997). The Handicap Principle. New York: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar

References

Adams, F., & Aizawa, K. (2001). The bounds of cognition. Philosophical Psychology, 14(1), 4364.Google Scholar
Adams, F., & Aizawa, K. (2011). The Bounds of Cognition. New York: John Wiley & Sons.Google Scholar
Barkow, J. H. (2006). Introduction: Sometimes the bus does wait. In Barkow, J. H., ed., Missing the Revolution: Darwinism for Social Scientists. Oxford: Oxford University Press, pp. 360.Google Scholar
Barrett, H. C. (2015). The Shape of Thought: How Mental Adaptations Evolve. New York: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Barrett, H. C., & Kurzban, R. (2006). Modularity in cognition: Framing the debate. Psychological Review, 113, 628647.Google Scholar
Barrett, L. (2011). Beyond the Brain: How Body and Environment Shape Animal and Human Minds. Princeton, NJ: Princeton University Press.Google Scholar
Barrett, L. (2012). Why behaviorism isn’t satanism. In Shackelford, T. & Vonk, J., eds., The Oxford Handbook of Comparative Evolutionary Psychology. New York: Oxford University Press, pp. 1738.Google Scholar
Barrett, L., Pollet, T. V., & Stulp, G. (2014). From computers to cultivation: Reconceptualizing evolutionary psychology. Frontiers in Psychology, 5, 867.Google Scholar
Bateson, G. (1960). Minimal requirements for a theory of schizophrenia. AMA Archives of General Psychiatry, 2(5), 477491.Google Scholar
Bennett, M. R., & Hacker, P. M. S. (2003). Philosophical Foundations of Neuroscience. Oxford: Blackwell Publishing.Google Scholar
Brooks, R. A. (1999). Cambrian Intelligence: The Early History of the New AI. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.Google Scholar
Burke, D. (2014). Why isn’t everyone an evolutionary psychologist? Frontiers in Psychology, 5, 910.Google Scholar
Chemero, A. (2009). Radical Embodied Cognitive Science. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.Google Scholar
Clark, A. (1997). Being There: Putting Brain, Body, and World Together Again. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.Google Scholar
Clark, A. (2001). Reasons, robots and the extended mind. Mind & Language, 16(2), 121145.Google Scholar
Clark, A. (2003). Natural-Born Cyborgs: Minds, Technologies, and the Future of Human Intelligence. Oxford: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Clark, A. (2007). Curing cognitive hiccups: A defense of the extended mind. Journal of Philosophy, 104(4), 163192.Google Scholar
Clark, A. (2008). Supersizing the Mind: Embodiment, Action, and Cognitive Extension. New York: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Clark, A. (2014). Mindware: An Introduction to the Philosophy of Cognitive Science. Oxford: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Clark, A., & Chalmers, D. (1998). The extended mind. Analysis, 58(1), 719.Google Scholar
Cosmides, L. (2016). Evolutionary psychology and the generation of culture. Invited presentation. Big Questions in Evolutionary Science and what they mean for Social-Personality Psychology. Presented at 17th Annual Convention of the Society for Personality and Social Psychology, San Diego, January 28–30.Google Scholar
Cosmides, L., & Tooby, J. (1997). Evolutionary Psychology: A Primer. www.cep.ucsb.edu/primer.html.Google Scholar
Cottingham, J., Stoothoff, R., & Murdoch, D., eds. (1985). The Philosophical Writings of Descartes. Vol. 1. Cambridge, UK: Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
Daston, L., & Galison, P. (2007). Objectivity. New York: Zone Books.Google Scholar
Dewey, J. (1916). Essays in Experimental Logic. Chicago, IL: University of Chicago Press.Google Scholar
Dewey, J. (1981). The Later Works: 1925–1953. Chicago, IL: Southern Illinois University Press.Google Scholar
Fisher, S. E. (2006). Tangled webs: Tracing the connections between genes and cognition. Cognition, 101(2), 270297.Google Scholar
Fodor, J. A. (2008). LOT 2: The Language of Thought Revisited. Oxford: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Geertz, C. (1973). The Interpretation of Cultures: Selected Essays. New York: Basic books.Google Scholar
Gintis, H. (2016). Individuality and Entanglement: The Moral and Material Bases of Social Life. Princeton, NJ: Princeton University Press.Google Scholar
Henrich, J. (2016). The Secret of Our Success: How Culture Is Driving Human Evolution, Domesticating Our Species, and Making Us Smarter. Princeton, NJ: Princeton University Press.Google Scholar
Hurley, S. (2010). Varieties of externalism. In Menary, R. A., ed., The Extended Mind. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press, pp. 101154.Google Scholar
Hutchins, E. (1995). Cognition in the Wild. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.Google Scholar
Hutchins, E. (2008). The role of cultural practices in the emergence of modern human intelligence. Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences, 363(1499), 20112019.Google Scholar
Hutchins, E. (2010). Cognitive ecology. Topics in Cognitive Science, 2(4), 705715.Google Scholar
Hutchins, E. (2011). Enculturating the supersized mind. Philosophical Studies, 152(3), 437446.Google Scholar
Hutto, D. D. (2013). Psychology unified: From folk psychology to radical enactivism. Review of General Psychology, 17(2), 174184.Google Scholar
Hutto, D. D., & Myin, E. (2013). Radicalizing Enactivism: Basic Minds without Content. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.Google Scholar
Jonason, P. K., & Dane, L. K. (2014). How beliefs get in the way of the acceptance of evolutionary psychology. Frontiers in Psychology, 5, 1212.Google Scholar
Jonason, P. K., Jones, A., & Lyons, M. (2013). Creatures of the night: Chronotypes and the Dark Triad traits. Personality and Individual Differences, 55(5), 538541.Google Scholar
Landy, D., & Goldstone, R. L. (2007a). How abstract is symbolic thought? Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 33(4), 720733.Google Scholar
Landy, D., & Goldstone, R. L. (2007b). The alignment of ordering and space in arithmetic computation. Proceedings of the Cognitive Science Society, 29, 437442.Google Scholar
Landy, D., & Goldstone, R. L. (2010). Proximity and precedence in arithmetic. Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 63, 19531968.Google Scholar
Latour, B. (2005). Reassembling the Social: An Introduction to Actor-Network-Theory. Oxford: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Lock, M. (2013). The Alzheimer Conundrum: Entanglements of Dementia and Aging. Princeton, NJ: Princeton University Press.Google Scholar
Marks, J. M. (2009). Why I Am Not a Scientist: Anthropology and Modern Knowledge. Oakland, CA: University of California Press.Google Scholar
Menary, R. A. (2007). Cognitive Integration: Mind and Cognition Unbounded. London: Palgrave Macmillan.Google Scholar
Menary, R. A. (2010). Cognitive integration and the extended mind. In Menary, R. A., ed., The Extended Mind. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press, pp. 227243.Google Scholar
Mesoudi, A. (2011). Cultural Evolution: How Darwinian Theory Can Explain Human Culture and Synthesize the Social Sciences. Chicago, IL: University of Chicago Press.Google Scholar
Nicholson, J. K., Holmes, E., Kinross, J., et al. (2012). Host–gut microbiota metabolic interactions. Science, 336(6086), 12621267.Google Scholar
Park, J. H. (2007). Persistent misunderstandings of inclusive fitness and kin selection: Their ubiquitous appearance in social psychology textbooks. Evolutionary Psychology, 5(4), 860873.Google Scholar
Payne, S. J., Howes, A., & Reader, W. R. (2001). Adaptively distributing cognition: A decision-making perspective on human–computer interaction. Behaviour & Information Technology, 20(5), 339346.Google Scholar
Pickering, A. (2010). The Mangle of Practice: Time, Agency, and Science. Chicago, IL: University of Chicago Press.Google Scholar
Richerson, P. J., & Boyd, R. (2005). Not by Genes Alone: How Culture Transformed Human Evolution. Chicago, IL: University of Chicago Press.Google Scholar
Ross, A., ed. (1996). Science Wars. Durham, NC: Duke University Press.Google Scholar
Rowlands, M. (1999). The Body in Mind: Understanding Cognitive Processes. Cambridge, UK: Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
Rowlands, M. (2010). The New Science of the Mind: From Extended Mind to Embodied Phenomenology. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.Google Scholar
Rupert, R. D. (2009). Cognitive Systems and the Extended Mind. Oxford: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Ryle, G. (1949). The Concept of Mind. London: Hutchinson.Google Scholar
Shapin, S. (2010). Never Pure: Historical Studies of Science as if It Was Produced by People with Bodies, Situated in Time, Space, Culture, and Society, and Struggling for Credibility and Authority. Baltimore, MD: Johns Hopkins University Press.Google Scholar
Shapin, S., & Schaffer, S. (1985). Leviathan and the Air-Pump: Hobbes, Boyle, and the Experimental Life. Princeton, NJ: Princeton University Press.Google Scholar
Shapiro, L. (2010). James Bond and the barking dog: Evolution and extended cognition. Philosophy of Science, 77(3), 400418.Google Scholar
Smith, B. D., & Zeder, M. A. (2013). The onset of the Anthropocene. Anthropocene, 4, 813.Google Scholar
Sperber, D. (1994). The modularity of thought and the epidemiology of representations. In Hirschfeld, L. A. & Gelman, S. A., eds., Mapping the Mind: Domain Specificity in Cognition and Culture, Cambridge, UK: Cambridge University Press, pp. 3967.Google Scholar
Sperber, D. (1996). Explaining Culture. Oxford: Blackwell Publishers.Google Scholar
Sprevak, M. (2010). Inference to the hypothesis of extended cognition. Studies in History and Philosophy of Science Part A, 41(4), 353362.Google Scholar
Steffen, W., Grinevald, J., Crutzen, P., & McNeill, J. (2011). The Anthropocene: Conceptual and historical perspectives. Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society of London A: Mathematical, Physical and Engineering Sciences, 369(1938), 842867.Google Scholar
Sterelny, K. (2010). Minds: Extended or scaffolded? Phenomenology and the Cognitive Sciences, 9(4), 465481.Google Scholar
Sussman, R. W. (2014). The Myth of Race: The Troubling Persistence of an Unscientific Idea. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press.Google Scholar
Sutton, J. (2006). Distributed cognition: Domains and dimensions. Pragmatics & Cognition, 14(2), 235247.Google Scholar
Sutton, J. (2010). Exograms and interdisciplinarity: History, the extended mind, and the civilizing process. In Menary, R. A., ed., The Extended Mind. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press, pp. 189225.Google Scholar
Sutton, J. (2015). Remembering as public practice: Wittgenstein, memory, and distributed cognitive ecologies. In Moyal-Sharrock, D., Munz, V., & Coliva, A., eds., Mind, Language, and Action: Proceedings of the 36th Wittgenstein Symposium. Berlin: De Gruyter, pp. 409443.Google Scholar
Thompson, E. (2007). Mind in Life: Biology, Phenomenology, and the Sciences of Mind. Harvard, MA: Harvard University Press.Google Scholar
Tooby, J., & Cosmides, L. (1992). The psychological foundations of culture. In Barkow, J., Cosmides, L., & Tooby, J., eds., The Adapted Mind: Evolutionary Psychology and the Generation of Culture. Oxford: Oxford University Press, pp. 19136.Google Scholar
Tooby, J., & Cosmides, L. (2005). Evolutionary psychology: Conceptual foundations. In Buss, D., ed., The Handbook of Evolutionary Psychology. New York: John Wiley & Sons, pp. 567.Google Scholar
Tribble, E. (2011). Cognition in the Globe: Attention and Memory in Shakespeare’s Theatre. New York: Springer.Google Scholar
Vaesen, K. (2014). Dewey on extended cognition and epistemology. Philosophical Issues, 24(1), 426438.Google Scholar
van Dijk, L. (2016). A horizontal attitude: Gibsonian psychology and an ontology of doing. Unpublished PhD thesis, University of Antwerp.Google Scholar
Varela, F. J., Thompson, E., & Rosch, E. (1992). The Embodied Mind: Cognitive Science and Human Experience. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.Google Scholar
West, S. A., El Mouden, C., & Gardner, A. (2011). Sixteen common misconceptions about the evolution of cooperation in humans. Evolution and Human Behavior, 32(4), 231262.Google Scholar
Wheeler, M., & Clark, A. (2008). Culture, embodiment and genes: Unravelling the triple helix. Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society of London B: Biological Sciences, 363(1509), 35633575.Google Scholar
Wrangham, R. (2009). Catching Fire: How Cooking Made Us Human. New York: Basic Books.Google Scholar

Save book to Kindle

To save this book to your Kindle, first ensure coreplatform@cambridge.org is added to your Approved Personal Document E-mail List under your Personal Document Settings on the Manage Your Content and Devices page of your Amazon account. Then enter the ‘name’ part of your Kindle email address below. Find out more about saving to your Kindle.

Note you can select to save to either the @free.kindle.com or @kindle.com variations. ‘@free.kindle.com’ emails are free but can only be saved to your device when it is connected to wi-fi. ‘@kindle.com’ emails can be delivered even when you are not connected to wi-fi, but note that service fees apply.

Find out more about the Kindle Personal Document Service.

  • Group Living
  • Edited by Lance Workman, University of South Wales, Will Reader, Sheffield Hallam University, Jerome H. Barkow, Dalhousie University, Nova Scotia
  • Book: The Cambridge Handbook of Evolutionary Perspectives on Human Behavior
  • Online publication: 02 March 2020
Available formats
×

Save book to Dropbox

To save content items to your account, please confirm that you agree to abide by our usage policies. If this is the first time you use this feature, you will be asked to authorise Cambridge Core to connect with your account. Find out more about saving content to Dropbox.

  • Group Living
  • Edited by Lance Workman, University of South Wales, Will Reader, Sheffield Hallam University, Jerome H. Barkow, Dalhousie University, Nova Scotia
  • Book: The Cambridge Handbook of Evolutionary Perspectives on Human Behavior
  • Online publication: 02 March 2020
Available formats
×

Save book to Google Drive

To save content items to your account, please confirm that you agree to abide by our usage policies. If this is the first time you use this feature, you will be asked to authorise Cambridge Core to connect with your account. Find out more about saving content to Google Drive.

  • Group Living
  • Edited by Lance Workman, University of South Wales, Will Reader, Sheffield Hallam University, Jerome H. Barkow, Dalhousie University, Nova Scotia
  • Book: The Cambridge Handbook of Evolutionary Perspectives on Human Behavior
  • Online publication: 02 March 2020
Available formats
×