Skip to main content Accessibility help
×
Hostname: page-component-8448b6f56d-jr42d Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-04-25T03:43:28.419Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

4 - Language and self-consciousness: modes of self-presentation in language structure

from Part I - Conceptual background

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  18 December 2009

Maxim I. Stamenov
Affiliation:
Georg-August-Universität Göttingen, Germany and Bulgarian Academy of Sciences, Sofia, Bulgaria
Tilo Kircher
Affiliation:
Eberhard-Karls-Universität Tübingen, Germany
Anthony David
Affiliation:
Institute of Psychiatry, London
Get access

Summary

Abstract

One of the most prominent diagnostic symptoms of schizophrenia amounts to hearing an inner voice that is not attributed to the self. A proper explanation of the nature of the schizophrenic condition requires, correspondingly, an orientation of the relations between language faculty and self-consciousness – both in norm and in pathology. The aim of this chapter is to offer an overview of some basic aspects of this relationship. The presentation will be by necessity brief and selective, as the problem area is wide and there is virtually no literature explicitly dealing with it from a linguistic perspective.

In the first part of the chapter the point is made that speech, inner speech in particular, serves as the subtlest (and, in a sense, the ultimate) vehicle of online maintained self-awareness. The experience of voice here and now provides the self with the means of making itself ‘visible’ during language performance. In inner speech the loop between perception cum motor behaviour cum perception is the shortest one and is enacted with the highest possible time resolution. Thus, inner speech forms the most compact perceptual–motor loop (the shortest ‘specious present’) capable of supporting self-awareness online. In the very same time, it serves as a phenomenal correlate of very complex cognitive processes implementing human thinking. If this is the case, the estrangement from one's voice is indicative of a foundational disturbance in self-awareness.

Type
Chapter
Information
Publisher: Cambridge University Press
Print publication year: 2003

Access options

Get access to the full version of this content by using one of the access options below. (Log in options will check for institutional or personal access. Content may require purchase if you do not have access.)

References

Abu-Akeel, A. (1999). Impaired theory of mind in schizophrenia. Pragmatics and Cognition, 7, 247–82Google Scholar
American Psychiatric Association. (1994). Diagnostic and Statistical Manual of Mental Disorders, 4th edn. Washington: APA
Brook, A. & DeVidi, R. (eds) (2001). Self-awareness and Self-reference. Amsterdam: John Benjamins
Chomsky, N. (1993). Language and Thought. Wakefield, RI: Moyer Bell
Chomsky, N. (2000). New Horizons in the Study of Language. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press
Crow, T. J. (2000). Schizophrenia as the price the Homo sapiens pays for language: a resolution of the central paradox in the origin of the species. Brain Research Reviews, 31, 118–29Google Scholar
Cutler, A., Mehler, J., Norris, D. & Segui, J. (1992). The monolingual nature of speech segmentation in bilinguals. Cognitive Psychology, 24, 381–410Google Scholar
DeCasper, A. J. & Spence, M. J. (1986). Prenatal maternal speech influences newborns' perception of speech sounds. Infant Behavior and Development, 9, 133–50Google Scholar
Dennett, D. (1991). Consciousness Explained. Boston: Little, Brown
Georgov, I. (1905). Die ersten Anfänge des sprachlichen Ausdrucks für das Selbstbewusstsein bei Kindern. Archiv für die gesamte Psychologie, 5, 329–404Google Scholar
Gernsbacher, M., Tallent, K. A. & Bolliger, C. M. (1999). Disordered discourse in schizophrenia described by the structure building framework. Discourse Studies, 1, 355–72Google Scholar
Haegeman, L. & Guéron, J. (1999). English Grammar: A Generative Perspective. Oxford: Blackwell
Haiman, J. (1998). Talk is Cheap: Sarcasm, Alienation, and the Evolution of Language. New York: Oxford University Press
James, W. (1890). Principles of Psychology, vols I–II. New York: Henry Holt
Langacker, R. W. (1987). Foundations of Cognitive Grammar, vol. 1: Theoretical Prerequisites. Stanford: Stanford University Press
Langacker, R. W. (1997). Consciousness, construal, and subjectivity. In Language Structure, Discourse and the Access to Consciousness, ed. M. Stamenov, pp. 49–75. Amsterdam: John Benjamins
Levelt, W. J. M. (1989). Speaking: From Intention to Articulation. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press
Levelt, W. J. M., Praamstra, P., Meyer, A. S., Helenius, P. & Salmelin, R. (1998). A MEG study of picture naming. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 10, 553–67Google Scholar
Levelt, W. J. M., Roelofs, A. & Meyer, A. S. (1999). A theory of lexical access in speech production. Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 22, 1–75Google Scholar
Liberman, A. M. & Mattingly, I. G. (1985). The motor theory of speech perception revised. 21, 1–36Google Scholar
Lieberman, P. (1991). Uniquely Human: The Evolution of Speech, Thought, and Selfless Behavior. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press
Lyons, J. (1977). Semantics, vols I–II. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press
Mehler, J. & Dupoux, E. (1994). What Infants Know: The New Cognitive Science of Early Development. Cambridge: Blackwell
Mehler, J., Sebastian, N., Altmann, G. et al. (1993). Understanding compressed sentences: the role of rhythm and meaning. In Temporal Information Processing in the Nervous System, ed. P. Tallal, A. M. Galaburda, R. Llinas & C. von Euler. Annals of the New York Academy of Sciences, 682, 272–282
Miller, G. (1956). The magic number 7 plus minus two: some limits in our capacity for processing information. Psychological Review, 50, 135–47Google Scholar
Nazzi, T., Bertoncini, J. & Mehler, J. (1998). Language discrimination by newborns: towards an understanding of the role of rhythm. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 24, 756–66Google Scholar
Postma, A. (2000). Detection of errors during speech production: a review of speech monitoring models. Cognition, 77, 97–131Google Scholar
Ramus, F., Hauser, M. D., Miller, C., Morris, D. & Mehler, J. (2000). Language discrimination by human newborns and by cotton-top tamarin monkeys. Science, 288, 349–51Google Scholar
Rovee-Collier, C., Hayne, H. & Colombo, M. (2000). The Development of Implicit and Explicit Memory. Advances in Consciousness Research, vol. 24. Amsterdam: John Benjamins
Spence, M. J. & DeCasper, A. J. (1987). Prenatal experience with low-frequency maternal-voice sounds influences neonatal perception of maternal voice samples. Infant Behavior and Development, 10, 133–42Google Scholar
Stamenov, M. I. (1997). Grammar, meaning and consciousness: what sentence structure can tell us about the structure of consciousness. In Language Structure, Discourse and the Access to Consciousness, ed. M. Stamenov, pp. 277–342. Amsterdam: John Benjamins
Stamenov, M. I. (1999). First language acquisition and the ontogenetic development of self-consciousness in the work of Ivan Georgov. In The Emergence of Modern Language Sciences: Studies on the Transition from Historical-comparative to Structural Linguistics in Honor of E. F. Konrad Koerner, ed. S. Embleton, J. E. Joseph & H. J. Niederehe, pp. 91–106. Amsterdam: John Benjamins
Stamenov, M. I. & Gallese, V. (eds) (2002). Mirror Neurons and the Evolution of Brain and Language. Amsterdam: John Benjamins
Torey, E. F. (1988). Surviving Schizophrenia: A Family Manual. New York: Harper & Row
van Dijk, T. A. (1997). Cognitive context models and discourse. In Language Structure, Discourse and the Access to Consciousness, ed. M. Stamenov, pp. 189–226. Amsterdam: John Benjamins
Vogeley, K., Kurthen, M., Falkai, P. & Maier, W. (1999). Essential functions of the human self model are implemented in the prefrontal cortex. Consciousness and Cognition, 8, 343–63Google Scholar
Wheeldon, L. R. & Levelt, W. J. M. (1995). Monitoring the time course of phonological encoding. Journal of Memory and Language, 34, 311–34Google Scholar
Wheeler, M. A. (2000). Varieties of consciousness and memory in the developing child. In Memory, Consciousness, and the Brain: The Tallinn Conference, ed. E. Tulving, pp. 188–99. Philadelphia: Psychology Press
Zahavi, D. (ed.) (2000). Exploring the Self: Philosophical and Psychopathological Perspectives on Self-experience. Advances in Consciousness Research, vol. 23. AmsterdamI: John Benjamins

Save book to Kindle

To save this book to your Kindle, first ensure coreplatform@cambridge.org is added to your Approved Personal Document E-mail List under your Personal Document Settings on the Manage Your Content and Devices page of your Amazon account. Then enter the ‘name’ part of your Kindle email address below. Find out more about saving to your Kindle.

Note you can select to save to either the @free.kindle.com or @kindle.com variations. ‘@free.kindle.com’ emails are free but can only be saved to your device when it is connected to wi-fi. ‘@kindle.com’ emails can be delivered even when you are not connected to wi-fi, but note that service fees apply.

Find out more about the Kindle Personal Document Service.

Available formats
×

Save book to Dropbox

To save content items to your account, please confirm that you agree to abide by our usage policies. If this is the first time you use this feature, you will be asked to authorise Cambridge Core to connect with your account. Find out more about saving content to Dropbox.

Available formats
×

Save book to Google Drive

To save content items to your account, please confirm that you agree to abide by our usage policies. If this is the first time you use this feature, you will be asked to authorise Cambridge Core to connect with your account. Find out more about saving content to Google Drive.

Available formats
×