Skip to main content Accessibility help
×
Home
  • Cited by 2
  • Print publication year: 2015
  • Online publication date: July 2015

7 - Perception and Attention

from Part II - Attention and Perceptual Processes

References

Allport, D. A. (1987). Selection-for-action: Some behavioral and neurophysiological considerations of attention and action. In H. Heuer and A. F. Sanders (Eds.), Perspectives on perception and action (pp. 395–419). Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum.
Ashby, F. G. (Ed.) (1992). Multidimensional models of perception and cognition. Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum.
Ashby, F. G. and Perrin, N. A. (1988). Toward a unified theory of similarity and recognition. Psychological Review, 95, 124–150.
Audley, R. J. and Pike, A. R. (1965). Some alternative stochastic models of choice. British Journal of Mathematical and Statistical Psychology, 18, 207–225.
Bekkering, H. and Neggers, S. F. W. (2002). Visual search modulated by action intentions. Psychological Sciences, 13, 370–374.
Broadbent, D. E. (1958). Perception and communication. New York: Pergamon Press.
Busemeyer, J. R. and Diederich, A. (2010). Cognitive modeling. Thousand Oaks, CA: Sage.
Cowan, N. (1995). Attention and memory. New York: Oxford University Press.
Davies, D. R. and Parasuraman, R. (1982). The psychology of vigilance. London: Academic Press.
De Soete, G. and Carroll, J. D. (1983). A maximum likelihood method for fitting the wandering vector model. Psychometrika, 48, 553–566.
De Soete, G. and Carroll, J. D. (1992). Probabilistic multidimensional models of pairwise choice data. In F. G. Ashby (Ed.), Multidimensional models of perception and cognition (pp. 61–88). Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum.
Eckstein, M. P., Thomas, J. P., Palmer, J. and Shimozaki, S. S. (2000). A signal detection model predicts the effects of set size on visual search accuracy for feature, conjunction, triple conjunction, and disjunction displays. Perception and Psychophysics, 62, 425–451.
Engle, R. W. (2002). Working memory capacity as executive attention. Current Directions in Psychological Science, 11, 19–23.
Fagioli, S., Hommel, B. and Schubotz, R. I. (2007). Intentional control of attention: Action planning primes action-related stimulus dimensions. Psychological Research, 71, 22–29.
Fechner, G. T. (1966). Elements of psychophysics, Vol. 1 (H. E. Adler, Trans.). New York: Holt, Rinehart & Winston. (Original work published in 1860).
Fitts, P. M. and Seeger, C. M. (1953). S-R compatibility: Spatial characteristics of stimulus and response codes. Journal of Experimental Psychology, 46, 199–210.
Funke, M. E., Warm, J., Matthews, G., Finomore, V., Jr., Vidulich, M. A., Knott, B. A., Helton, W. S., Shaw, T. H. and Parasuraman, R. (2010). Static and dynamic discriminations in vigilance: Effects on cerebral hemodynamics and workload. In T. Marke, W. Karwoski and V. Rice (Eds.), Advances in understanding human performance: Neuroergonomics, human factors design, and special populations (pp. 80–90). Boca Raton, FL: CRC Press.
Green, D. M. and Swets, J. A. (1966). Signal detection theory and psychophysics. New York: John Wiley.
Johnson, A. and Proctor, R. W. (2004). Attention: Theory and practice. Thousand Oaks, CA: Sage.
Lewandowsky, S. and Farrell, S. (2010). Computational modeling in cognition: Principles and practice. Thousand Oaks, CA: Sage.
Lu, Z.-L., Liu, C. Q. and Dosher, B. A. (2000). Attention mechanisms for multi-location first- and second-order motion perception. Vision Research, 40, 173–186.
Macmillan, N. A. and Creelman, C. D. (2005). Detection theory: A user’s guide (2nd ed.). Mahwah, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum.
Maddox, W. T. and Ashby, F. G. (1996). Perceptual separability, decisional separability, and the identification-speeded classification relationship. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 22, 795–817.
Manly, T., Robertson, I. H., Galloway, M. and Hawkins, K. (1999). The absent mind: Further investigations of sustained attention to response. Neuropsychologia, 37, 661–670.
McVay, J. C. and Kane, M. J. (2010). Does mind wandering reflect executive function or executive failure? Comment on Smallwood and Schooler (2006) and Watkins (2008). Psychological Bulletin, 136, 188–197.
Murdock, B. B. (1985). An analysis of the strength-latency relationship. Memory and Cognition, 13, 511–521.
Navon, D. and Gopher, D. (1979). On the economy of the human-processing system. Psychological Review, 86, 214–255.
Neumann, O. (1987). Beyond capacity: A functional view of attention. In H. Heuer and A. F. Sanders (Eds.), Perspectives on perception and action (pp. 361–394). Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum.
Nosofsky, R. M. (1986). Attention, similarity, and the identification-categorization relationship. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 115, 39–57.
Parasuraman, R. and Davies, D. R. (1976). Decision theory analysis of response latencies in vigilance. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 2, 578–590.
Proctor, R. W. and Vu K.-P. L. (2006). Stimulus-response compatibility principles: Data, theory and application. Boca Raton, FL: CRC Press.
Shaw, T. H., Funke, M. E., Dillard, M., Funke, G. J., Warm, J. S. and Parasuraman, R. (2013). Event-related cerebral hemodynamics reveal target-specific resource allocation for both “go” and “no-go” response-based vigilance tasks. Brain and Cognition, 82, 265–273.
Shepard, R. N. (1957). Stimulus and response generalization: A stochastic model relating generalization to distance in psychological space. Psychometrika, 22, 325–345.
Smallwood, J. and Schooler, J. W. (2006). The restless mind. Psychological Bulletin, 132, 946–958.
Sutton, J. (2010). Carelessness and inattention: Mind-wandering and the physiology of fantasy from Locke to Hume. In C. Wolfe and G. Ofer (Eds.), The body as object and instrument of knowledge: Embodied empiricism in early modern science (pp. 243–263). New York: Springer.
Swets, J. (1984). Mathematical models of attention. In R. Parasuraman and D. R. Davies (Eds.), Varieties of attention (pp. 183–242). New York: Academic Press.
Tanner, W. P. (1956). Theory of recognition. Journal of Acoustic Society of America, 28, 882–888.
Thurstone, L. L. (1927). A law of comparative judgment. Psychological Review, 34, 273–287.
Thurstone, L. L. (1945). The prediction of choice. Psychometrika, 10, 237–253.
Tombu, M. and Jolicœur, P. (2003). A central capacity sharing model of dual-task performance. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 29, 3–18.
Treisman, A. M. and Gelade, G. (1980). A feature-integration theory of attention. Cognitive Psychology, 12, 97–136.
Warm, J. S. (1984). Sustained attention in human performance. Chichester, UK: Wiley.
Warm, J. S., Parasuraman, R. and Matthews, G. (2008). Vigilance requires hard mental work and is stressful. Human Factors, 50, 433–441.
Welford, A. T. (1952). The “psychological refractory period” and the timing of high-speed performance – a review and a theory. British Journal of Psychology, 43, 2–19.
Wickens, C. D. (1984). Processing resources in attention. In R. Parasuraman and D. R. Davies (Eds.), Varieties of attention (pp. 63–102). Orlando: Academic Press.
Wickens, T. (2002). Elementary signal detection theory. New York: Oxford University Press.
Yamaguchi, M. and Proctor, R. W. (2012). Multidimensional vector model of stimulus-response compatibility. Psychological Review, 119, 272–303. DOI: 10.1037/a0026620.
Zhang, H., Zhang, J. and Kornblum, S. (1999). A parallel distributed processing model of stimulus-stimulus and stimulus-response compatibility. Cognitive Psychology, 38, 386–432.
Zorzi, M. and Umiltà, C. (1995). A computational model of the Simon effect. Psychological Research, 58, 193–205.