Skip to main content Accessibility help
×
Home
Hostname: page-component-558cb97cc8-9njm9 Total loading time: 2.912 Render date: 2022-10-06T19:37:36.341Z Has data issue: true Feature Flags: { "shouldUseShareProductTool": true, "shouldUseHypothesis": true, "isUnsiloEnabled": true, "useRatesEcommerce": false, "displayNetworkTab": true, "displayNetworkMapGraph": true, "useSa": true } hasContentIssue true

References

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  12 October 2017

David Hargreaves
Affiliation:
Roehampton University, London
Alexandra Lamont
Affiliation:
Keele University
Get access

Summary

Image of the first page of this content. For PDF version, please use the ‘Save PDF’ preceeding this image.'
Type
Chapter
Information
Publisher: Cambridge University Press
Print publication year: 2017

Access options

Get access to the full version of this content by using one of the access options below. (Log in options will check for institutional or personal access. Content may require purchase if you do not have access.)

References

Abbott, A., & Collins, D. (2004). Eliminating the dichotomy between theory and practice in talent identification and development: Considering the role of psychology. Journal of Sport Science, 22(5), 395408.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Abikoff, H., Courtney, M. E., Szeibel, P. J., & Koplewicz, H. S. (1996). The effects of auditory stimulation on the arithmetic performance of children with ADHD and nondisabled children. Journal of Learning Disabilities, 29(3), 238246.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Abrahams, F., & Abrahams, D. (2016). Child as musical apprentice. In McPherson, G. E. (Ed.), The child as musician: A handbook of musical development (2nd edition, pp. 538555). Oxford: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Adachi, M. (1994). The role of the adult in the child’s early musical socialization: A Vygotskian perspective. Quarterly Journal of Music Teaching and Learning, 5, 2635.Google Scholar
Adachi, M., & Trehub, S. E. (1998). Children’s expression of emotion in song. Psychology of Music, 26, 133153.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Adachi, M., & Trehub, S. E. (2011). Canadian and Japanese preschoolers’ creation of happy and sad songs. Psychomusicology: Music, Mind & Brain, 21(1/2), 130143.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Adams, G., & Markus, H. R. (2004). Toward a conception of culture suitable for a social psychology of culture. In Schaller, M. & Crandall, C. S (Eds.), The psychological foundations of culture (pp. 335360). Mahwah, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates.Google Scholar
Addessi, A. R., Baroni, M., Luzzi, C., & Tafuri, J. (1995). The development of musical stylistic competence in children. Council for Research in Music Education (CRME) Bulletin, 127, 815.Google Scholar
Adorno, T. W. (1941). On popular music. Zeitschrift für Sozialforschung, 9, 1749.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Adorno, T. W. (1976). Introduction to the sociology of music. Stanford, CA: Stanford University Press.Google Scholar
Alexander, R. J. (2001). Culture and pedagogy: International comparisons in primary education. Oxford: Blackwell Publishing.Google Scholar
Alexander, R. J. (2008). Towards dialogic teaching (4th edition). York: Dialogos.Google Scholar
Alluri, V., Toiviainen, P., Jääskeläinen, I. P., Glerean, E., Sams, M., & Brattico, E. (2012). Large-scale brain networks emerge from dynamic processing of musical timbre, key and rhythm. NeuroImage, 59(4), 36773689.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Altenmüller, E., & Jabusch, H. C. (2010). Focal dystonia in musicians: Phenomenology, pathophysiology, triggering factors, and treatment. Medical Problems of Performing Artists, 25(1), 39.Google ScholarPubMed
Amabile, T. M. (1983). The social psychology of creativity: A componential conceptualization. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 45(2), 357–377.
Amabile, T. M. (1996). Creativity in context. Boulder, CO: Westview Press.Google Scholar
Ammirante, P., Patel, A. D., & Russo, F. A. (2016). Synchronizing to auditory and tactile metronomes: A test of the auditory-motor enhancement hypothesis. Psychonomic Bulletin & Review, 23(6), 1882–1890.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Anderson, S. A., & Fuller, G. B. (2010). Effect of music on reading comprehension of junior high school students. School Psychology Quarterly, 25(3), 178187.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Ando, Y., & Hattori, H. (1970). Effects of intense noise during fetal life upon postnatal adaptability (statistical study of the reactions of babies to aircraft noise). Journal of the Acoustical Society of America, 47, 1128.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Andrade, P. E., Vanzella, P., Andrade, O. V. C. A., & Schellenberg, E. G. (2016). Associating emotions with Wagner’s music: A developmental perspective. Psychology of Music, Online First, DOI: 10.1177/0305735616678056.CrossRef
Anvari, S. H., Trainor, L. J., Woodside, J., & Levy, B. A. (2002). Relations among musical skills, phonological processing, and early reading ability in preschool children. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 83(2), 111113.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Araújo, M. V. (2016). Measuring self-regulated practice behaviours in highly skilled musicians. Psychology of Music, 44(2), 278292.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Argyle, M. (1996). The social psychology of leisure. Harmondsworth: Penguin.Google Scholar
Ascenso, S., Williamon, A., & Perkins, R. (2017). Understanding the wellbeing of professional musicians through the lens of positive psychology. Psychology of Music, 45(1), 65–81.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Atkinson, W. (2011). The context and genesis of musical tastes: Omnivorousness debunked, Bourdieu buttressed. Poetics, 39, 169186.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Auh, M.-S., & Walker, R. (2017). Musical identities in Australia and South Korea and new identities emerging through social media and digital technology. In MacDonald, R. A. R, Hargreaves, D. J., & Miell, D. E (Eds.), Handbook of musical identities (pp. 789805). Oxford: Oxford University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Austin, J., Renwick, J., & McPherson, G. E. (2006). Developing motivation. In McPherson, G. E. (Ed.), The child as musician: A handbook of musical development (pp. 213238). Oxford: Oxford University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Azzara, C. D. (1993). Audiation-based improvisation techniques and elementary instrumental students’ music achievement. Journal of Research in Music Education, 41(4), 328342.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Bacher, L. F., & Robertson, S. S. (2001). Stability of coupled fluctuations in movement and visual attention in infants. Developmental Psychobiology, 39, 99106.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Bahrick, L., Flom, R., & Lickliter, R. (2002). Intersensory redundancy facilitates discrimination of tempo in 3-month-old infants. Developmental Psychobiology, 41, 352363.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Bahrick, L., Lickliter, R., & Flom, R. (2004). Intersensory redundancy guides the development of selective attention, perception, and cognition in infancy. Current Directions in Psychological Science, 13(3), 99102.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Bailey, B., & Davidson, J. W. (2005). Effects of group singing and performance for marginalized and middle-class singers. Psychology of Music, 33, 269303.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Bakagiannis, S., & Tarrant, M. (2006). Can music bring people together? Effects of shared musical preference on intergroup bias in adolescence. Scandinavian Journal of Psychology, 47(2), 129136.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Baker, G. (2014). El Sistema: Orchestrating Venezuela’s youth. New York: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Baltes, P. (1987). Theoretical propositions of life-span developmental psychology: On the dynamics between growth and decline. Developmental Psychology, 23(5), 611626.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Balzano, G. J. (1982). The pitch set as a level of description for studying musical pitch perception. In Clynes, M. (Ed.), Music, mind and brain (pp. 321351). New York: Plenum.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Bamberger, J. (1982). Revisiting children’s drawings of simple rhythms: A function for reflection-in-action. In Strauss, S. & Stavy, R. (Eds.), U-shaped behavioral growth (pp. 191226). New York: Academic Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Bamberger, J. (1986). Cognitive issues in the development of musically gifted children. In Sternberg, R. J. & Davidson, J. E (Eds.), Conceptions of giftedness (pp. 388413). Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
Bamberger, J. (1991). The mind behind the musical ear. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press.Google Scholar
Bamberger, J. (2006). What develops in musical development? In McPherson, G. E. (Ed.), The child as musician: A handbook of musical development (pp. 6991). Oxford: Oxford University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Bamberger, J. (2013). Discovering the musical mind: A view of creativity as learning. Oxford: Oxford University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Bandura, A. (1969). Social-learning theory of identificatory processes. In Goslin, D. A. (Ed.), Handbook of socialization theory and research (pp. 213–262). Chicago, IL: Rand-McNally.Google Scholar
Bandura, A. (1977). Social learning theory. Englewood Cliffs, NJ: Prentice-Hall.Google ScholarPubMed
Bandura, A. (1986). Social foundations of thought and action: A social cognitive theory. Englewood Cliffs, NJ: Prentice-Hall.Google Scholar
Bandura, A. (1997). Self-efficacy: The exercise of control. New York: W. H. Freeman.Google Scholar
Bandura, A. (2000) Exercise of human agency through collective efficacy. Current Directions in Psychological Science, 9(3), 7578.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Bangerter, A., & Heath, C. (2004). The Mozart effect: Tracking the evolution of a scientific legend. British Journal of Social Psychology, 43(40), 605623.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Bannan, N., & Montgomery-Smith, C. (2008). ‘Singing for the brain’: Reflections on the human capacity for music arising from a pilot study of group singing with Alzheimer’s patients. Journal for the Royal Society for the Promotion of Health, 128(2), 7378.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Bannan, N., & Woodward, S. (2009). Spontaneity in the musicality and music learning of children. In Malloch, S. and Trevarthen, C. (Eds.), Communicative musicality: Exploring the basis of human companionship (pp. 465494). Oxford: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Barbot, B., & Lubart, T. (2012). Creative thinking in music: Its nature and assessment through musical exploratory behaviors. Psychology of Aesthetics, Creativity, and the Arts, 6(3), 231242.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Barlow, H., & Morgenstern, S. (1948). A dictionary of musical themes. New York: Crown.Google Scholar
Barlow, H., & Morgenstern, S. (1976). A dictionary of opera and song themes (revised edition). New York: Crown.Google Scholar
Baron-Cohen, S., Leslie, A. M., & Frith, U. (1985). Does the autistic child have a ‘theory of mind’? Cognition, 21(1), 3746.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Barrett, M. (2007). Children’s knowledge, beliefs and feelings about nations and national groups. Hove, UK: Psychology Press.Google Scholar
Barrett, M. S. (1996). Children’s aesthetic decision-making: An analysis of children’s musical discourse as composers. International Journal of Music Education, 28, 3762.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Barrett, M. S. (1997). Invented notations: A view of young children’s musical thinking. Research Studies in Music Education, 8, 214.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Barrett, M. S. (2010). A cultural psychology of music education. Oxford: Oxford University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Barrett, M. S. (2011). Musical narratives: A study of a young child’s identity work in and through music-making. Psychology of Music, 39(4), 403423.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Barrett, M. S., & Tafuri, J. (2012). Creative meaning-making in infants’ and young children’s musical cultures. In McPherson, G. E. & Welch, G. F. (Eds.), Oxford handbook of music education (pp. 296331). New York, NY: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Barry, N. H., & Hallam, S. (2002). Practice. In Parncutt, R. & McPherson, G. E (Eds.), The science and psychology of music performance (pp. 151165). Oxford: Oxford University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Baruch, C., & Drake, C. (1997). Tempo discrimination in infants. Infant Behavior and Development, 20, 573577.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Baruch, C., Panissal-Vieu, N., & Drake, C. (2004). Preferred perceptual tempo for sound sequences: Comparison of adults, children and infants. Perceptual & Motor Skills, 98, 325339.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Bashwiner, D. M., Wertz, C. J., Flores, R. A., & Jung, R. E. (2016). Musical creativity ‘revealed’ in brain structure: Interplay between motor, default mode, and limbic networks. Scientific Reports, 6:20482, DOI: 10.1038/srep20482.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Batt-Rawden, K., & DeNora, T. (2005). Music and informal learning in everyday life. Music Education Research, 7(3), 289304.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Beaty, R. E. (2015). The neuroscience of musical improvisation. Neuroscience and Biobehavioral Reviews, 51, 108117.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Beck, R. J., Cesario, C., Yousefi, A., & Enamoto, H. (2000). Choral singing, performance perception, and immune system changes in salivary immunoglobulin A and cortisol. Music Perception, 18(1), 87106.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Beck, R. J., Gottfried, T. L., Hall, D. J., Cisler, C. A., & Bozeman, K. W. (2006). Supporting the health of college solo singers: The relationship of positive emotions and stress to changes in salivary IgA and cortisol during singing. Journal of Learning through the Arts: A Research Journal on Arts Integration in Schools and Communities, 2(1), article 19.Google Scholar
Beck, U. (1992). Risk society: Towards a new modernity. London and Thousand Oaks: Sage Publications.Google Scholar
Behne, K.-E. (1975). Musikalische Konzepte: Zur Schicht- und Altersspezifität musikalischer Präferenzen. In E. Kraus (Ed.), Forschung in der Musikerziehung (pp. 35–61). Mainz: Schott.
Bennett, M., Lyons, E., Sani, F., & Barrett, M. (1998). Children’s subjective identification with the group and ingroup favoritism. Developmental Psychology, 34, 902909.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Bergeson, T. R., & Trehub, S. E. (2006). Infants’ perception of rhythmic patterns. Music Perception, 23(4), 345360.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Berliner, P. F. (1994). Thinking in jazz: The infinite art of improvisation. Chicago, IL: University of Chicago Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Berlyne, D. E. (1971). Aesthetics and psychobiology. New York: Appleton-Century-Crofts.Google Scholar
Bernard, M. (1995). Social forces and aging: An introduction to social gerontology. Ageing and Society, 15, 566569.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Bernard, M., Phillipson, C., Phillips, J., & Ogg, J. (2001). Continuity and change in the family and community life of older people. Journal of Applied Gerontology, 20(3), 259278.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Berzonsky, M. D. (1989). Identity style: Conceptualization and measurement. Journal of Adolescent Research, 4, 268282.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Berzonsky, M. D. (1990). Self-construction over the lifespan: A process perspective on identity formation. In Neimeyer, G. J & Neimeyer, R. A (Eds.), Advances in personal construct theory (vol. 1, pp. 155186). Greenwich, CT: JAI Press.Google Scholar
Bharucha, J. J. (1984). Event hierarchies, tonal hierarchies, and assimilation: A reply to Deutsch and Dowling. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 6, 501515.Google Scholar
Bibace, R., & Walsh, M. E. (1980). Development of children’s concepts of illness. Pediatrics, 66, 912917.Google ScholarPubMed
Bigand, E., Delbé, C., Poulin-Charronnat, B., Leman, M., & Tillmann, B. (2014). Empirical evidence for musical syntax processing? Computer simulations reveal the contribution of auditory short-term memory. Frontiers in Systems Neuroscience, DOI: 10.3389/fnsys.2014.00094.CrossRef
Bilhartz, T. D., Bruhn, R. A., & Olson, J. E. (1999). The effect of early music training on child cognitive development. Journal of Applied Developmental Psychology, 20, 615636.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Billig, M., & Tajfel, H. (1973). Social categorization and similarity of intergroup behaviour. European Journal of Social Psychology, 3, 2752.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Bird, J. M., Hall, J., Arnold, R., Karageorghis, C. I., & Hussein, A. (2016). Effects of music and music-video on core affect during exercise at the lactate threshold. Psychology of Music, 44(6), 14711487.CrossRef
Birns, B. (1965). Individual differences in human neonates’ responses to stimulation. Child Development, 36(1), 249256.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Birns, B., Blank, M., Bridger, W. H., & Escalona, S.K. (1965). Behavioral inhibition in neonates produced by auditory stimuli. Child Development, 36(3), 639645.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Bjurström, E., & Wennhall, J. (1991). Ungdomar och music. In Statens Ungdomsråds Årsbok om ungdom (pp. 82–88). Stockholm: Statens ungdomsråd.Google Scholar
Blacking, J. (1973). How musical is man? Seattle: University of Washington Press.Google Scholar
Blair, C., & Razza, R. P. (2007) Relating effortful control, executive function, and false belief understanding to emerging math and literacy ability in kindergarten. Child Development, 78(2), 647663.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Blakemore, S.-J. (2010). The developing social brain: Implications for education. Neuron, 65, 744747.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Blood, A. J., & Zatorre, R. J. (2001). Intensely pleasurable responses to music correlate with activity in brain regions implicated in reward and emotion. Proceedings of National Academy of Sciences, 98, 1181811823.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Boal-Palheiros, G. M., & Hargreaves, D. J. (2001). Listening to music at home and at school. British Journal of Music Education, 18, 103118.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Boden, M. (1994). Dimensions of creativity. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.
Boden, M. (1999). Computer models of creativity. In R. J. Sternberg (Ed.), Handbook of creativity (pp. 351–372). Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.
Bodner, E., & Bensimon, M. (2015). Problem music and its different shades over its fans. Psychology of Music, 43(5), 641660.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Boer, D., Fischer, R., Strack, M., Bond, M. H., Lo, E., & Lam, J. (2011). How shared preferences in music create bonds between people. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 37(9), 11591171.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Bonneville-Roussy, A., & Bouffard, T. (2015). When quantity is not enough: Disentangling the roles of practice time, deliberate practice and self-regulation in musical achievement. Psychology of Music, 43(5), 686704.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Bonneville-Roussy, A., Lavigne, G. L., & Vallerand, R. J. (2011). When passion leads to excellence: The case of musicians. Psychology of Music, 39(1), 123138.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Bonneville-Roussy, A., Rentfrow, P. J., Xu, M. K., & Potter, J. (2013). Music through the ages: Trends in musical engagement and preferences from adolescence through middle adulthood. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 105(4), 703717.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Bonneville-Roussy, A., Stillwell, D., Kosinski, M., & Rust, J. (2016). Age trends in musical preferences in adulthood 1: Conceptualization and empirical investigation. Musicae Scientiae. Online First, DOI: 10.1177/1029864917691571.CrossRef
Boone, R. T., & Cunningham, J. G. (2001). Children’s expression of emotional meaning in music through expressive body movement. Journal of Nonverbal Behavior, 25(1), 2141.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Bottiroli, S., Rosi, A., Russo, R., Vecchi, T., & Cavallini, E. (2014). The cognitive effects of listening to background music on older adults: Processing speed improves with upbeat music, while memory seems to benefit from both upbeat and downbeat music. Frontiers in Aging Neuroscience, 6(284), 17, DOI: 10.3389/fnagi.2014.00284.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Bourdieu, P. (1971). Intellectual field and creative project. In Young, M. F. D. (Ed.), Knowledge and control (pp. 161188). London: Collier-Macmillan.Google Scholar
Bourdieu, P. (1984). Distinction: A social critique of the judgement of taste. London: Routledge.Google Scholar
Boysson-Bardies, B. de (1999). How language comes to children: From birth to two years. Cambridge, MA: The MIT Press.Google Scholar
Brace, G. (1968). The story of music. London: Ladybird Books.Google Scholar
Bradley, B. S. (2009). Early trios: Patterns of sound and movement in the genesis of meaning between infants. In Malloch, S. and Trevarthen, C. (Eds.), Communicative musicality: Exploring the basis of human companionship (pp. 263280). Oxford: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Brand, E. (2000). Children’s mental musical organisations as highlighted by their singing errors. Psychology of Music, 28(1), 6280.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Brattico, E., & Pearce, M. (2013). The neuroaesthetics of music. Psychology of Aesthetics, Creativity, and the Arts, 7(1), 48.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Braun Janzen, T., Thompson, W. F., & Ranvaud, R. (2014). A developmental study of the effect of music training on timed movements. Frontiers in Human Neuroscience, 8, 801, DOI: 10.3389/fnhum.2014.00801.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Brodsky, W., & Slor, Z. (2013). Background music as a risk factor for distraction among young-novice drivers. Accident Analysis & Prevention, 59, 382393.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Bronfenbrenner, U. (1979). The ecology of human development. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press.Google Scholar
Bronfenbrenner, U. (1986). Ecology of the family as a context for human development: Research perspectives. Developmental Psychology, 22(6), 723742.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Bronfenbrenner, U. (2001). Human development, bioecological theory of. In N. J. Smelser & B. Baltes (Eds.), International encyclopedia of the social and behavioral sciences. 10–6963.CrossRef
Bronfenbrenner, U. (2005). Making human beings human: Bioecological perspectives on human development. Thousand Oaks, CA: Sage Publications.Google Scholar
Brophy, T. S. (2005). A longitudinal study of selected characteristics of children’s melodic improvisations. Journal of Research in Music Education, 53(2), 120133.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Brown, A. R. (2016). Engaging in a sound musicianship. In McPherson, G. E (Ed.), The child as musician: A handbook of musical development (2nd edition, pp. 208220). Oxford: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Brown, B., & Sellen, A. (2006). Sharing and listening to music. In O’Hara, K. & Brown, B. (Eds.), Consuming music together: Social and collaborative aspects of music consumption technologies (pp. 3756). London: Springer.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Brown, H., Butler, D., & Jones, M. R. (1994). Musical and temporal influences on key discovery. Music Perception, 11, 371407.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Brown, S. C. (2014). Approaches to digital piracy research: A call for innovation. Convergence: The International Journal of Research in New Music Technologies, 20(2), 129139.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Brown, S. C. (2015). The psychology of music piracy. Unpublished Ph.D. thesis, Glasgow Caledonian University.
Brown, W. A., Cammuso, K., Sachs, H., Winklosky, B., Mullane, J., Bernier, R., Svenson, S., Arin, D., Rosen-Sheidley, B., & Folstein, S. E. (2003). Autism-related language, personality, and cognition in people with absolute pitch: Results of a preliminary study. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 33, 163167.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Bruner, J. S. (1960). The process of education. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press.Google Scholar
Bruner, J. S. (1966). Toward a theory of instruction. Cambridge, MA: Belknapp Press.Google Scholar
Bruner, , J. S. (1986). Actual minds, possible worlds. Cambridge. MA: Harvard University Press.Google Scholar
Bruner, J. S. (1990). Acts of meaning. Cambridge. MA: Harvard University Press.Google Scholar
Bunte, N. (2014). Musical concepts as explanation for children’s musical preference in primary school age. In Jakubowski, K., Farrugia, N., Floridou, G. A, & Gagen, J. (Eds.), Proceedings of the 7th International Conference of Students of Systematic Musicology (SysMus14). London, UK, 18–20 September 2014. www.musicmindbrain.com/#!sysmus-2014/cf, accessed 8 April 2017.Google Scholar
Bunte, N., & Busch, V. (2015). Exploring musical concepts for their relevance to children’s music preferences. In Ginsborg, J., Lamont, A., Phillips, M., & Bramley, S. (Eds.), Proceedings of the Ninth Triennial Conference of the European Society for the Cognitive Sciences of Music, 17–22 August 2015, Manchester, UK.Google Scholar
Bunting, R. (1977). The common language of music, music in the secondary school curriculum. Working Paper 6.
Burnard, P. (2006). Children’s meaning-making as composers. In Deliège, I. & Wiggins, G. A (Eds.), Musical creativity: Multidisciplinary research in theory and practice (pp. 111133). Hove: Psychology Press.Google Scholar
Burnard, P., & Kuo, H.-C. (2016). The individual and social worlds of children’s musical creativities. In McPherson, G. E (Ed.), The child as musician: A handbook of musical development (pp. 485499). Oxford: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Burnard, P., & Younker, B.A. (2008). Investigating children’s musical interactions within the activities systems of group composing and arranging: An application of Engeström’s activity theory. International Journal of Educational Research, 47(1), 1126.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Busch, V., Bunte, N., & Schurig, M. (2016). Open-earedness, musical concepts, and gender identity. In Krämer, O. & Malmberg, I. (Eds.), European perspectives on music education VI: Open ears – open minds. Listening and understanding music (pp. 151–165). Innsbruck, Esslingen, Bern-Belp: Helbling.Google Scholar
Byrne, C., MacDonald, R., & Carlton, L. (2003). Assessing creativity in musical compositions: Flow as an assessment tool. British Journal of Music Education, 20(3), 277–290.CrossRef
Campbell, D. (2001). The Mozart Effect: Tapping the power of music to heal the body, strengthen the mind, and unlock the creative spirit. London: HarperCollins.Google Scholar
Campbell, D. (2009). The Mozart Effect for children: Awakening your child’s mind, health, and creativity with music. London: HarperCollins.Google Scholar
Campbell, J. D., Trapnell, P. D., Heine, S. J., Katz, I. M., Lavalle, L. F., & Lehman, D. R. (1996). Self-concept clarity: Measurement, personality correlates, and cultural boundaries. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 70, 141156.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Campbell, P. S. (1991). Lessons from the world: A cross-cultural guide to teaching and learning. New York; Schirmer.
Campbell, P. S. (2010). Songs in their heads: Music and its meaning in children’s lives (2nd edition). Oxford: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Campbell, P. S. (2011). Musical enculturation: Sociocultural influences and meanings of children’s experiences in and through music. In Barrett, M. S (Ed.), A cultural psychology of music education (pp. 6181). Oxford: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Campbell, P. S. (2016). Global practices. In McPherson, G. E (Ed.), The child as musician: A handbook of musical development (2nd edition, pp. 556576). Oxford: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Capra, F. (1996). The web of life: A new scientific understanding of living systems. New York: HarperCollins.Google Scholar
Castano, E., Paladino, M. P., Coull, A., & Yzerbyt, V. Y. (2002). Protecting the ingroup stereotype: Ingroup identification and the management of deviant ingroup members. British Journal of Social Psychology, 41, 365385.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Castell, K. C. (1982). Children’s sensitivity to stylistic differences in ‘classical’ and ‘popular’ music. Psychology of Music, Special issue, 22–25.
Castellano, M. A., Bharucha, J. J., & Krumhansl, C. L. (1984). Tonal hierarchies in the music of North India. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 113, 394412.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Chaffin, R., Imreh, G., & Crawford, M. (2002). Practicing perfection: Memory and piano performance. New York: Psychology Press.Google ScholarPubMed
Chan, T., & Goldthorpe, J. H. (2007). The social stratification of cultural consumption: Some policy implications of a research project. Cultural Trends, 16(4), 373–384.CrossRef
Chancellor, J., Lyubomirsky, S., & Wang, L. (2011). Delivering happiness: Translating positive psychology intervention research for treating major and minor depressive disorders. Journal of Alternative and Complementary Medicine, 17(8), 19.Google Scholar
Chandler, C. L., & Connell, J. P. (1987). Children’s intrinsic, extrinsic and internalized motivation: A developmental study of children’s reasons for liked and disliked behaviours. British Journal of Developmental Psychology, 5, 357365.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Chang, H. W., & Trehub, S. E. (1977). Infants’ perception of temporal grouping in auditory patterns. Child Development, 48, 16661670.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Chappell, T. (2011). On the very idea of criteria for personhood. Southern Journal of Philosophy, 49(1), 127.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Cheetham, T. J., Turner-Cobb, J. M., & Gamble, T. (2016). Children’s implicit understanding of the stress-illness link: Testing development of health cognitions. British Journal of Health Psychology, 21(4), 781795.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Chelli, D., & Chanoufi, B. (2008). Audition foetale: Mythe ou realité? Journal de Gynécologie Obstétrique et Biologie de la Reproduction, 37(6), 554558.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Cheong-Clinch, C. (2009). Music for engaging young people in education. Youth Studies Australia, 28(2), 2957.Google Scholar
Chobart, J., François, C., Velay, J. L., & Besson, M. (2014). Twelve months of active musical training in 8- to 10-year-old children enhances the preattentive processing of syllabic duration and voice onset time. Cerebral Cortex, 24, 956967.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Choi, A.-N., Lee, M. S., & Lee, J.-S. (2010). Group music intervention reduces aggression and improves self-esteem in children with highly aggressive behavior: A pilot controlled trial. eCAM, 7(2), 213217.Google ScholarPubMed
Cirelli, L. K., Bosnyak, D., Manning, F. C., Spinelli, C., Marie, C., Fujioka, T., Ghahremani, A., & Trainor, L. J. (2014). Beat-induced fluctuations in auditory cortical beta-band activity: Using EEG to measure age-related changes. Frontiers in Psychology, 5(742), DOI: 10.3389/fpsych.2014.00742.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Cirelli, L. K., Einarson, K. M., & Trainor, L. J. (2013). Interpersonal synchrony increases prosocial behavior in infants. Developmental Science, 16(6), 10031011.Google Scholar
Cirelli, L. K., Spinelli, C., Nozaradan, S., & Trainor, L. J. (2016). Measuring neural entrainment to beat and meter in infants: Effects of musical background. Frontiers in Neuroscience, DOI: 10.3389/fnins.2016.00229.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Clarke, E. (1988). Generative principles in music performance. In Sloboda, J. A (Ed.), Generative processes in music (pp. 126). Oxford: Clarendon Press.Google Scholar
Clarke, E. (1989). Mind the gap: formal structures and psychological processes in music. Contemporary Music Review, 3, 1–13.CrossRef
Clarke, E. (2004). Empirical methods in the study of performance. In E. Clarke & N. Cook (Eds.), Empirical musicology: Aims, methods, prospects (pp. 77–102). Oxford: Oxford University Press.
Clarke, E. (2005). Ways of listening: An ecological approach to the perception of musical meaning. Oxford: Oxford University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Clarke, E. (2012). Creativity in performance. In Hargreaves, D. J, Miell, D. E, & MacDonald, R. A. R (Eds.), Musical Imaginations: Multidisciplinary perspectives on creativity, performance and perception (pp. 1730). Oxford: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Clarke, E., & Cook, N. (Eds.) (2004). Empirical musicology: Aims, methods, prospects. Oxford: Oxford University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Clift, S. (2012). Singing, wellbeing, and health. In MacDonald, R. A. R, Kreutz, G., & Mitchell, L. (Eds.), Music, health, & wellbeing (pp. 113124). Oxford: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Clift, S., & Hancox, G. (2001). The perceived benefits of singing: Findings from preliminary surveys of a university college choral society. Journal of the Royal Society for the Promotion of Health, 121(4), 248256.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Clift, S., Hancox, G., Morrison, I., Hess, B., Kreutz, G., & Stewart, D. (2010). Choral singing and psychological wellbeing: Quantitative and qualitative findings from English choirs in a cross-national survey. Journal of Applied Arts and Health, 1(1), 1934.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Clift, S., Nicol, J., Raisbeck, M., Whitmore, C., & Morrison, I. (2010). Group singing, wellbeing and health: A systematic mapping of research evidence. Kent: Sidney De Haan Research Centre for Arts and Health, Canterbury Christ Church University.Google Scholar
Cole, M. (1996). Cultural psychology: A once and future discipline. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press.Google ScholarPubMed
Coleman, J. C., & Hagell, A. (Eds.) (2007). Adolescence: Risk and resilience. Chichester: John Wiley.Google Scholar
Coleman, J. C., & Hendry, L. B. (1999). The nature of adolescence (3rd edition). London: Routledge.Google Scholar
Colwell, R., & Abrahams, F. (1991). Edwin Gordon’s contribution: An appraisal. The Quarterly Journal of Music Teaching and Learning, 2(1/2), 1936.Google Scholar
Connell, J. P. (1990). Context, self, and action: A motivational analysis of self-system processes across the life span. In Cicchetti, D. (Ed.), The self in transition: Infancy to adulthood (pp. 6197). Chicago, IL: University of Chicago Press.Google Scholar
Conrad, N. J., Walsh, J., Allen, J. M., & Tsang, C. D. (2011). Examining infants’ preferences for tempo in lullabies and playsongs. Canadian Journal of Experimental Psychology, 65(3), 168172.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Conway, C. M., Pisoni, D. B., & Kronenberger, W. G. (2009). The importance of sound for cognitive sequencing abilities: The auditory scaffolding hypothesis. Current Directions in Psychological Science, 18(5), 275279.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Cook, N. (1987). The perception of large-scale tonal closure. Music Perception, 5(2), 197206.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Cook, N. (1998). Music: A very short introduction. Oxford: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Cooper, R. P., & Aslin, R.N. (1990). Preference for infant-directed speech in the first month after birth. Child Development, 61, 15841595.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Corrigall, K. A., & Trainor, L. J. (2009). Effects of musical training on key and harmony perception. The Neurosciences and Music III: Disorders and Plasticity: Annals of the New York Academy of Sciences, 1169, 164168.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Corrigall, K. A., & Trainor, L. J. (2010). Musical enculturation in preschool children: Acquisition of key and harmonic knowledge. Music Perception, 28(2), 195200.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Corrigall, K. A., & Trainor, L. J. (2014). Enculturation to musical pitch structure in young children: Evidence from behavioural and electrophysiological methods. Developmental Science, 17(1), 142158.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Costa, P. T. Jr., & McCrae, R. R. (1985). The NEO Personality Inventory. Odessa, FL: Psychological Assessment Resources.PubMed
Costa-Giomi, E. (1999). The effects of three years of piano instruction on children’s cognitive development. Journal of Research in Music Education, 47, 198212.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Costa-Giomi, E. (2004). Effects of three years of piano instruction on children’s academic achievement, school performance and self-esteem. Psychology of Music, 32(2), 139152.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Costa-Giomi, E., & Descombes, V. (1996). Pitch labels with single and multiple meanings: A study with French-speaking children. Journal of Research in Music Education, 44(3), 204214.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Costa-Giomi, E., & Ilari, B. (2014). Infants’ preferential attention to sung and spoken stimuli. Journal of Research in Music Education, 62(2), 188194.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Coulton, S., Clift, S., Skingley, A., & Rodriguez, J. (2015). Effectiveness and cost-effectiveness of community singing on mental health-related quality of life of older people: Randomised controlled trial. The British Journal of Psychiatry, 206, 16.Google Scholar
Cox, G., Crickmore, L., Plummeridge, C., & Sergeant, D. (2012). SEMPRE: Forty years on. Psychology of Music, 40(5), 523538.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Crafts, S. D., Cavicchi, D., Keil, C., and the Music in Daily Life Project (1993). My Music. Hanover, NH: Wesleyan University Press /University Press of New England.Google Scholar
Creech, A., & Hallam, S. (2003). Parent-teacher-pupil interactions in instrumental music tuition: A literature review. British Journal of Music Education, 20, 2944.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Creech, A., Hallam, S., Varvarigou, M., & McQueen, H. (2014a). Active ageing with music: Supporting wellbeing in the third and fourth ages. Institute of Education, London.Google Scholar
Creech, A., Hallam, S., Varvarigou, M., Gaunt, H., McQueen, H., & Pincas, A. (2014b). The role of musical possible selves in supporting subjective well-being in later life. Music Education Research, 16, 3249.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Creel, S. C. (2016). Ups and downs in auditory development: Preschoolers’ sensitivity to pitch contour and timbre. Cognitive Science, 40(2), 373–403.CrossRef
Crisp, R. J., Stone, C. H., & Hall, N. R. (2006). Recategorization and subgroup identification: Predicting and preventing threats from common ingroups. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 32(2), 230243.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Črnčec, R., Wilson, S. J., & Prior, M. (2006). No evidence for the Mozart effect in children. Music Perception, 23(4), 305317.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Croom, A. M. (2015). Music practice and participation for psychological well-being: A review of how music influences positive emotion, engagement, relationships, meaning, and accomplishment. Musicae Scientiae, 19(1), 4464.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Crow, G., & Allan, G. (1994). Community life. Hemel Hempstead: Harvester Wheatsheaf.Google Scholar
Crowne, D. P., & Marlowe, D. (1960). A new scale of social desirability independent of psychopathology. Journal of Consulting Psychology, 24, 349354.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Crozier, J. (1997). Absolute pitch: Practice makes perfect, the earlier the better. Psychology of Music, 25(2), 110119.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Csikszentmihalyi, M. (1990). Flow: The psychology of optimal experience. New York: Harper and Row.Google Scholar
Csikszentmihalyi, M. (2002). Flow: The classic work on how to achieve happiness. London: Rider.Google Scholar
Csikszentmihalyi, M., & Csikszentmihalyi, I. S. (1988). Optimal experience. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Csikszentmihalyi, M., & Lefèvre, J. (1989). Optimal experience in work and leisure. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 56(5), 815822.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Csikszentmihalyi, M., & Rochberg-Halton, E. (1981). The meaning of things: Domestic symbols and the self. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Cuddy, L. L., & Badertscher, B. D. (1987). Recovery of the tonal hierarchy: Some comparisons across age and levels of musical experience. Perception and Psychophysics, 41, 609620.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Cunningham, J. G., & Sterling, R. S. (1988). Developmental change in the understanding of affective meaning in music. Motivation and Emotion, 12, 399413.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Custodero, L. A. (2005). Observable indicators of flow experience: A developmental perspective on musical engagement in young children from infancy to school age. Music Education Research, 7(2), 185209.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Custodero, L. A. (2006). Singing practices in 10 families with young children. Journal of Research in Music Education, 54(1), 3756.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Custodero, L. A., Britto, P. R., & Xin, T. (2002). From Mozart to Motown, lullabies to love songs: A preliminary report on the Parents’ Use of Music with Infants Survey (PUMIS). Zero to Three, 23(1), 4146.Google Scholar
Custodero, L. A., Calì, C., & Diaz-Donoso, A. (2016). Music as transitional object and practice: Children’s spontaneous musical behaviors in the subway. Research Studies in Music Education, 38(1), 5574.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Dalla Bella, S., Peretz, I., Rousseau, L., & Gosselin, N. (2001). A developmental study of the affective value of tempo and mode in music. Cognition, 80(3), B1B10.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Davidson, C. W., & Powell, L. A. (1986). The effects of easy-listening background music on the on-task-performance of fifth-grade children. The Journal of Educational Research, 80(1), 2933.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Davidson, J. W. (2004). Music as social behavior. In Clarke, E. & Cook, N. (Eds.), Empirical musicology: Aims, methods, prospects (pp. 5775). Oxford: Oxford University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Davidson, J. W. (2011). Musical participation: Expectations, experiences, and outcomes. In Deliège, I. & Davidson, J. W (Eds.), Music and the mind: Essays in honour of John Sloboda (pp. 6587). Oxford: Oxford University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Davidson, J. W., & Burland, K. (2006). Musician identity formation. In McPherson, G. E. (Ed.), The child as musician: A handbook of musical development (pp. 475490). Oxford: Oxford University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Davidson, J. W., & Faulkner, R. (2013). Music in our lives. In Kaufmann, S. B (Ed.), Beyond ‘talent or practice?’: The multiple determinants of greatness (pp. 367390). Oxford: Oxford University Press.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Davidson, J. W., & Fedele, J. (2011). Investigating group singing activity with people with dementia and their caregivers: Problems and positive prospects. Musicae Scientiae, 15(3), 402422.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Davidson, J. W., Howe, M. J. A., Moore, D. M., & Sloboda, J. A. (1996). The role of parental influences in the development of musical ability. British Journal of Developmental Psychology, 14, 399412.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Davidson, J. W., Moore, D. G., Howe, M. J. A., & Sloboda, J. A. (1998). Characteristics of music teachers and the progress of young instrumentalists. Journal of Research in Music Education, 46,141160.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Davidson, L. (1985). Tonal structures of children’s early songs. Music Perception, 2(3), 361373.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Davidson, L. (1994). Songsinging by young and old: A developmental approach to music. In Aiello, R. with Sloboda, J. A (Eds.), Musical perceptions, pp. 99130. Oxford: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Davidson, L., & Scripp, L. (1988). Young children’s musical representations: Windows on music cognition. In Sloboda, J. A (Ed.), Generative processes in music: The psychology of performance, improvisation, and composition (pp. 195230). Oxford: Clarendon Press.Google Scholar
Davidson, L., & Scripp, L. (1989). Education and developmental in music from a cognitive perspective. In Hargreaves, D. J (Ed.), Children and the arts (pp. 5986). Milton Keynes: Open University Press.Google Scholar
Davidson, L., McKernon, P., & Gardner, H. (1981). The acquisition of song: A developmental approach. In Documentary report of the Ann Arbor symposium on the applications of psychology to the teaching and learning of music (pp. 301315). Reston, VA: Music Educators National Council (MENC).Google Scholar
Davies, C. (1992). Listen to my song: A study of songs invented by children aged 5 to 7 years. British Journal of Music Education, 9(1), 1948.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Davies, J. B. (1978). The psychology of music. London: Hutchinson.Google Scholar
Davies, J. B. (1994). Seeds of a false consciousness. In Peer Commentary Issue: Is everyone musical? The Psychologist, 7(8), 355356.Google Scholar
Davis, J., & Gardner, H. (1992). The cognitive revolution: Consequences for the understanding and education of the child as artist. In Reimer, B. & Smith, R. A (Eds.), The arts, education, and aesthetic knowing (vol. 2, pp. 92123). Chicago, IL: The National Society for the Study of Education.Google Scholar
Davis, S. (2016). Children, popular music, and identity. In McPherson, G. E. (Ed.), The child as musician: A handbook of musical development (2nd edition, pp. 265283). Oxford: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
De Boise, S. (2016). Post-Bourdieusian moments and methods in music sociology: Toward a critical, practice-based approach. Cultural Sociology, 10(2), 178–194.CrossRef
De Vries, P. (2009). Music at home with the under fives: What is happening? Early Child Development and Care, 179(4), 395–405.
DeCasper, A. J., & Fifer, W. P. (1980). Of human bonding: Newborns prefer their mother’s voice. Science, 208, 11741176.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
DeCasper, A. J., Granier-Deferre, C., Fifer, W. P., & Moon, C. M. (2011). Measuring fetal cognitive development: When methods and conclusions don’t match. Developmental Science, 14(2), 224225.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
DeCasper, A. J., Lecanuet, J.-P., Maugeais, R., Granier-Deferre, C., & Busnel, M.-C. (1994). Fetal reactions to recurrent maternal speech. Infant Behavior & Development, 17, 159164.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Deci, E. L., & Ryan, R. M. (2002). Handbook of self-determination research. Rochester: University of Rochester Press.Google Scholar
Deffler, S. A., & Halpern, A. R. (2011). Contextual information and memory for unfamiliar tunes in older and younger adults. Psychology and Aging, 26(4), 900904.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Delalande, F. (Ed.) (2009). La nascita della musica. Esplorazioni sonore nella prima infanzia. Milano: Franco Angeli.Google Scholar
Delsing, M. J. M. H., ter Bogt, T. F. M., Engels, R. C. M. E., & Meeus, W. H. J. (2008). Adolescents’ music preferences and personality characteristics. European Journal of Personality, 22, 109130.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Demorest, S. M., & Pfordresher, P. Q. (2015). Singing accuracy development from K–adult: A comparative study. Music Perception, 32(3), 293302.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Dennis, M. (1980). Capacity and strategy for syntactic comprehension after left or right hemidecortication. Brain and Language, 10, 287317.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
DeNora, T. (2000). Music in everyday life. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
DeNora, T. (2001). Aesthetic agency and musical practice: New directions in the sociology of music. In Juslin, P. N. & Sloboda, J. A (Eds.), Music and emotion: Theory and research (pp. 161180). Oxford: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
DeNora, T. (2017). Music-ecology and everyday action: Creating, changing and contesting identities. In MacDonald, R. A. R, Hargreaves, D. J, & Miell, D. E (Eds.), Handbook of musical identities (pp. 4662). Oxford: Oxford University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Department for Education (DfE) (2011). The importance of music: A national plan for music education. London: DfE.
Department for Education and Science (DfES) (2004). The music manifesto. www.musicmanifesto.co.uk/
Deutsch, D. (Ed.). (1982). The psychology of music. New York: Academic Press.Google Scholar
Deutsch, D. (Ed.). (1999). The psychology of music (2nd edition). New York: Academic Press.Google Scholar
Deutsch, D. (Ed.). (2013). The psychology of music (3rd edition). New York: Academic Press.Google Scholar
Deutsch, D., & Dooley, K. (2013). Absolute pitch is associated with a large auditory digit span: A clue to its genesis (L). Journal of the Acoustical Society of America, 133(4), 18591891.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Di Benedetto, P., Cavazzon, M., Mondolo, F., Rugiu, G., Peratoner, A., & Biasutti, E. (2009). Voice and choral singing treatment: A new approach for speech and voice disorders in Parkinson’s disease. European Journal of Physical and Rehabilitation Medicine, 45(1), 1319.Google ScholarPubMed
Diener, E., & Chan, M. (2011). Happy people live longer: Subjective well-being contributes to health and longevity. Applied Psychology: Health and Wellbeing, 3(1), 143.Google Scholar
DiMaggio, P., & Useem, M. (1978). Social class and arts consumption: The origins and consequences of class differences in exposure to the arts in America. Theory and Society, 5, 141161.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Dingle, G. A., Brander, C., Ballantyne, J., & Baker, F. A. (2013). ‘To be heard’: The social and mental health benefits of choir singing for disadvantaged adults. Psychology of Music, 41(4), 405421.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Dissanayake, E. (2000). Antecedents of the temporal arts in early mother-infant interaction. In Wallin, N. K., Merker, B., & Brown, S. (Eds.), The origins of music (pp. 389410). Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.Google Scholar
Doiron, B. A. H., Lehnhard, R. A., Butterfield, S. A., & Whitesides, J. F. (1999). Betaendorphin response to high intensity exercise and music in college-age women. Journal of Strength and Conditioning Research, 13, 2428.Google Scholar
Dowling, W. J., Bartlett, J. C., Halpern, A. R., & Andrews, M. W. (2008). Melody recognition at fast and slow tempos: Effects of age, experience, and familiarity. Perception & Psychophysics, 70(3), 496502.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Drake, C. (1993). Influence of age and experience on timing and intensity variations in the reproduction of short musical rhythms. Psychological Belgica, 33, 217–228.
Drake, C., Jones, M. R., & Baruch, C. (2000). The development of rhythmic attending in auditory sequences: attunement, reference period, focal attending. Cognition, 77, 251–288.CrossRef
Drayton, S., Turley-Ames, K. J., & Guajardo, N. R. (2011). Counterfactual thinking and false belief: The role of executive function. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 108(3), 523548.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Duba, J. D., & Roseman, C. (2012). Musical ‘Tune-Ups’ for couples: Brief treatment interventions. The Family Journal: Counseling and Therapy for Couples and Families, 20(3), 322326.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Duffey, T., Somody, C., & Clifford, S. (2008). Conversations with my father: Adapting A Musical Chronology and The Emerging Life Song with older adults. Journal of Creativity in Mental Health, 2(4), 4563.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Duffey, T., Somody, C., & Eckstein, D. (2009). Musical relationship metaphors: Using A Musical Chronology and The Emerging Life Song with couples. The Family Journal: Counseling and Therapy for Couples and Families, 17(2), 151155.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Dunn, K., Reissland, N., & Reid, V. M. (2015). The functional foetal brain: A systematic preview of methodological factors in reporting foetal visual and auditory capacity. Developmental Cognitive Neuroscience, 13, 4352.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Dweck, C. S. (2000). Self-theories: Their role in motivation, personality and development. Philadelphia, PA: Psychology Press.Google ScholarPubMed
Dys, S. P., Schellenberg, E. G., & McLean, K. C. (2017). Musical identities, music preferences, and individual differences. In MacDonald, R. A. R, Hargreaves, D. J, & Miell, D. E (Eds.), Handbook of musical identities (pp. 247266). Oxford: Oxford University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Eccles, J. S., Wigfield, A., & Schiefele, U. (1998). Motivation to succeed. In Eisenberg, N. (Ed.), Handbook of child psychology: Volume 3. Social, emotional and personality development (5th edition) (pp. 10171095). New York: Wiley.Google Scholar
Eccles, J., Adler, T. F., Futterman, R., Goff, S. B., Kaczala, C. M., Meece, J. L., & Midgley, C. (1983). Expectancy, values, and academic behaviours. In Spence, J. T (Ed.), Achievement and achievement motives: Psychological and sociological motives (pp. 75146). San Francisco: Freeman.Google Scholar
Einarson, K. M., & Trainor, L. J. (2015). The effect of visual information on young children’s perceptual sensitivity to musical beat alignment. Timing & Time Perception, 3(1–2), 88101.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Einarson, K. M., & Trainor, L. J. (2016). Hearing the beat: Young children’s perceptual sensitivity to beat alignment varies according to metric structure. Music Perception, 34(1), 5670.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Eitan, Z., & Timmers, R. (2010). Beethoven’s last piano sonata and those who follow crocodiles: Cross-domain mappings of auditory pitch in a musical context. Cognition, 114, 405422.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
El Haj, M., Postal, V., & Allain, P. (2012). Music enhances autobiographical memory in mild Alzheimer’s disease. Educational Gerontology, 38, 3041.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Elder, G. H., & Giele, J. Z. (2009). The craft of life course research. New York: Guilford Press.Google Scholar
Elliott, D. J., & Silverman, M. (2015). Music matters: A philosophy of music education (2nd edition). New York: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Elliott, D. J., & Silverman, M. (2017). Identities and musics: Reclaiming personhood. In MacDonald, R. A. R, Hargreaves, D. J, & Miell, D. E (Eds.), Handbook of musical identities (pp. 2745). Oxford: Oxford University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Elpus, K. (2017). Music education promotes lifelong engagement with the arts. Psychology of Music. Online First, DOI: 10.1177/0305735617697508.CrossRef
Elvers, P., Omigie, D., Fuhrmann, W., & Fischinger, T. (2015). Exploring the musical taste of expert listeners: musicology students reveal tendency toward omnivorous taste. Frontiers in Psychology, 6. DOI: 10.3389/fpsyg.2015.01252.CrossRef
Engeström, Y. (1987). Learning by expanding: An activity theoretical approach to developmental research. Helsinki: Orienta-Konsultit Oy.Google Scholar
Engeström, Y. (2001). Expansive learning at work: Toward an activity theory reconceptualisation. Journal of Education and Work, 14, 133161.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Engeström, Y., & Miettinen, R. (1999). Introduction, and activity theory and individual and social transformation. In Engeström, Y., Miettinen, R., & Punamäki, R. L (Eds.), Perspectives on activity theory. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Ericsson, K. A., & Smith, J. (Eds.) (1991). Toward a general theory of expertise: Prospects and limits. New York: Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
Ericsson, K. A., Krampe, R., & Tesch-Römer, C. (1993). The role of deliberate practice in the acquisition of expert performance. Psychological Review, 100, 363406.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Erikson, E. H. (1950).Childhood and society.New York: Norton.Google Scholar
Erikson, E. H. (1968). Identity, youth, and crisis. New York: Norton.Google Scholar
Erikson, E. H. (1982). The life cycle completed: A review. New York: W. W. Norton & Company, Inc.Google Scholar
Eriksson, H., Harmat, L., Theorell, T., & Ullén, F. (2016). Similar but different: Interviewing monozygotic twins discordant for musical practice. Musicae Scientiae, Online First, DOI: 10.1177/1029864916649791.CrossRef
Evans, P. (2015). Self-determination theory: An approach to motivation in music education. Musicae Scientiae, 19(1), 6583.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Evans, P. (2016). Motivation. In McPherson, G. E (Ed.), The child as musician: A handbook of musical development (2nd edition, pp. 325339). Oxford: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Evans, P., & Bonneville-Roussy, A. (2016). Self-determined motivation for practice in university music students. Psychology of Music, 44(5), 10951110.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Evans, P., & McPherson, G. E. (2017). Processes of musical identity consolidation during adolescence. In MacDonald, R. A. R, Hargreaves, D. J, & Miell, D. E (Eds.), Handbook of musical identities (pp. 213231). Oxford: Oxford University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Evans, P., McPherson, G. E., & Davidson, J. W. (2013). The role of psychological needs in ceasing music and music learning activities. Psychology of Music, 41(5), 598617.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Everett, W. (2001). The Beatles as musicians: The quarry men through rubber soul. New York: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Eysenck, M. W. (Ed.) (1998). Psychology: An integrated approach. Harlow, Essex: Addison-Wesley-Longman.Google Scholar
Fagen, J., Prigot, J., Carroll, M., Pioli, L., Stein, A., & Franco, A. (1997). Auditory context and memory retrieval in young infants. Child Development, 68(6), 10571066.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Fancourt, D. (2016). An introduction to the psychoneuroimmunology of music: History, future collaboration and a research agenda. Psychology of Music, 44(2), 168182.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Fancourt, D., Ockelford, A., & Abebech Belai, A. (2014). The psychoneuroimmunological effects of music: A systematic review and a new model. Brain, Behavior and Immunity, 36, 1526.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Fancourt, D., Williamon, A., Carvalho, L. A., Steptoe, A., Dow, R., & Lewis, I. (2016). Singing modulates mood, stress, cortisol, cytokine and neuropeptide activity in cancer patients and carers. eCancer, 10, 631, DOI: 10.3332/ecancer.2016.631.
Farnsworth, P. R. (1954). The social psychology of music. Ames: Iowa State University Press.Google Scholar
Farnsworth, P. R. (1969). The social psychology of music (2nd edition). Ames: Iowa State University Press.Google Scholar
Farrell, G. (2001). India. In Hargreaves, D. J & North, A. C (Eds.), Musical development and learning: The international perspective (pp. 5672). London: Continuum.Google Scholar
Faulkner, R., & Davidson, J. W. (2004). Men’s vocal behaviour and the construction of self. Musicae Scientiae, 8(2), 231255.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Feldman, D. H. with Goldsmith, L. T. (1986). Nature’s gambit: Child prodigies and the development of human potential. New York: Basic Books.Google Scholar
Fenigstein, A., Scheier, M. F., & Buss, A. H. (1975). Public and private self-consciousness: Assessment and theory. Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology, 43, 522527.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Finnegan, R. (1989). The hidden musicians: Music-making in an English town. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
Fishbein, M., & Ajzen, I. (1975). Belief, attitude, intention, and behavior: An introduction to theory and research. Reading: Addison-Wesley.Google Scholar
Flavell, J. H. (1977). Cognitive development. Englewood Cliffs, NJ: Prentice-Hall.Google Scholar
Flavell, J. H. (1979). Metacognition and cognitive monitoring. American Psychologist, 34(10), 906911.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Flecha, R. (2000). Sharing words: Theory and practice of dialogic learning. Lanham, MD: Rowman & Littlefield.Google Scholar
Flohr, J. (1985). Young children’s improvisations: Emerging creative thought. Creative Child and Adult Quarterly, 10, 7985.Google Scholar
Flom, R., Gentile, D. A., & Pick, A. D. (2008). Infants’ discrimination of happy and sad music. Infant Behavior and Development, 31, 716728.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Folkestad, G. (2002). National identity and music. In MacDonald, R. A. R, Hargreaves, D. J, & Miell, D. (Eds.), Musical identities (pp. 151162). Oxford: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Folkestad, G. (2005). Here, there and everywhere: Music education research in a globalised world. Music Education Research, 7, 279287.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Folkestad, G. (2006). Formal and informal learning situations or practices vs. formal and informal ways of learning. British Journal of Music Education, 23, 135145.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Folkestad, G. (2012). Digital tools and discourse in music: The ecology of composition. In Hargreaves, D. J., Miell, D. E, & MacDonald, R. A. R. (Eds.), Musical imaginations (pp. 193205). Oxford: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Folkestad, G., Lindström, B., & Hargreaves, D. J. (1997). Young people’s music in the digital age. Research Studies in Music Education, 9, 112.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Folkestad, G., Hargreaves, D. J., & Lindström, B. (1998). Compositional strategies in computer-based music-making. British Journal of Music Education, 15(1), 8398.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Fox, W. S., & Wince, M. H. (1975). Musical taste cultures and taste publics. Youth and Society, 7, 198224.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Franco, F., Chew, M., & Swaine, J. (2017). Preschoolers’ attribution of affect to music: A comparison between vocal and instrumental performance. Psychology of Music, 45(1), 131149.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Frankl, V. E. (2004). Man’s search for meaning: The classic tribute to hope from the Holocaust. New York: Rider.Google Scholar
Fredrickson, B. L. (2001). The role of positive emotions in positive psychology: The broaden and build theory of positive emotions. American Psychologist, 56(3), 218226.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Freeston, M. H., Rhéaume, J., Letarte, H., Dugas, M. J., & Ladouceur, R. (1994). Why do people worry? Journal of Personality and Individual Differences, 17, 791802.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Freire, P. (1973). Education for critical consciousness. New York: Continuum.Google Scholar
Frith, S. (1978). The sociology of rock. London: Constable.Google Scholar
Frith, S. (1981). Sound effects: Youth, leisure, and the politics of rock ‘n’ roll. New York: Pantheon.Google Scholar
Frith, S. (1990). Facing the music: Essays on pop, rock and culture. London: Mandarin.Google Scholar
Frith, S. (1996). Performing rites. Oxford: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Fritz, B. S., & Avsec, A. (2007). The experience of flow and subjective well-being of music students. Horizons of Psychology, 16, 517.Google Scholar
Fung, C. V., & Gromko, J. E. (2001). Effects of active versus passive listening on the quality of children’s invented notations and preferences for two pieces from an unfamiliar culture. Psychology of Music, 29(2), 128138.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Furnham, A., & Bradley, A. (1997). Music while you work: The differential distraction of background music on the cognitive test performance of introverts and extraverts. Applied Cognitive Psychology, 11, 445455.3.0.CO;2-R>CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Gabrielsson, A. (2010). Strong experiences with music. In Juslin, P. N. & Sloboda, J. A (Eds.), Handbook of music and emotion: Theory, research, applications (pp. 547574). Oxford: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Gabrielsson, A. (2011). Strong experiences with music: Music is much more than just music. Oxford: Oxford University Press (translation of Gabrielsson, A., 2008, Starka musikupplevelser – Musik är mycket mer än bara music, Hedemora: Gidlunds).CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Gaertner, S. L., Dovidio, J. F., Anastasio, P. A., Bachman, B. A., & Rust, M. C. (1993). The common ingroup identity model: Recategorization and the reduction of intergroup bias. In Stroebe, W. & Hewstone, M. (Eds.), The European Review of Social Psychology (vol. 4, pp. 126). Chichester: Wiley.Google Scholar
Gagné, F. (2009). Building gifts into talents: Detailed overview of the DMGT 2.0. In MacFarlane, B. & Stambaugh, T. (Eds.), Leading change in gifted education: The Festschrift of Dr Joyce Van Tassel–Basca (pp. 6180). Waco, TX: Prufrock Press.Google Scholar
Gall, M., & Breeze, N. (2008). Music and eJay: An opportunity for creative collaborations in the classroom. International Journal of Educational Research, 47(1), 2740.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
GalvinIII, J. J., Fu, Q.-J., & Oba, S. (2008). Effect of instrument timbre on melodic contour identification by cochlear implant users. Journal of the Acoustical Society of America, 124(4), EL189EL195.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Gans, H. J. (1974). Popular culture and high culture: An analysis and evaluation of taste. New York: Basic Books.Google Scholar
Garber, J., & Seligman, M. E. P. (1980). Human helpnessness: Theory and applications. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
Garcia, R. L., & Hand, C. J. (2016). Analgesic effects of self-chosen music type on cold pressor-induced pain: Motivating vs. relaxing music. Psychology of Music, 44(5), 967–983.CrossRef
Gardiner, M. F., Fox, A., Knowles, F., & Jeffrey, D. (1996). Learning improved by arts training. Nature, 381(6580), 284.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Gardner, H. (1970). Children’s sensitivity to painting styles. Child Development, 41, 813821.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Gardner, H. (1973a). Children’s sensitivity to musical styles. Merrill-Palmer Quarterly, 19, 6777.Google Scholar
Gardner, H. (1973b). The arts and human development. New York: John Wiley.Google Scholar
Gardner, H. (1979). Development psychology after Piaget: An approach in terms of symbolisation. Human Development, 22, 7388.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Gardner, H. (1983). Frames of mind: The theory of multiple intelligences. New York: Basic Books.Google Scholar
Gardner, H. (1987). The mind’s new science: A history of the cognitive revolution. New York: Basic Books.Google Scholar
Gardner, H. (1989). Zero-based arts education: An introduction to Arts Propel. Studies in Art Education, 30, 7183.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Gardner, H. (1993). Multiple intelligences: The theory in practice. New York: Basic Books.Google Scholar
Gardner, H. (1999). Intelligence reframed: Multiple intelligences for the 21st century. New York: Basic Books.Google Scholar
Gardner, H. (2003). MI after 20 years. Paper presented at the American Educational Research Association, Chicago.
Gardner, H., & Gardner, J. (1971). Children’s literary skills. Journal of Experimental Education, 39, 4246.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Gaser, C., & Schlaug, G. (2003). Brain structures differ between musicians and non-musicians. The Journal of Neuroscience, 23(27), 92409245.Google ScholarPubMed
Gaunt, H., & Westerlund, H. (Eds.) (2013). Collaborative learning in higher music education. Abingdon: Ashgate.Google Scholar
Gembris, H. (2012). Music-making as a lifelong development and resource for health. In MacDonald, R. A. R, Kreutz, G., & Mitchell, L. (Eds.), Music, health, and wellbeing (pp. 367382). Oxford: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Gembris, H., & Schellberg, G. (2003). Musical preferences of elementary school children (p. 324). Abstracts of the 5th ESCOM conference, University of Hanover, Germany.Google Scholar
Gentile, D. (1998). Infants’ discrimination of musical affective expressiveness. Unpublished doctoral dissertation. University of Minnesota.
Gergen, K. J., & Davis, K. E. (1985). The social construction of the person. New York: Springer-Verlag.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Gerry, D. W., Faux, A. L., & Trainor, L. J. (2010). Effects of Kindermusik training on infants’ rhythmic enculturation. Developmental Science, 13(3), 545551.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Gerry, D. W., Unrau, A., & Trainor, L. J. (2012). Active music classes in infancy enhance musical, communicative and social development. Developmental Science, 15(3), 398407.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Gerson, S. A., Schiavio, A., Timmers, R., & Hunnius, S. (2015). Active drumming experience increases infants’ sensitivity to audiovisual synchrony during observed drumming actions. PLoS One, 10(6), e0130960, DOI: 10.1371/journal.pone.0130960.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Getz, L., Marks, S., & Roy, M. (2014). The influence of stress, optimism, and music training on music uses and preferences. Psychology of Music, 42(1), 7185.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Ghiselin, B. (Ed.) (1952). The creative process. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.
Gibson, E. J. (1969). Principles of perceptual learning and development. New York: Appleton-Century-Crofts.Google Scholar
Gibson, J. J. (1966). The senses considered as perceptual systems. Boston: Houghton Mifflin.
Gibson, J. J. (1979/2015). The ecological approach to visual perception. New York: Psychology Press.Google Scholar
Giddens, A. (1991). Modernity and self-identity: Self and society in the late modern age. Cambridge: Polity Press.Google Scholar
Gilder, E., & Port, J. G. (1978). The dictionary of composers and their music. New York: Ballantine.
Giles, D. C., Pietrzykowski, S., & Clark, K. E. (2007). The psychological meaning of personal record collections and the impact of changing technological forms. Journal of Economic Psychology, 28, 429443.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Gillham, J. E., Jaycox, L. H., Reivich, K. J., Seligman, M. E. P., & Silver, T. (1990). The Penn Resiliency Program. Philadelphia, PA: University of Pennsylvania.Google ScholarPubMed
Glover, J. (2000). Children composing 4–14. London: Routledge.Google Scholar
Gogate, L. J., Walker-Andrews, A. S., & Bahrick, L. E. (2001). The intersensory origins of word comprehension: An ecological-dynamic systems view. Developmental Science, 4, 118.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Goldman, S. L., Whitney-Saltiel, D., Granger, J., & Rodin, J. (1991). Children’s representations of ‘everyday’ aspects of health and illness. Journal of Pediatric Psychology, 16, 747766.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Gordon, E. E. (1965). Musical aptitude profile. Boston, MA: Houghton Mifflin.Google Scholar
Gordon, E. E. (1976). Learning sequence and patterns in music. Buffalo, NY: Tometic Associates Ltd.Google Scholar
Gordon, E. E. (1997). A music learning theory for newborn and young children. Chicago, IL: GIA Publications, Inc.Google Scholar
Gordon, E. E. (2007). Learning sequences in music: A contemporary music learning theory, 2007 edition. Chicago, IL: GIA publications, Inc.Google Scholar
Goswami, U. (2008). Cognitive development: The learning brain. Hove: Psychology Press.Google Scholar
Goswami, U., & Bryant, P. E. (2010). Children’s cognitive development and learning. In Alexander, R. J, Doddington, C., Gray, J., Hargreaves, L., & Kershner, R. (Eds.), The Cambridge primary review research surveys (pp. 141169). London: Routledge.Google Scholar
Gould, S. J. (1985). Ontogeny and phylogeny. Boston, MA: Belknap Press.Google ScholarPubMed
Granier-Deferre, C., Bassereau, S., Ribeiro, A., Jacquet, A. -Y., & DeCasper, A. J. (2011) A melodic contour repeatedly experienced by human near-term fetuses elicits a profound cardiac reaction one month after birth. PLoS ONE, 6(2): e17304, DOI: 10.1371/journal.pone.0017304.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Grape, C., Wikström, B.-M., Ekman, R., Hasson, D., & Theorell, T. (2010). Comparison between choir singing and group discussion in irritable bowel syndrome patients over one year: Saliva testosterone increases in new choir singers. Psychotherapy and Psychosomatics, 79, 196198.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Greasley, A. E. (2008). Engagement with music in everyday life: An in-depth study of adults’ musical preferences and listening behaviours. Unpublished PhD thesis, Keele University.
Greasley, A. E., & Lamont, A. (2011). Exploring engagement with music in everyday life using experience sampling methodology, Musicae Scientiae, 15(1), 4571.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Greasley, A. E., & Lamont, A. (2013). Keeping it fresh: How listeners regulate their own exposure to familiar music. In King, E. & Prior, H. (Eds.), Music and familiarity: Listening, musicology and performance (pp. 1331). Basingstoke: Ashgate.Google Scholar
Greasley, A. E., & Lamont, A. (2016). Musical preferences. In Hallam, S., Cross, I., & Thaut, M. (Eds.), The Oxford handbook of music psychology (2nd edition, pp. 263281). Oxford: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Greasley, A. E., Lamont, A., & Sloboda, J. A. (2013). Exploring musical preferences: An in-depth study of adults’ liking for music in their personal collections. Qualitative Research in Psychology, 10(4), 402427.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Green, L. (2002). How popular musicians learn: A way ahead for music education. Aldershot: Ashgate.Google Scholar
Green, L. (2005). Meaning, autonomy and authenticity in the music classroom: Professorial lecture. Institute of Education, University of London.Google Scholar
Green, L. (2008). Music, informal learning and the school: A new classroom pedagogy. Aldershot: Ashgate.Google Scholar
Greenberg, D. M., & Rentfrow, P. J. (2017). The psychological underpinnings and manifestations of musical identities. In MacDonald, R. A. R, Hargreaves, D. J, & Miell, D. E (Eds.), Handbook of musical identities (pp. 304321). Oxford: Oxford University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Gregory, A. H. (1997). The roles of music in society: The ethnomusicological perspective. In Hargreaves, D. J & North, A. C (Eds.), The social psychology of music (pp. 123140). Oxford: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Groarke, J. M., & Hogan, M. J. (2016). Enhancing wellbeing: An emerging model of the adaptive functions of music listening. Psychology of Music, 44(4), 769791.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Gudmundsdottir, H. (1999). Children’s auditory discrimination of simultaneous melodies. Journal of Research in Music Education, 47(2), 101110.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Haake, A. B. (2011). Individual music listening in workplace settings: An exploratory survey of offices in the UK. Musicae Scientiae, 15(1), 107129.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Habermas, J. (1987). The theory of communicative action. Vols. 1 and 2. Boston, MA: Beacon.
Habibi, A., Cahn, B. R., Damasio, A., & Damasio, H. (2016). Neural correlates of accelerated auditory processing in children engaged in music training. Developmental Cognitive Neuroscience, 21, 114.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Hadwin, A. F., Järvelä, S., & Miller, M. (2011). Self-regulated, co-regulated and socially shared regulation of learning. In Zimmerman, B. J & Schunk, D. H (Eds.), Handbook of self-regulation of learning and performance. Abingdon: Routledge.Google Scholar
Hallam, S. (1997). What do we know about practising? Towards a model synthesizing the research literature. In Jørgensen, H. & Lehmann, A. (Eds.), Does practice make perfect? (pp. 179231). Oslo: Norges Musikkhøgskole.Google Scholar
Hallam, S. (2002). Musical motivation: Towards a model synthesising the research. Music Education Research, 4, 225244.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Hallam, S. (2010). The power of music: Its impact on the intellectual, social and personal development of children and young people. International Journal of Music Education, 28(3), 269289.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Hallam, S. (2015). The power of music: A research synthesis of the impact of actively making music on the intellectual, social and personal development of children and young people. London: Music Education Council, retrieved from www.mec.org.uk.
Hallam, S. (2016). Musicality. In McPherson, G. E (Ed.), The child as musician: A handbook of musical development (2nd edition, pp. 6780). Oxford: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Hallam, S., & Creech, A. (2016). Can active music making promote health and well-being in older citizens? Findings of the Music for Life Project, London Journal of Primary Care, DOI: 10.1080/17571472.2016.1152099.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Hallam, S., Price, J., & Katsarou, G. (2002). The effects of background music on primary school pupils’ task performance. Educational Studies, 28(2), 111122.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Hallam, S., Creech, A., Sandford, C., Rinta, T., & Shave, K. (2008). Survey of Musical Futures: A report from Institute of Education University of London, http://eprints.ioe.ac.uk/2301/1/Hallam2008_Musical_Futures_report_final_version.pdf
Hallett, R., & Lamont, A. (2015). How do gym members engage with music during exercise? Qualitative Research in Sport, Exercise and Health, 7, 411427.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Halpern, A. R., & Bartlett, J. C. (2002). Aging and memory for music: A review. Psychomusicology, 18, 1027.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Halpern, A. R., & Zatorre, R. J. (1999). When that tune runs through your head: A PET investigation of auditory imagery for familiar melodies. Cerebral Cortex, 9, 697704.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Halpern, A. R., Bartlett, J. C., & Dowling, W. J. (1995). Aging and experience in the recognition of musical transpositions. Psychology and Aging, 10(3), 325342.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Halpern, A. R., Kwak, S-Y., Bartlett, J. C., & Dowling, W. J. (1996). Effects of aging and musical experience on the representation of tonal hierarchies. Psychology and Aging, 11(2), 235246.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Hambrick, D. Z., Oswald, F. L., Altmann, E. M., Meinz, E. J., Gobet, F., & Campitelli, G. (2014). Deliberate practice: Is that all it takes to become an expert? Intelligence, 45, 34–45.
Hannon, E. E., & Trehub, S. E. (2005a). Metrical categories in infancy and adulthood. Psychological Science, 16, 4855.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Hannon, E. E., & Trehub, S.E. (2005b). Tuning in to musical rhythms: Infants learn more readily than adults. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences, 102(35), 1263912643CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Hansen, C. H., & Hansen, R. D. (1991). Constructing personality and social reality through music: Individual differences among fans of punk and heavy metal music. Journal of Broadcasting and Electronic Media, 35, 355350.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Hargreaves, D. J. (1982). The development of aesthetic reactions to music. Psychology of Music, Special Issue, 5154.Google Scholar
Hargreaves, , D. J. (1986a). The developmental psychology of music. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Hargreaves, D. J. (1986b). Developmental psychology and music education. Psychology of Music, 14, 8396.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Hargreaves, D. J. (Ed.) (1989). Children and the arts. Milton Keynes: Open University Press.Google Scholar
Hargreaves, D. J. (1994). Musical education for all: Open peer commentary on ‘Is everyone musical?’, by Sloboda, J. A., Davidson, J. W., & Howe, M. J. A.. The Psychologist, 7(8), 357358.
Hargreaves, D. J. (1996). The development of musical and artistic competence. In Deliège, I. & Sloboda, J. A. (Eds.), Musical beginnings: Origins and development of musical competence (pp. 145170). Oxford: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Hargreaves, D. J. (2008). Commentary. Special issue of the International Journal of Educational Research. In D. Miell, K. Littleton, & S. Rojas-Drummond (Eds.), Music education: A site for collaborative creativity, 47, 75–77.CrossRef
Hargreaves, D. J. (2012). Musical imagination: Perception and production, beauty and creativity. Psychology of Music, 40(5), 539–57.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Hargreaves, D. J., & Bonneville-Roussy, A. (2017). What is ‘open-earedness’, and how can it be measured? Musicae Scientiae. Online First, DOI: 10.1177/1029864917697783.CrossRef
Hargreaves, D. J., & Galton, M. (1992). Aesthetic learning: Psychological theory and educational practice. In Reimer, B. & Smith, R. A (Eds.), National Society for the Study of Education yearbook on the arts in education (pp. 124150). Chicago, IL: NSSE.Google Scholar
Hargreaves, D. J., & Marshall, N. A. (2003). Developing identities in music education. Music Education Research, 5(3), 263273.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Hargreaves, D. J., & North, A. C. (Eds.) (1997). The social psychology of music. Oxford: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Hargreaves, D. J., & North, A. C. (1999). Developing concepts of musical style. Musicae Scientiae, 3(2), 193216.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Hargreaves, D. J., & North, A. C. (Eds.) (2001). Musical development and learning: The international perspective. London & New York: Continuum.Google Scholar
Hargreaves, D. J., & Zimmerman, M. (1992). Developmental theories of music learning. In Colwell, R. (Ed.), Handbook for research in music teaching and learning (pp. 377391). New York: Schirmer / Macmillan.Google Scholar
Hargreaves, D. J., Comber, C. J. F., & Colley, A. M. (1995). Effects of age, gender, and training on the musical preferences of British secondary school students. Journal of Research in Music Education, 43(3), 242250.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Hargreaves, D. J., Cork, C. A., & Setton, T. (1991). Cognitive strategies in jazz improvisation: An exploratory study. Canadian Music Educators Journal, 33, 4754.Google Scholar
Hargreaves, D. J., Galton, M., & Robinson, S. (1996). Teachers’ assessments of primary children’s classroom work in the creative arts. Educational Research, 38, 199–211.CrossRef
Hargreaves, D. J., Hargreaves, J. J., & North, A. C. (2012). Imagination and creativity in music listening. In Hargreaves, D. J., Miell, D. E, & MacDonald, R. A. R (Eds.), Musical imaginations (pp. 156–72). Oxford: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Hargreaves, D. J., Jones, P., & Martin, D. (1981). The air gap phenomenon in children’s landscape drawings. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 32, 1120.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Hargreaves, D. J., MacDonald, R. A. R., & Miell, D. E. (2002 ). What are musical identities, and why are they important? In MacDonald, R. A. R, Hargreaves, D. J, & Miell, D. E (Eds.), Musical identities (pp. 120). Oxford: Oxford University Press.Google ScholarPubMed
Hargreaves, D. J., MacDonald, R. A. R., & Miell, D. E. (2005). How do people communicate using music? In Miell, D. E., MacDonald, R. A. R, & Hargreaves, D. J (Eds.), Musical communication (pp. 125). Oxford: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Hargreaves, D. J., MacDonald, R. A. R., & Miell, D.E (2012a). Musical identities mediate musical development. In G. McPherson and G. Welch (Eds.), The Oxford handbook of music education (pp. 125–142). Oxford: Oxford University Press.
Hargreaves, D. J., MacDonald, R. A. R., & Miell, D. E. (2012b). Explaining musical imaginations: Creativity, performance and perception. In Hargreaves, D. J, Miell, D. E, & MacDonald, R. A. R (Eds.), Musical imaginations (pp. 114). Oxford: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Hargreaves, D. J., MacDonald, R. A. R., & Miell, D. E. (2017). The changing identity of musical identities. In MacDonald, R. A. R, Hargreaves, D. J, & Miell, D. E (Eds.), Handbook of musical identities (pp. 323). Oxford: Oxford University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Hargreaves, D. J., Marshall, N., & North, A. C. (2003). Music education in the 21st century: A psychological perspective. British Journal of Music Education, 20(2), 147163.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Hargreaves, D. J., North, A. C., & Tarrant, M. (2016). How and why do musical preferences change during childhood and adolescence? In McPherson, G. (Ed.), The child as musician: A handbook of musical development (pp. 303322). Oxford: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Hargreaves, D. J., Purves, R. M., Welch, G. F., & Marshall, N. A. (2007). Developing identities and attitudes in musicians and music teachers. British Journal of Educational Psychology, 77(3), 665682.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Hargreaves, D. J., Welch, G. F., Purves, R., & Marshall, N. (2003). Effective teaching in secondary school music: teacher and pupil identities. End of Award Report, ESRC award R00022375l.
Harkins, C., Garnham, L., Campbell, A., & Tannahill, C. (2016). Hitting the right note for child and adolescent mental and emotional wellbeing: A formative qualitative evaluation of Sistema Scotland’s “Big Noise” orchestral programme. Journal of Public Mental Health, 15(1), 2536.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Harland, J., Kinder, K., Lord, P., Stott, A., Schagen, I., Haynes, J., Cusworth, L., White, R., & Paola, R. (2000). Arts education in secondary schools: Effects and effectiveness. Slough: NFER.Google Scholar
Harris, K. C., Mills, J. H., He, N.-J., & Dubno, J. R. (2008). Age-related differences in sensitivity to small changes in frequency assessed with cortical evoked potentials. Hearing Research, 243, 4756.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Harris, , P. L. (1989). Children and emotion: The development of psychological understanding. Oxford: Basil Blackwell.Google Scholar
Harter, S. (1999). The construction of the self: A developmental perspective. New York: Guilford Press.Google Scholar
Hayes, J. R. (1989). The complete problem solver (2nd edition). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.Google Scholar
Hays, T., & Minichiello, V. (2005). The meaning of music in the lives of older people: A qualitative study. Psychology of Music, 33(4), 437451.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
He, C., Hotson, L., & Trainor, L. J. (2007). Mismatch responses to pitch changes in early infancy. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 19, 878892.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
He, C., Hotson, L., & Trainor, L. J. (2009). Maturation of cortical mismatch responses to occasional pitch change in early infancy: Effects of presentation rate and magnitude of change. Neuropsychologia, 47, 218229.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
He, N.-J., Mills, J. H., & Dubno, J. R. (2007). Frequency modulation detection: Effects of age, psychophysical method, and modulation waveform. Journal of the Acoustical Society of America, 122(1), 467477.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Heath, S. B. (2001). Three’s not a crowd: Plans, roles and focus in the arts. Educational Researcher, 30, 1017.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Heaton, P. (2009). Assessing musical skills in autistic children who are not savants. Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society B, 364, 14431447.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Heaton, P., Hermelin, B., & Pring, L. (1998). Autism and pitch processing: A precursor for savant musical ability. Music Perception, 15, 291305.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Heider, F. (1958). The psychology of interpersonal relationships. New York: Wiley.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Hekkert, P., & van Wieringen, P. C. W. (1990). Complexity and prototypicality as determinants of the appraisal of cubist paintings. British Journal of Psychology, 81(4), 483495.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Hemming, J. (2013). Is there a peak in popular music preference at a certain song-specific age? A replication of Holbrook & Schindler’s 1989 study. Musicae Scientiae, 17(3), 293304.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Henley, D. (2011). Music education in England: A review by Darren Henley for the Department of Education and the Department for Culture, Media and Sport. www.educationengland.org.uk/documents/pdfs/2011-music-henley-review.pdf
Hentschke, L., & Del Ben, L. (1999). The assessment of audience-listening: Testing a model in the educational setting of Brazil. Music Education Research, 1(2), 127146.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Hepper, P. G. (1991). An examination of fetal learning before and after birth. Irish Journal of Psychology, 12, 95107CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Hepper, P. G. (1996). Fetal memory: Does it exist? What does it do? Acta Paediatrica Supplement, 416, 1620.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Herholz, S. C., Halpern, A. R., & Zatorre, R. J. (2012). Neuronal correlates of perception, imagery, and memory for familiar tunes. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 24(6), 13821397.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Herrera, L., Lorenzo, O., Defior, S., Fernandez-Smith, G., & Costa-Giomi, E. (2011). Effects of phonological and musical training on the reading readiness of native- and foreign-Spanish-speaking children. Psychology of Music, 39(1), 6881.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Hetland, L., & Winner, E. (2000). The arts and academic achievement: What the evidence shows. The Journal of Aesthetic Education, 34(3/4), 310.Google Scholar
Hewitt, A. (2008). Children’s creative collaboration during a computer-based music task. International Journal of Educational Research, 47 (1), 1126.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Hickey, M. (2001). An application of Amabile’s consensual assessment technique for rating the creativity of children’s musical compositions. Journal of Research in Music Education, 49(3), 234–244.CrossRef
Hickey, M., & Lipscomb, S. D. (2006). How different is good? How good is different? The assessment of children’s creative musical thinking. Musical Creativity: Multidisciplinary Research in Theory and Practice, 97–110.
Holahan, J., (1986). Teaching music through music learning theory: The contribution of Edwin E. Gordon. In Mark, M. (Ed.), Contemporary music education (pp. 152172). New York: Schirmer.Google Scholar
Holbrook, M. B. (1995). An empirical approach to representing patterns of consumer tastes, nostalgia, and hierarchy in the market for cultural products. Empirical Studies of the Arts, 13(1), 5571.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Holbrook, M. B., & Schindler, R. M. (1989). Some exploratory findings on the development of musical tastes. Journal of Consumer Research, 16(6), 119124.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Holbrook, M. B., & Schindler, R. M. (2013). Commentary on ‘Is there a peak in popular music preference at a certain song-specific age? A replication of Holbrook & Schindler’s 1989 study’. Musicae Scientiae, 17(3), 305308.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Honing, H. (2004). The comeback of systematic musicology: New empiricism and the cognitive revolution. Tijdschrift voor Muziektheorie [Dutch Journal of Music Theory], 9(3), 241244.Google Scholar
Hopper, M. J., Curtis, S., Hodge, S., & Simm, R. (2016). A qualitative study exploring the effects of attending a community pain service choir on wellbeing in people who experience chronic pain. British Journal of Pain, 10(3), 124134.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Howe, C., & Abedin, M. (2013). Classroom dialogue: A systematic review across four decades of research. Cambridge Journal of Education, 43(3), 325356.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Howe, M. J. A., Davidson, J. W., & Sloboda, J. A. (1998). Innate gifts and talents: Reality or myth? Behavioural and Brain Sciences, 21, 399407.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Howe, M. J. A., Davidson, J. W., Moore, D. M., & Sloboda, J. A. (1995). Are there early signs of musical excellence? Psychology of Music, 23, 162176.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Hruska, E., Hargreaves, D. J., & Ockelford, A. (submitted). Exploring parental and educational influences on the development of performance anxiety and perfectionism in classical musicians.
Huffman, J. C., Mastromauro, C. A., Boehm, J. K., Seabrook, R., Fricchione, G. L., Denninger, J. W., & Lyubomirsky, S. (2011). Development of a positive psychology intervention for patients with acute cardiovascular disease. Heart International, 6:e14, 4754.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Hunter, P. G., Schellenberg, E. G., & Stalinski, S. (2011). Liking and identifying emotionally expressive music: Age and gender differences. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 110, 8093.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Huron, D. (1990). Review of Music as cognition, by M. L. Serafine. Psychology of Music, 18, 99103.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Husserl, E. (1936/1970). The crises of the European sciences and transcendental phenomenology. D. Carr (Trans.). Evanston, IL: Northwestern University Press.Google Scholar
Hutchinson, J. C., Karageorghis, C. I., & Jones, L. (2015). See hear: Psychological effects of music and music-video during treadmill running. Annals of Behavioral Medicine, 49, 199211.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Hutka, S., Bidelman, G. M., & Moreno, S. (2015). Pitch expertise is not created equal: Cross-domain effects of musicianship and tone language experience on neural and behavioural discrimination of speech and music. Neuropsychologia, 71, 5263.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Hyde, K. L., & Peretz, I. (2004). Brains that are out of tune but in time. Psychological Science, 15(5), 356360.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Hyde, K. L., Lerch, J., Norton, A., Forgeard, M., Winner, E., Evans, A. C., & Schlaug, G. (2009). Musical training shapes structural brain development. The Journal of Neuroscience, 29(10), 30193025.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Ilari, B. (2016). Music in the early years: Pathways into the social world. Research Studies in Music Education, 38(1), 2339.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Ilari, B. (2017). Children’s ethnic identity, cultural diversity, and music education. In MacDonald, R. A. R, Hargreaves, D. J, & Miell, D. E (Eds.), Handbook of musical identities (pp. 527542). Oxford: Oxford University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Ilari, B., & Polka, I. (2006). Music cognition in early infancy: Infants’ preferences and long-term memory for Ravel. International Journal of Music Education, 24, 720.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Ilari, B., & Sundara, M. (2009). Music listening preferences in early life: Infants’ responses to accompanied versus unaccompanied listening. Journal of Research in Music Education, 56(4), 357369.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Ilari, B., & Young, S. (Eds.) (2016). Children’s home musical experiences across the world. Bloomington and Indianapolis: Indiana University Press.Google Scholar
Ivanov, V. K., & Geake, J. G. (2003). The Mozart Effect and primary school children. Psychology of Music, 31(4), 405413.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Jakubowski, K., Müllensiefen, D., & Stewart, L. (2017). A developmental study of latent absolute pitch memory. The Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 70(3), 434443.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
James, W. (1890/1950). The principles of psychology, vol.1. New York: Holt.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Janus, M., Lee, Y., Moreno, S., & Bialystok, E. (2016). Effects of short-term music and second-language training on executive control. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 144, 8497.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Jayawickreme, E., Forgeard, M. J. C., & Seligman, M. E. P. (2012). The engine of well-being. Review of General Psychology, 16(4), 327342.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Jentschke, S., & Koelsch, S. (2009). Musical training modulates the development of syntax processing in children. NeuroImage, 47, 735744.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Jentschke, S., Friederici, A. D., & Koelsch, S. (2014). Neural correlates of music-syntactic processing in two-year old children. Developmental Cognitive Neuroscience, 9, 200208.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Johansson, K. (2012). Organ improvisation: Edition, extemporisation, expansion and instant composition. In Hargreaves, D. J, Miell, D. E, & MacDonald, R. A. R. (Eds.), Musical imaginations (pp. 220232). Oxford: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
John, O. P., & Srivastava, S. (1999). The Big Five trait taxonomy: History, measurement, and theoretical perspectives. Handbook of personality: Theory and research, 2, 102138.Google Scholar
John-Steiner, V. (2000). Creative collaboration. New York: Oxford University Press.
Johnson, J. K., Louhivuori, J., & Siljander, E. (2016). Comparison of well-being of older adult choir singers and the general population in Finland: A case-control study. Musicae Scientiae, DOI: 10.1177/1029864916644486.Google ScholarPubMed
Johnson, J. K., Louhivuori, J., Stewart, A. L., Tolvanen, A., Ross, L., & Era, P. (2013). Quality of Life (QOL) of older adult community choral singers in Finland. International Psychogeriatrics, 25(7), 10551064.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Johnson-Laird, P. N. (1988a). Freedom and constraint in creativity. In: R. J. Sternberg (ed.), The nature of creativity (pp. 202–219). Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.
Johnson-Laird, P. N. (1988b). The computer and the mind: An introduction to cognitive science. Harvard University Press.
Jørgensen, H., & Lehmann, A. C. (Eds.) (1997). Does practice make perfect?: Current theory and research on instrumental music practice. Norges musikkhøgskole.Google Scholar
Joseph, R. (2000). Fetal brain behavior and cognitive development. Developmental Review, 20, 8198.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Jourdan, K., & Holloway, R. (2017). Sistema Scotland: Emerging musical identities in Raploch. In MacDonald, R. A. R, Hargreaves, D. J, & Miell, D. E (Eds.), Handbook of musical identities (pp. 768788). Oxford: Oxford University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Judd, M., & Pooley, J. A. (2014). The psychological benefits of participating in group singing for members of the general public. Psychology of Music, 42(2), 269283.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Juslin, P. N. (2013). From everyday emotions to aesthetic emotions: Toward a unified theory of musical emotions. Physics of Life Reviews, 10(3), 235266.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Juslin, P. N., & Laukka, P. (2004). Expression, perception, and induction of musical emotions: A review and a questionnaire study of everyday listening. New Music Research, 33(3), 217238.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Juslin, P. N., Liljeström, S., Västfjäll, D., Barradas, G., & Silva, A. (2008). An experience sampling study of emotional reactions to music: Listener, music, and situation. Emotion, 8(5), 668683.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Juuti, S., & Littleton, K. (2010). Musical identities in transition: Solo-piano students’ accounts of entering the academy. Psychology of Music, 38(4), 481497.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Kahneman, D., Diener, E., & Schwarz, N. (Eds.) (1999). Well-being: The foundations of hedonic psychology. New York: Russell Sage Foundation.Google Scholar
Kämpfe, J., Sedlmeier, P., & Renkewitz, F. (2011). The impact of background music on adult listeners: A meta-analysis. Psychology of Music, 39(4), 424448.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Karageorghis, C. I. (2016). The scientific application of music in exercise and sport: Towards a new theoretical model. In Lane, A. M. (Ed.), Sport and exercise psychology (2nd edition, pp. 276322). London, UK: Routledge.Google Scholar
Karageorghis, C. I., & Jones, L. (2014). On the stability and relevance of the exercise heart rate–music-tempo preference relationship. Psychology of Sport and Exercise, 15, 299310.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Karageorghis, C. I., Terry, P. C., & Lane, A. M. (1999). Development and initial validation of an instrument to assess the motivational qualities of music in exercise and sport: The Brunel Music Rating Inventory. Journal of Sports Sciences, 17, 713724.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Karageorghis, C. I., Jones, L., Priest, D. L., Akers, R. I., Clarke, A., Perry, J. M., et al. (2011). Revisiting the exercise heart rate-music tempo preference relationship. Research Quarterly for Exercise and Sport, 82, 274e284.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Karageorghis, C. I., Priest, D. L., Terry, P. C., Chatzisarantis, N. D., & Lane, A. M. (2006). Redesign and initial validation of an instrument to assess the motivational qualities of music in exercise: The Brunel Music Rating Inventory-2. Journal of Sports Sciences, 24, 899909.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Karlsen, S. (2011). Using musical agency as a lens: Researching music education from the angle of experience. Research Studies in Music Education, 33(2), 107121.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Kastner, M. P., & Crowder, R. G. (1990). Perception of the major/minor distinction: IV. Emotional connotations in young children. Music Perception, 8, 189202.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Kaufman, J. C., & Sternberg, R. J. (Eds.) (2006). The international handbook of creativity. Cambridge University Press.CrossRef
Keeler, J. R., Roth, E. A., Neuser, B. L., Spitsbergen, J. M., Waters, D. J. M., & Vianney, J.-M. (2015). The neurochemistry and social flow of singing: Bonding and oxytocin. Frontiers in Human Neuroscience, 9, 518, DOI: 10.3389/fnhum.2015.00518.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Kelley, H. H. (1973). The processes of causal attribution. American Psychologist, 28, 107128.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Kemp, A. E. (1981a). The personality structure of the musician: I. Identifying a profile of traits for the performer. Psychology of Music, 9, 314.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Kemp, A. E. (1981b). Personality differences between the players of string, woodwind, brass and keyboard instruments, and singers. Council for Research in Music Education Bulletin, 66, 33–8.Google Scholar
Kemp, A. E. (1982). Personality traits of successful student music teachers. Psychology of Music, Special Issue, 7275.Google Scholar
Kemp, A. E. (1996). The musical temperament: Psychology and personality of musicians. Oxford: Oxford University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Kenny, D. T. (2004). Music performance anxiety: Is it the music, the performance or the anxiety? Music Forum, 10(4), 3843.Google Scholar
Kenny, , D. T. (2006). A systematic review of treatments for music performance anxiety. Anxiety, Stress & Coping: An International Journal, 18(3), 183208.CrossRef
Kenny, D. T. (2011). The psychology of music performance anxiety. New York: Oxford University Press.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Kenny, D. T., & Osborne, M. S. (2006). Music performance anxiety: New insights from young musicians. Advances in Cognitive Psychology, 2(2–3), 103112.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Kirschner, S., & Tomasello, M. (2009). Joint drumming: Social context facilitates synchronization in preschool children. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 102, 299314.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Kirschner, S., & Tomasello, M. (2010). Joint music making promotes prosocial behavior in 4-year-old children. Evolution and Human Behavior, 31(5), 354364.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Kisilevsky, B. S., Hains, S. M., Jacquet, A.-Y., Granier-Deferre, C., & Lecanuet, J. P. (2004). Maturation of fetal responses to music. Developmental Science, 7, 550559.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Kisilevsky, B. S., Hains, S. M., Lee, K., Xie, X., Huang, H., et al. (2003). Effects of experience on fetal voice recognition. Psychological Science, 4, 220224.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Koelsch, S., Fritz, T., Schulze, K., Alsop, D., & Schlaug, G. (2005). Adults and children processing music: an fMRI study. Neuroimage, 25(4), 1068–1076.CrossRef
Koelsch, S., Gunter, T. C., Friederici, A. D., & Schröger, E. (2000). Brain indices of music processing: ‘nonmusicians’ are musical. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 12, 520541.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Koelsch, S., Grossman, T., Gunter, T. C., Hahne, A., Schröger, E., & Friederici, A. D. (2003). Children processing music: Electrical brain responses reveal musical competence and gender differences. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 15(5), 683693.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Koelsch, S., Gunter, T. C., Schröger, E., & Friederici, A. D. (2003). Processing tonal modulations: An ERP study. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 15(8), 11491159.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Kohn, D., & Eitan, Z. (2016). Moving music: Correspondences of musical parameters and movement dimensions in children’s motion and verbal responses. Music Perception, 34(1), 4055.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Koltko-Rivera, M. E. (2006). Rediscovering the later version of Maslow’s hierarchy of needs: Self-transcendence and opportunities for theory, research, and unification. Review of General Psychology, 10(4), 302317.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Konečni, V. J. (1982). Social interaction and music preference. In Deutsch, D. (Ed.), The psychology of music (1st edition, pp. 497516). New York: Academic Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Koniari, D., Predazzer, S., & Mélen, M. (2001). Categorization and schematization processes used in music perception by 10- to 11-year old children. Music Perception, 18(3), 297324.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Koopman, H. M., Baars, R. M., Chaplin, J., & Zwinderman, K. H. (2004). Illness through the eyes of the child: The development of children’s understanding of the causes of illness. Patient Education and Counseling, 55, 363370.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Kopiez, R., & Lehmann, M. (2008). The ‘open-earedness’ hypothesis and the development of age-related reactions to music in elementary school children. British Journal of Music Education, 25(2), 121138.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Kopiez, R., Lehmann, A. C., & Klassen, J. (2009). Clara Schumann’s collection of program leaflets: A historiometric analysis of life-span development, mobility, and repertoire canonization. Poetics, 37(1), 5073.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Kopp, C. B. (1982). Antecedents of self-regulation: A developmental perspective. Developmental Psychology, 18(2), 199214.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Koutsoupidou, T., & Hargreaves, D. J. (2009). An experimental study of the effects of improvisation on the development of creative thinking in music. Psychology of Music, 37(3), 251278.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Kratus, J. (1994). Relationships among children’s music audiation and their compositional processes and products. Journal of Research in Music Education, 42(2), 115130.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Kraus, N., Hornickel, J., Strait, D. L., Slater, J., & Thompson, E. (2014). Engagement in community music classes sparks neuroplasticity and language development in children from disadvantaged backgrounds. Frontiers in Psychology, 5, DOI: 10.3389/fpsyg.2014.01403.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Krause, A. E., & Hargreaves, D. J. (2013). myTunes: Digital music library users and their self-images. Psychology of Music, 41(5), 531544.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Krause, A. E., North, A. C., & Hewitt, L. Y. (2014). Music selection behaviors in everyday listening. Electronic Media, 58(2), 306323.Google Scholar
Krause, A. E., North, A. C., & Hewitt, L. Y. (2015). Music-listening in everyday life: Devices and choice. Psychology of Music, 43(2), 155270.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Krause, A. E., North, A. C., & Hewitt, L. Y. (2016). The role of location in everyday experiences of music. Psychology of Popular Media Culture, 5(3), 232257.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Kreutz, G., Bongard, S., Rohrmann, S., Grebe, D., Bastian, H. G., & Hodapp, V. (2004). Effects of choir singing or listening on secretory immunoglobulin: A, cortisol and emotional state. Journal of Behavioral Medicine, 27(6), 623635.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Krueger, C., Holditch-Davis, D., Quint, S., & DeCasper, A. J. (2004). Recurring auditory experience in the 28– to 34–week old fetus. Infant Behavior & Development, 27, 537543.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Krumhansl, C. L. (2010). Plink: “Thin slices” of music. Music Perception, 27(5), 337–354.CrossRef
Krumhansl, C. L., & Jusczyk, P. W. (1990). Infants’ perception of phrase structure in music. Psychological Science, 1(1), 7073.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Krumhansl, C. L., & Keil, F. C. (1982). Acquisition of the hierarchy of tonal functions in music. Memory and Cognition, 10, 243251.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Krumhansl, C. L., & Zupnick, J. A. (2013). Cascading reminiscence bumps in popular music. Psychological Science, 24, 20572068.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Kuhl, P. K., Tsao, F.-M., & Liu, H.-M. (2003). Foreign-language experience in infancy: Effects of short-term exposure and social interaction on phonetic learning. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences, 100(15), 90969101.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Labbé, C., & Grandjean, D. (2014). Musical emotions predicted by feelings of entrainment. Music Perception, 32(2), 170185.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Lamont, A. (1998). Music, education, and the development of pitch perception: The role of context, age, and musical experience. Psychology of Music, 26(1), 725.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Lamont, A. (1999). A contextual account of developing representations of music. Paper presented at the International Conference on Research in Music Education, Exeter University, UK.
Lamont, A. (2002). Musical identities and the school environment. In MacDonald, R. A. R, Hargreaves, D. J, & Miell, D. E (Eds.), Musical identities (pp. 4159). Oxford: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Lamont, A. (2006). What do monkeys’ music choices mean? Trends in Cognitive Science, 9(8), 359361.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Lamont, A. (2008). Young children’s musical worlds: Musical engagement in three-year-olds. Journal of Early Childhood Research. 6(3), 247261.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Lamont, A. (2011a). University students’ strong experiences of music: Pleasure, engagement and meaning. Musicae Scientiae, 15(2), 229249.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Lamont, A. (2011b). The beat goes on: Music education, identity and lifelong learning. Music Education Research, 13(4), 369388.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Lamont, A. (2012). Emotion, engagement and meaning in strong experiences of music performance. Psychology of Music, 40(5), 574594.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Lamont, A. (2016). Musical development from the early years onwards. In Hallam, S., Cross, I., & Thaut, M. (Eds.), The Oxford handbook of music psychology (2nd edition, pp. 399414). Oxford: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Lamont, A. (2017). Musical identity, interest, and involvement. In MacDonald, R. A. R, Hargreaves, D. J, & Miell, D. E (Eds.), Handbook of musical identities (pp. 176196). Oxford: Oxford University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Lamont, A., & Cross, I. (1994). Children’s cognitive representations of musical pitch. Music Perception, 12, 2755.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Lamont, A., & Maton, K. (2008). Choosing music: Exploratory studies into the low uptake of music GCSE. British Journal of Music Education, 25(3), 267282.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Lamont, A., & Maton, K. (2010). Unpopular music: Beliefs and behaviours towards music in education. In Wright, R. (Ed.), Sociology and music education (pp. 6380). Basingstoke: Ashgate.Google Scholar
Lamont, A., & Webb, R. J. (2010). Short- and long-term musical preferences: What makes a favourite piece of music? Psychology of Music, 38(2), 222241.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Lamont, A., Greasley, A. E., & Sloboda, J. A. (2016). Choosing to hear music: motivation, process and effect. In S. Hallam, I. Cross, & M. Thaut (Eds.), The Oxford handbook of music psychology (2nd edition, pp. 711–724), Oxford: Oxford University Press.
Lamont, A., Hargreaves, D. J., Marshall, N. A., & Tarrant, M. (2003). Young people’s music in and out of school. British Journal of Music Education, 20(3), 229241.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Lamont, A., Murray, M., Hale, R., & Wright-Bevans, K. (2017). Singing in later life: The anatomy of a choir. Psychology of Music, Online First, DOI: 10.1177/0305735617715514.CrossRef
Lane, A. M., Davis, P. A., & Devonport, T. J. (2011). Effects of music interventions on emotional states and running performance. Journal of Sports Science and Medicine, 10, 400407.Google ScholarPubMed
Lanovaz, M. J., & Huxley, S. C. (2017). Effects of background music on socially reinforced problem behaviors in children with autism spectrum disorders. Psychology of Music, 45(3), 450456.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Lartillot, O., & Toiviainen, P. (2007). MIR in Matlab (II): A toolbox for musical feature extraction from audio. In S. Dixon, D. Bainbridge, & R. Typke (Eds.), Proceedings of the 8th International Conference on Music Information Retrieval (pp. 237–244). Vienna, Austria: Osterreichische Computer Gesellschaft.
Laukka, P. (2007). Uses of music and psychological well-being among the elderly. Journal of Happiness Studies, 8, 215241.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Lave, J., & Wenger, E. (1991). Situated learning: Legitimate peripheral participation. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Layard, R., & Dunn, J. (2009). A good childhood – Searching for values in a competitive age. London: Penguin Books.Google Scholar
LeBlanc, A. (1991). Effect of maturation/aging on music listening preference: A review of the literature. Paper presented at the Ninth National Symposium on Research in Music Behavior, Cannon Beach, Oregon, USA, 7–9 March 1991.
Lee, H. L., & Noppeney, U. (2011). Long-term music training tunes how the brain temporally binds signals from multiple senses. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences, 108(51), 2029520296.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Lehman, H. C. (1953). Age and achievement. Princeton, NJ: Princeton University Press.Google ScholarPubMed
Lehmann, A. C. (1997). Acquired mental representations in music performance: Anecdotal and preliminary empirical evidence. In Jørgensen, H. & Lehmann, A. (Eds.), Does practice make perfect? (pp. 141164). Oslo: Norges Musikkhøgskole.Google Scholar
Lehmann, A. C., & Davidson, J. W. (2002). Taking an acquired skills perspective on music performance. In Colwell, R. & Richardson, C. (Eds.), Second handbook on music teaching and learning (pp. 542562). Oxford: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Lehmann, A. C., Sloboda, J. A., & Woody, R. H. (2007). Psychology for musicians: Understanding and acquiring the skills. Oxford: Oxford University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Leighton, G. L., & Lamont, A. (2006). Exploring children’s singing development: Do experiences in early schooling help or hinder? Music Education Research, 8(3), 311330.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Leont’ev, A.N. (1981). Problems of the development of mind. Moscow: Progress Publishers.Google Scholar
Lesiuk, T. (2005). The effect of music listening on work performance. Psychology of Music, 33(2), 173191.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Levitin, D. (1994). Absolute memory for musical pitch: Evidence from the production of learned melodies. Perception & Psychophysics, 52, 599608.Google Scholar
Lewis, K. (2011). In Harmony Lambeth – An evaluation. www.ihsc.org.uk/evaluation-Lambeth-2011.
Limb, C. J., & Braun, A. R. (2008). Neural substrates of spontaneous musical performance: An FMRI study of jazz improvisation. PLoS ONE, I, e1679.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Lindblom, A. (2017). ‘It gives them a place to be proud’ – Music and social inclusion. Two diverse cases of young First Nations people diagnosed with autism in British Columbia, Canada. Psychology of Music, 45(2), 268282.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Littleton, K., & Mercer, N. (2012). Communication, collaboration, and creativity: How musicians negotiate a collective ‘sound’. In Hargreaves, D. J., Miell, D. E, & MacDonald, R. A. R. (Eds.), Musical imaginations (pp. 233241). Oxford: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
London, J. (2004). Hearing in time: Psychological aspects of musical meter. Oxford: Oxford University Press.CrossRef