Skip to main content Accessibility help
×
Hostname: page-component-7bb8b95d7b-wpx69 Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-10-04T15:17:21.121Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

Bibliography

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  05 June 2014

Julie Tetel Andresen
Affiliation:
Duke University, North Carolina
Get access

Summary

Image of the first page of this content. For PDF version, please use the ‘Save PDF’ preceeding this image.'
Type
Chapter
Information
Linguistics and Evolution
A Developmental Approach
, pp. 281 - 297
Publisher: Cambridge University Press
Print publication year: 2013

Access options

Get access to the full version of this content by using one of the access options below. (Log in options will check for institutional or personal access. Content may require purchase if you do not have access.)

References

Aarsleff, H. 1976. An Outline of Language Origins Theory Since the Renaissance. In Origins and Evolution of Language and Speech, eds. Harnad, S. R. et al. Annals of the New York Academy of Sciences 280: 4–13.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Abutalebi, J., Annoni, J.-M., Zimine, I. et al. 2008. Language Control and Lexical Competition in Bilinguals: An Event-Related fMRI Study. Cerebral Cortex 18.7: 1496–1505.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Aitchison, J. 1996. The Seeds of Speech: Language Origin and Evolution. Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
Aitchison, J. 1998. On Discontinuing the Continuity-Discontinuity Debate. In Hurford et al., pp. 17–43.
Akmajian, A., Demers, R. A., Farmer, A. K. and Harnish, R. M. 2001. Linguistics: An Introduction to Language and Communication, 5th edn. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.Google Scholar
Alter, S. 2005. William Dwight Whitney and the Science of Language. Baltimore, MD: The Johns Hopkins University Press.Google Scholar
Anderson, S. 2008. The Logical Structure of Linguistic Theory. Language 84.4: 795–813.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Andresen, J. T. 1981. Linguistic Metaphors in Charles de Brosses’s Traité of 1765 and the History of Linguistics. Linguisticae Investigationes V: 1.1–24.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Andresen, J. T. 1990a. Linguistics in America: 1769–1924. A Critical History. London: Routledge.Google Scholar
Andresen, J. T. 1990b. Skinner and Chomsky Thirty Years Later. Historiographia Linguistica.17: 145–65. (Reprinted in The Behavior Analyst 1: 49–60, 1991.)CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Andresen, J. T. 1990c. On Genetic Encoding and Communication. Reply to Newmeyer. Language and Communication 11: 29–32.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Andresen, J. T. 1992a. The Behaviorist Turn in Recent Theories about Language. Behavior and Philosophy 20: 1–19.Google Scholar
Andresen, J. T. 1992b. The Contemporary Linguist Meets the Postmodernist. Beiträge zur Geschichte der Sprachwissenschaft 2: 213–23.Google Scholar
Andresen, J. T. 1999. Pragmatism, Behaviorism, and the Evolutionary Script. The Peirce Seminar Papers. Essays in Semiotic Analysis, vol. IV, ed. Shapiro, M., pp. 481–514.
Andresen, J. T. 2010. Historiography’s Contribution to Theoretical Linguistics. In Chomskyan (R)evolutions, ed. Kibbee, D., pp. 445–71. Amsterdam/Philadelphia: John Benjamins.Google Scholar
Appleman, P. ed. 1970. Darwin. A Norton Critical Edition. New York: Norton.
Arbib, M. and Rizzolatti, G. 1997. Neural Expectations: A Possible Evolutionary Path from Manual Skills to Language. Communication and Cognition 29: 393–424.Google Scholar
Armstrong, D. 2008. The Gestural Theory of Language Origins. Sign Language Studies 8.3: 289–314.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Attwood, A., Frith, U. and Hermelin, B. 1988. The Understanding and Use of Interpersonal Gestures by Autistic and Down’s Syndrome Children. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders 18.2: 241–57.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Au, T. K., Knightly, L. M., Jun, S.-A. and Oh, J. S. 2002. Overhearing a Language During Childhood. American Psychological Society 13.3: 238–43.Google ScholarPubMed
Austin, J. L. 1962. How To Do Things With Words. Cambridge MA: Harvard University Press.Google Scholar
Bakhurts, D. and Shanker, S. 2001. Jerome Bruner. Language, Culture, Self. London: Sage.Google Scholar
Balter, M. 2007. Brain Evolution Studies Go Micro. Science 315: 1208–11.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Barkow, J., Cosmides, L. and Tooby, J., eds. 1992. The Adapted Mind: Evolutionary Psychology and the Generation of Culture. Oxford University Press.
Barrett, H. C. 2004. Dispelling Rumors of a Gene Shortage. Science 304: 1601–602.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Barrett, L., Dunbar, R. and Lycett, J. 2002. Human Evolutionary Psychology. Princeton University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Bates, E., Dale, P. S. and Thal, D. 1995. Individual Differences and Their Implications for Theories of Language Development. In Handbook of Child Language, eds. Fletcher, P. and MacWhinney, B., Chapter 4. Oxford: Basil Blackwell.Google Scholar
Beadle, G. and Beadle, M. 1966. The Language of Life: An Introduction to the Science of Genetics. Garden City, NY: Doubleday.Google Scholar
Becker, A. L. 1991. A Short Essay on Languaging. In Research and Reflexivity, ed. Stiner, F., pp. 226–34. London: Sage Publications.Google Scholar
Berlin, B. and Kay, P. 1969. Basic Color Terms: Their Universality and Evolution. Berkeley, CA: University of California Press.Google Scholar
Bialystok, E. and Shapero, D. 2005. Ambiguous Benefits: The Effect of Bilingualism on Reversing Ambiguous Figures. Developmental Science 8.6: 595–604.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Bickerton, D. 1990. Language and Species. University of Chicago Press.Google Scholar
Bickerton, D. 2009. Adam’s Tongue. How Humans Made Language. How Language Made Humans. New York: Hill and Wang.Google Scholar
Bloom, P. 2004. Children Think Before They Speak. Nature 430: 410–11.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Bloom, P. and Keil, F. 2001. Thinking Through Language. Mind & Language 16.4: 351–67.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Bloom, P., Peterson, M., Nadel, L. and Garrett, M., eds. 1996. Language and Space. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.
Bloomfield, L. 1933. Language. University of Chicago Press.Google Scholar
Bod, R. 1998. Beyond Grammar. An Experience-Based Theory of Language. Stanford, CA: Center for the Study of Language and Information.Google Scholar
Bolinger, D. 1981 [1968]. Aspects of Language, 3rd edn. New York: Harcourt Brace Jovanovich.Google Scholar
Boroditsky, L. 2001. Does Language Shape Thought? Mandarin and English Speakers’ Conceptions of Time. Cognitive Psychology 4: 1–22.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Boroditsky, L., Fuhrman, O. and McCormick, K. 2011. Do English and Mandarin Speakers Think About Time Differently?Cognition 118.1: 123–29.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Bowerman, M. and Levinson, S., eds. 2001. Language Acquisition and Conceptual Development. Cambridge University Press.CrossRef
Brace, C. L. 2004. Bipedalism, Canine Tooth Reduction, and Obligatory Tool Use. Response to Falk (2004), Behavioral and Brain Sciences 24.4: 507–08.Google Scholar
Brosnan, S. and de Waal, F. 2003. Monkeys Reject Unequal Pay. Nature: 297–99.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Brown, P. and Levinson, S. 1987 [1978]. Politeness: Some Universals in Language Usage. Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
Brown, R. 1973. A First Language: The Early Stages. Cambridge MA: Harvard University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Brown, R. L. 1967. Wilhelm von Humboldt’s Conception of Linguistic Relativity. The Hague: Mouton.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Bruner, J. 1983. Child’s Talk: Learning to Use Language. New York: Norton.Google Scholar
Bruner, J. 1990. Acts of Meaning. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press.Google Scholar
Buller, D. and Hardcastle, V. Gray 2000. Evolutionary Psychology, Meet Developmental Neurobiology: Against Promiscuous Modularity. Brain and Mind 1: 307–25.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Burling, R. 1992. Patterns of Language: Structure, Variation, Change. San Diego: Academic Press.Google Scholar
Buttelmann, D., Carpenter, M., Call, J. and Tomasello, M. 2007. Enculturated Apes Imitate Rationally. Developmental Science 10.4: 31–38.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Calvin, W 1991. The Throwing Madonna: Essays on the Brain. New York: McGraw-Hill.Google Scholar
Campbell, R. S. and Pennebaker, J. 2003. The Secret Life of Pronouns: Flexibility in Writing Style and Physical Health. Psychological Science 14.1: 60–64CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Cantlon, J. and Brannon, E. 2005. Semantic Congruity Affects Numerical Judgments Similarly in Monkeys and Humans. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences 102.45: 16507–11.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Cantlon, J. and Brannon, E. 2006. Shared System for Ordering Small and Large Numbers in Monkeys and Humans. Psychological Science 17.5: 402–407.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Cavalli-Sforza, L. 2000. Genes, Peoples, and Languages. New York: North Point Press.Google Scholar
Chambers, J. K. 2003 [1995]. Sociolinguistic Theory, 2nd edn. Oxford: Blackwell.Google Scholar
Changeux, J.-P. 1985. Neuronal Man: The Biology of Mind, trans. Garey, L.. New York: Pantheon Books.Google Scholar
Changeux, J.-P. and Ricoeur, P. 2000. What Makes Us Think? A Neuroscientist and a Philosopher Argue about Ethics, Human Nature, and the Brain, trans. DeBevoise, M. B.. Princeton University Press.Google Scholar
Changizi, M. 2001. Universal Scaling Laws for Hierarchical Complexity in Languages, Organisms, Behaviors and Other Combinatorial Systems. Journal of Theoretical Biology 211: 277–95.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Chen, J.-Y. 2007. Do Chinese and English Speakers Think About Time Differently? Failure of Replicating Boroditsky (2001). Cognition 104: 427–36.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Cheney, D. and Seyfarth, R. 1990. How Monkeys See the World: Inside the Mind of Another Species. University of Chicago Press.Google Scholar
Choi, S., McDonough, L., Bowerman, M. and Mandler, J. M. 1999. Early Sensitivity to Language-Specific Spatial Categories in English and Korean. Cognitive Development 14: 241–68.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Chomsky, N. 1957. Syntactic Structures. Berlin: Mouton.Google Scholar
Chomsky, N. 1959. Review of Verbal Behavior. Language 35.1: 26–58.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Chomsky, N. 1965. Aspects of the Theory of Syntax. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.Google Scholar
Chomsky, N. 1966. Cartesian Linguistics. A Chapter in the History of Rationalist Thought. New York: Harper & Row.Google Scholar
Chomsky, N. 1968. Language and Mind. New York: Harcourt, Brace & World.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Chomsky, N. 1971. The Case Against B.F. Skinner, Review of Beyond Freedom and Dignity by B. F. Skinner. New York Review of Books, December 30.
Chomsky, N. 1972. Language and Mind (enl. edn.) New York: Harcourt Brace Jovanovich.Google Scholar
Chomsky, N. 1975. Reflections on Language. New York: Pantheon.Google Scholar
Chomsky, N. 1980. Rules and Representations. Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Chomsky, N. 1988. Language and Problems of Knowledge. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.Google Scholar
Chomsky, N. 1995a. The Minimalist Program. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.Google Scholar
Chomsky, N. 1995b. Language and Nature. Mind 104: 1–61.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Chomsky, N. 2002. Nature and Language. Cambridge University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Chomsky, N. 2006. Language and Mind, 3rd edn. Cambridge University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Chomsky, N. and Halle, M. 1968. The Sound Pattern of English. New York: Harper & Row.Google Scholar
Chomsky, N., Hauser, M. D. and Fitch, W. T. 2005. Appendix. The Minimalist Program. A response to Pinker and Jackendoff (2005). Retrieved online.
Christiansen, M. and Kirby, S., eds. 2003. Language Evolution: States of the Art. New York: Oxford University Press.CrossRef
Chung, C. and Pennebaker, J. 2007. The Psychological Functions of Function Words. In Social Communication, ed. Fielder, K., pp. 343–59. New York: Psychology Press.Google Scholar
Clark, A. 1997. Being There. Putting Brain, Body, and World Together Again. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.Google Scholar
Cohen, J. 2007. Relative Differences: The Myth of 1%. Science 316: 1836.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Cohen, R. and Borsoi, D. 1996. The Role of Gesture in Description-Communication: A Cross-Sectional Study of Aging. Journal of Nonverbal Behavior 20.1: 45–63.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Colopento, J. 2007. The Interpreter. Has a Remote Amazonian Tribe Upended Our Understanding of Language?New Yorker April 16, pp. 118–37.Google Scholar
Condon, W. and Sander, L.. 1974. Neonate Movement Is Synchronized with Adult Speech: Interactional Participation and Language Acquisition. Science 183: 99–101.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Connolly, W. 1995. The Ethos of Pluralization. Minneapolis: University of Minnesota Press.Google Scholar
Connolly, W. 1999. Why I Am Not A Secularist. Minneapolis: University of Minnesota Press.Google Scholar
Connolly, W. 2002. Neuropolitics. Thinking, Culture, Speed. Minneapolis: University of Minnesota Press.Google Scholar
Connolly, W. 2005. Pluralism. Durham, NC: Duke University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Cooperrider, K. 2003. Observing Language, Observing Linguistics: Toward a Biological Systems Epistemology of Languaging. Senior Honors Thesis, Duke University.Google Scholar
Corballis, M. 2002. From Hand to Mouth: The Origins of Language. Princeton, NJ: Princeton University Press.Google Scholar
Cowie, F. 1999. What’s Within? Nativism Reconsidered. New York: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Crick, F. 1957. On Protein Synthesis. Symposium of the Society of Experimental Biology 12: 138–63.Google Scholar
Crick, F. 1970. Central Dogma of Molecular Biology. Nature 227: 561–63.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Crick, F. 1989. The Recent Excitement About Neural Networks. Nature 337: 129–31.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Critchley, M. 1959. The Evolution of Man’s Capacity for Language. Evolution after Darwin 2: 289. University of Chicago Press.Google Scholar
Crow, T. 1997. Is Schizophrenia the Price that Homo Sapiens Pays for Language?Schizophrenia Research 28: 127–41.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Crow, T. 2005. Who Forgot Paul Broca? The Origin of Language as Test Case of Speciation Theory. Journal of Linguistics 41: 133–56.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Crow, T. 2008. The “Big Bang” Theory of the Origin of Psychosis and the Faculty of Language. Schizophrenia Research 102.1–3: 31–52.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Dąbrowska, E. 2004. Language, Mind and Brain: Some Psychological and Neurological Constraints on Theories of Grammar. Edinburgh University Press.Google Scholar
Damasio, A. 1994. Descartes’ Error: Emotion, Reason, and the Human Brain. New York: Putnam.Google Scholar
Damasio, A. 2003. Looking for Spinoza: Joy, Sorrow, and the Feeling Brain. Orlando: Harcourt.Google Scholar
Danziger, S. and Ward, R. 2010. Language Changes Implicit Associations Between Ethnic Groups and Evaluation in Bilinguals. Psychological Science 21.6: 799–800.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Darwin, C. 1968 [1859]. Origin of the Species by Means of Natural Selection or The Preservation of Favoured Races in the Struggle for Life, edited and with an introduction by Burrow, J.W.. London: Penguin Books.Google Scholar
Darwin, C. 1979, 1970 [1871]. Descent of Man. New York: Norton.Google Scholar
Davies, I. and Corbett, G. 1997. A Cross-Cultural Study of Colour Grouping: Evidence for Weak Linguistic Relativity. British Journal of Psychology 88: 493–517.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Deacon, T. 1997. The Symbolic Species. The Co-Evolution of Language and the Brain. New York: W. W. Norton.Google Scholar
de Laguna, G. 1927. Speech: Its Function and Development. New Haven: Yale University Press.Google Scholar
de Waal, F. 1982. Chimpanzee Politics: Power and Sex Among Apes, with forward by Desmond Morris. Baltimore, MD: The Johns Hopkins University Press.Google Scholar
de Waal, F. 2001. The Ape and the Sushi Master: Cultural Reflections of a Primatologist. New York: Basic Books.Google Scholar
Dediu, D. and Ladd, D. R. 2007. Linguistic Tone Is Related to the Population Frequency of the Adaptive Haplogroups of Two Brain Size Genes, ASPM and Microcephalin. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences 104: 10944–49.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Deleuze, G. and Guattari, F. 1987. A Thousand Plateaus. Capitalism and Schizophrenia, translation and Foreword by Massumi, B.. Minneapolis: University of Minnesota Press.Google Scholar
Derrida, J. 1967. De la grammatologie. Paris: Editions de Minuit.Google Scholar
Derrida, J. 1988. Limited, Inc. Evanston, IL: Northwestern University Press.Google Scholar
Descartes, R. 1996. Discourse on the Method [Discours de la Méthode 1637] and Meditations on First Philosophy [Meditationes de prima philosophia 1641], ed. Weissman, D.. New Haven: Yale University Press.Google Scholar
Deutsch, D. and Henthorn, T. 2004. Absolute Pitch, Speech, and Tone Language: Some Experiments and a Proposed Framework. Music Perception 21.3: 339–56.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Deutscher, G. 2005. The Unfolding of Language. An Evolutionary Tour of Mankind’s Greatest Invention. New York: Henry Holt.Google Scholar
Dick, A. S., Goldin-Meadow, S., Hasson, U., Skipper, J. and Small, S. 2009. Co-Speech Gestures Influence Neural Activity in Brain Regions Associated with Processing Semantic Information. Human Brain Mapping 30: 3509–26.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Dickerson, P., Stribling, P. and Rae, J. 2007. Tapping into Interaction: How Children with Autism Spectrum Disorders Design and Place Tapping in Relation to Activities in Progress. Gesture 7.3: 271–303.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Dolan, R. L. 2002. Emotion, Cognition, Behavior. Science 2989: 1191–94.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Donald, M. 1991. Origins of the Modern Mind: Three Stages in the Evolution of Culture and Cognition. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press.Google Scholar
Donald, M. 2001. A Mind So Rare: The Evolution of Human Consciousness. New York: W.W. Norton.Google Scholar
Doolittle, W. F. and Bapteste, E. 2007. Pattern Pluralism and the Tree of Life Hypothesis. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences 104: 2043–49.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Dubner, S. and Levitt, S. 2005. Keith Chen’s Monkey Research. New York Times Magazine, June 5.
Elman, J. 1990. Finding Structure in Time. Cognitive Science 14.179–211.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Elman, J. 2003. Generalization from Sparse Input. Proceedings of the 38th Annual Meeting of the Chicago Linguistic Society.
Elman, J., Bates, E. A., Johnson, M. H. et al. 1999. Rethinking Innateness: A Connectionist Perspective on Development. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.Google Scholar
Erard, M. 2007. Read My Slips: Speech Errors Show How Language is Processed. Science 317: 1674–76.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Evans, N. and Levinson, S. 2009. The Myth of Language Universals: Language Diversity and Its Importance for Cognitive Science. Behavioral and Brain Sciences 32: 429–92.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Everett, D. 2009. Pirahã Culture and Grammar: A Response to Some Criticism. Language 85.2: 405–42.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Falk, D. 2004. Prelinguistic Evolution in Early Hominins: Whence Motherese?Behavioral and Brain Sciences 27.4: 491–503.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Fedorenko, E., Behr, M. and Kanwisher, N. 2011. Localizing Language in the Brain. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences August 30, 2011.Google Scholar
Fillmore, C. and Kay, P. 1999. Grammatical Constructions and Linguistic Generalizations: The What’s X Doing Y? Construction. Language 75: 1–33.Google Scholar
Fillmore, C., Kay, P. and O’Connor, M. C. 1988. Regularity and Idiomaticity in Grammatical Constructions: The Case of LET ALONE. Language 64.3: 501–38.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Fisher, S. 2006. Tangled Webs: Tracing the Connections Between Genes and Cognition. Cognition 101: 270–97.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Fishman, J. 1960. Systematization of the Whorfian Hypothesis. Behavioral Science 5: 323–39.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Fitch, W. T. 2010. The Evolution of Language. Cambridge University Press.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Fleck, L. 1979 [1935]. Genesis and Development of a Scientific Fact, ed. Trenn, Th. J. and Merton, R. K., trans. Bradley, F. and Trenn, Th. J.. University of Chicago Press.Google Scholar
Fodor, J. 1983. The Modularity of Mind. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.Google Scholar
Fodor, J. 1998. Concepts: Where Cognitive Science Went Wrong. Oxford University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Fodor, J. 2000. The Mind Doesn’t Work That Way: The Scope and Limits of Computational Psychology. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.Google Scholar
Foley, W. 1997. Anthropological Linguistics: An Introduction. Malden, MA: Blackwell.Google Scholar
Foucault, M. 1971. The Discourse on Language. Social Science Information 10.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Foucault, M. 1994. Ethics: Subjectivity and Truth. Essential Works of Foucault 1954–1984, vol. I, ed. Rabinow, P., trans. Hurley, R. and others. New York: New Press.Google Scholar
Freeman, S. and Herron, J. C. 2004. Evolutionary Analysis, 3rd edn. Upper Saddle River, NJ: Pearson/Prentice Hall.Google Scholar
Friederici, A. 2006. What’s in Control of Language?Nature Neuroscience 9.8: 991–92.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Fromkin, V., Rodman, R. and Hyams, N. 2003. An Introduction to Language, 7th edn. Boston: Thomson/Wadsworth.Google Scholar
Fromkin, V., Rodman, R. and Hyams, N. 2011. An Introducction to Language, 9th edn. Boston: Thomson/Wadsworth.Google Scholar
Futuyma, D. 1986. Evolutionary Biology, 2nd edn. Sunderland, MA: Sinauer Associates.Google ScholarPubMed
Futuyma, D. 1998. Evolutionary Biology, 3rd edn. Sunderland, MA: Sinauer Associates.Google Scholar
Gabrieli, J. 2009. Dyslexia: A New Synergy Between Energy and Cognitive Science. Science 325: 280–83.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Gamble, C. 1994. Timewalkers. The Prehistory of Global Colonization. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press.Google Scholar
Gazzaniga, M., Ivry, R. and Mangum, G. 2002. Cognitive Neuroscience. The Biology of Mind, 2nd ed. New York: W.W. Norton.Google Scholar
Geertz, C. 1962. The Growth of Culture and the Evolution of Mind. In Theories of the Mind, ed. Scher, J. M., pp. 713–37. New York: The Free Press of Glencoe.Google Scholar
Gellatly, A. 1995. Colourful Whorfian Ideas: Linguistic and Cultural Influences on the Perception and Cognition of Colour, and on the Investigation of Them. Mind & Language 10.3: 199–225.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Gentner, D. and Goldin-Meadow, S. 2003. Language in Mind. Advances in the Study of Language and Thought. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.Google Scholar
Gibbons, A. 2007. European Skin Turned Pale Only Recently, Gene Suggests. Science 316: 364.Google ScholarPubMed
Gibson, J. J. 1979. The Ecological Approach to Visual Perception. Boston: Houghton Mifflin.Google Scholar
Gibson, J. J. 1982. Reason for Realism. Selected Essays of James J. Gibson, ed. Reed, E. and Jones, R.. Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum.Google Scholar
Gibson, K. 1994. Continuity Theories of Human Language Origins Versus the Liberman Model. Language and Communication 14.1: 97–114.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Givón, T. 1998. On the Co-Evolution of Language, Mind and Brain. Evolution of Communication 2: 45–116.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Givón, T. 2002. The Visual Information-Processing System as an Evolutionary Precursor of Human Language. In Givón and Malle, pp. 3–50.
Givón, T. and Malle, B. F., eds. 2002. The Evolution of Language out of Pre-Language. Amsterdam: John Benjamins.CrossRef
Goldberg, A. 2001. Patient Arguments of Causative Verbs Can Be Omitted: The Role of Information Structure in Argument Distribution. Language Sciences 23: 503–24.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Goldberg, A. 2006. Constructions at Work: The Nature of Generalization in Language. Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Goldberg, A. and Jackendoff, R. 2004. The English Resultative as a Family of Constructions. Language 80.3: 532–68.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Goldin-Meadow, S. 2003. Hearing Gesture: How Our Hands Help Us Think. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press.Google Scholar
Goldstein, M. and West, M. 1999. Consistent Responses of Human Mothers to Prelinguistic Infants: The Effect of Prelinguistic Repertoire Size. Journal of Comparative Psychology 113.1: 52–58.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Goldstein, M., King, A. and West, M. 2003. Social Interaction Shapes Babbling: Testing Parallels Between Birdsong and Speech. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences 100.13: 8030–35.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Gopnik, A. 2001. Theories, Language, and Culture: Whorf Without Wincing. In Bowerman and Levinson, pp. 45–69.
González, J., Barros-Loscertales, A., Pulvermüller, F. et al. 2006. Reading Cinnamon Activates Olfactory Brain Regions. NeuroImage 32: 906–12.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Gordon, P. 2004. Numerical Cognition Without Words: Evidence from Amazonia. Science 306: 496–99.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Green, J. 1987. Romance Languages. In The World’s Major Languages, ed. B. Comrie, pp. 203–209.
Griffiths, P. 1997. What Emotions Really Are. University of Chicago Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Griffiths, P. 2007. Evo-Devo Meets the Mind: Towards a Developmental Evolutionary Psychology. In Integrating Evolution and Development, eds. Sansom, R. and Brandon, R.. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.Google Scholar
Hagoort, P. 2005. On Broca, Brains, and Binding: A New Framework. Trends in Cognitive Science 9.9: 416–23.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Hakuta, K., Bialystok, E. and Wiley, E. 2003. Critical Evidence: A Test of the Critical-Period Hypothesis for Second-Language Acquisition. Psychological Science 14.1: 31–38.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Harris, R. 1996. The Language Connection: Philosophy and Linguistics. Bristol, UK: Thoemmes Press.Google Scholar
Hauser, M. 2006. Moral Minds: How Nature Designed Our Universal Sense of Right and Wrong. New York: Harper Collins.Google Scholar
Hauser, M., Chomsky, N. and Fitch, W. T. 2002. The Faculty of Language: What Is It, Who Has It, and How Did It Evolve?Science 298: 1569–79.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Hayek, F. A. 1988. The Fatal Conceit. The Errors of Socialism. Chicago University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Hendriks-Jansen, H. 1996. Catching Ourselves in the Act. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.Google Scholar
Hespos, S. and Spelke, E. 2004. Conceptual Precursors to Languages. Nature 430: 453–56.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Hill, J. 1974. Possible Continuity Theories of Language. Language 50.1: 134–50.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Hockett, C. 1960. Origin of Speech. Scientific American 203: 89–97.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Holle, H., Obleser, J., Rueschemeyer, S.-A. and Gunter, T. 2010. Integration of Iconic Gestures and Speech in Left Superior Temporal Areas Boosts Speech Comprehension Under Adverse Listening Conditions. NeuroImage 49.1: 875–84.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Humboldt, W. von 1997. Essays on Language, ed. Harden, T. and Farrelly, D., trans. Wieczorek, J. and Roe, I.. Frankfurt am Main: Peter Lang.Google Scholar
Hunt, E. and Agnoli, F. 1991. The Whorfian Hypothesis: A Cognitive Psychology Perspective. Psychological Review 98.3: 377–89.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Hurford, J., Studdert-Kennedy, M. and Knight, C., eds. 1998. Approaches to the Evolution of Language. Cambridge University Press.
Huxley, Sir J. 1942 [1963]. Evolution: The Modern Synthesis. In Appleman, pp. 244–65.
Iverson, J. and Goldin-Meadow, S. 2001. The Resilience of Gesture in Talk: Gesture in Blind Speakers and Listeners. Developmental Science 4.4: 416–22.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Jablonka, E. and Lamb, M. 2005. Evolution in Four Dimensions: Genetic, Epigenetic, Behavioral, and Symbolic Variation in the History of Life. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.Google Scholar
Jackendoff, R. 1999. Possible Stages in the Evolution of the Language Capacity. Trends in Cognitive Sciences 3.7: 272–79.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Jackendoff, R. 2001. Language in the Ecology of the Mind. In The Routledge Companion to Semiotics and Linguistics, ed. Cobley, P., pp. 52–65. London: Routledge.Google Scholar
Jackendoff, R. 2002. Foundations of Language: Brain, Meaning, Grammar, Evolution. Oxford: Oxford University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Jackendoff, R. and Pinker, S. 2005. The Nature of the Language Faculty and Its Implications for Evolution of Language (Reply to Fitch, Hauser and Chomsky). Cognition 97: 211–25.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Jacob, F. 1966. Genetics of the Bacterial Cell. Science 152: 1470–748.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Jakobson, R. 1990 [1976]. The Speech Event and the Functions of Language. In On Language, ed. Waugh, L. and Monville-Burston, M., pp 69–79. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press.Google Scholar
James, W. 1981 [1890]. The Principles of Psychology. Cambridge MA: Harvard University Press.Google Scholar
James, W. 1981 [1907]. Pragmatism. Indianapolis: Hackett Publishing.Google Scholar
Jänke, L., Siegenthaler, Th., Preis, S. and Steinmetz, H. 2007. Decreased White-Matter Density in a Left-Sided Fronto-Temporal Network in Children with Development Language Disorder: Evidence for Anatomical Anomalies in a Motor-Language Network. Brain and Language 102: 91–98.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Jaworski, A. and Coupland, N. 1999. The Discourse Reader. London: Routledge.Google Scholar
Jerne, N. 1985. The Generative Grammar of the Immune System. Science 229: 1057–60.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Jespersen, O. 1922. Language: Its Nature, Development and Origin. London: George Allen & Unwin.Google Scholar
Johnstone, B. 1996. The Linguistic Individual: Self-Expression in Language and Linguistics. New York: Oxford University. Press.Google Scholar
Johnstone, B. 2000. The Individual Voice in Language. Annual Review of Anthropology 29: 405–24.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Johnstone, B. 2002. Discourse Analysis. Oxford: Blackwell.Google Scholar
Joseph, J. 2002. From Whitney to Chomsky. Essays in the History of American Linguistics. Amsterdam/Philadelphia: John Benjamins.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Kandel, E., Schwartz, J. and Jessell, T. 1995. Essentials of Neural Science and Behavior. Norwalk, CT: Appleton & Lange.Google Scholar
Karmiloff-Smith, A. 1995. Beyond Modularity: A Developmental Perspective on Cognitive Science. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.Google Scholar
Karmiloff-Smith, A. 1998. Development Itself Is the Key to Understanding Developmental Disorders. Trends in Cognitive Science 2.10: 389–398.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Karmiloff, K. and Karmiloff-Smith, A. 2001. Pathways to Language: From Fetus to Adolescent. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press.Google Scholar
Kay, P. and Fillmore, C. 1999. Grammatical Constructions and Linguistic Generalizations: The What’s X doing Y? Construction. Language 75.1: 1–33.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Kay, P. and Kempton, W. 1984. What Is the Sapir-Whorf Hypothesis?American Anthropologist 86: 65–79.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Kay, P. and Maffi, L. 2000. Color Appearance and the Emergence and Evolution of Basic Color Lexicons. American Anthropologist 101.4: 743–60.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Keller, E. F. and Lloyd, E. A., eds. 1992. Keywords in Evolutionary Biology. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press.
Kendon, A. 1991. Some Consideration for a Theory of Language Origins. Man (N.S.) 26: 199–221.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Kohler, E., Keysers, C., Umilta, M. A. et al. 2002. Hearing Sounds, Understanding Actions: Action Representation in Mirror Neurons. Science 297: 846–48.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Krauss, R. 1987. The Role of the Listener: Addressee Influences on Message Formulation. Journal of Language and Social Psychology 6.2: 81–98.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Kuhn, T. 1962. The Structure of Scientific Revolutions. Chicago, IL: University of Chicago Press.Google Scholar
Labov, W. 1963. The Social Motivation of Sound Change. Word 19: 273–309.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Labov, W. 1972. Sociolinguistic Patterns. Philadelphia: University of Pennsylvania Press.Google Scholar
Labov, W. 1987. The Overestimation of Functionalism. In Functionalism in Linguistics, ed. Dirven, R. and Fried, V., pp. 311–32. Amsterdam, Philadelphia: John Benjamins.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Lai, C., Fisher, S., Hurst, J., Vargha-Khadem, F. and Monaco, A. 2001. A Forkhead-Domain Gene Is Mutated in a Severe Speech and Language Disorder. Science 413: 519–23.Google Scholar
Lakoff, G. 1987. Women, Fire and Dangerous Things: What Categories Reveal About the Mind. University of Chicago Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Lakoff, G. 2004. Don’t Think of an Elephant!White River Junction, VT: Chelsea Green Publishers.Google Scholar
Lakoff, G. and Johnson, M. 1980. Metaphors We Live By. University of Chicago Press.Google Scholar
Lakoff, G. and Johnson, M. 1999. Philosophy in the Flesh. New York: Basic Books.Google Scholar
Lakoff, G. and Núñez, R. 2000. Where Mathematics Comes From: How the Embodied Mind Brings Mathematics Into Being. New York: Basic Books.Google Scholar
Langacker, R. 1987. Foundations of Cognitive Grammar. Palo Alto: Stanford University Press.Google Scholar
LeDoux, J. 1996. The Emotional Brain: The Mysterious Underpinning of Emotional Life. New York: Simon & Schuster.Google Scholar
Lenneberg, E. 1967. Biological Foundations of Language. New York: Wiley.Google Scholar
Levinson, S. C. 2001. Covariation Between Spatial Language and Cognition, and Its Implications for Language Learning. In Bowerman and Levinson, pp. 566–88.
Levinson, S. C. 2003. Space in Language and Cognition: Explorations in Cognitive Diversity. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Levinson, S. C., Kita, S., Haun, D. B. M. and Rasch, B. J. 2002. Returning the Tables: Language Affects Spatial Reasoning. Cognition 84.2: 155–88.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Lewontin, R. 2000. The Triple Helix: Gene, Organism, and Environment. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press.Google Scholar
Li, C. 2002. Missing Links, Issues and Hypotheses in the Evolutionary Origin of Language. In Givón and Malle, pp. 83–106.
Li, P. and Gleitman, L. 2002. Turning the Tables: Language and Spatial Reasoning. Cognition 83.3: 265–94.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Lieberman, P. 1984. The Biology and Evolution of Language. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press.Google Scholar
Lieberman, P. 1991. Uniquely Human: The Evolution of Speech, Thought, and Selfless Behavior. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press.Google Scholar
Lieberman, P. 2000. Human Language and Our Reptilian Brain: The Subcortical Bases of Speech, Syntax, and Thought. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press.Google Scholar
Lieberman, P. 2005. The Pied Piper of Cambridge. The Linguistic Review 22: 223–35.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Lieberman, P. 2006. Toward an Evolutionary Biology of Language. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press.Google Scholar
Locke, J. 1975 [1690]. An Essay Concerning Human Understanding, ed. Nidditch, P.. Oxford: Clarendon Press.Google Scholar
Luhmann, N. 1990. Essays on Self-Reference. New York: Columbia University Press.Google Scholar
MacFarquhar, L. 2003. The Devil’s Accountant. New Yorker March 31, pp. 64–79.Google Scholar
MacNeilage, P. 2008. The Origin of Speech. Oxford: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Maestripieri, D., Leoni, M., Raza, S., Hirsch, E. and Whitham, J. 2005. Female Copulation Calls in Guinea Baboons: Evidence for Postcopulatory Female Choice?International Journal of Primatology 26.4: 737–58.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Marr, D. 1982. Vision: A Computational Investigation into the Human Representation and Processing of Visual Information. New York: Freeman.Google Scholar
Martin, L. 1986. Eskimo Words for Snow: A Case Study in the Genesis and Decay of an Anthropological Example. American Anthropologist 88.2: 418–23.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Maturana, H. 1970. The Biology of Cognition. In Maturana and Varela 1980 [1972], pp. 1–58.
Maturana, H. 1987. Everything Is Said by an Observer. In Gaia: A Way of Knowing. Political Implications of the New Biology, ed. Thompson, W. I., pp. 11–36. New York: Lindisfarne Press.Google Scholar
Maturana, H. 1989. Lenguaje y realidad: El origin de lo humano. Arch. Biol. Med. Exp. 22: 77–81. Chile.Google Scholar
Maturana, H. 1991 [1988]. Reality: The Search for Objectivity, or the Quest for a Compelling Argument. In Die Gedankenwelt Sir Karl Poppers. Kritischer Rationalismus im Dialog, ed. Leser, N., Seifer, J. and Plitzner, K., pp. 282–357. Heidelberg: Car Winter.Google Scholar
Maturana, H. and Mpodozis, J. 1992. Origen de las Espécies por medio de la Deriva natural o La diversificación de los linajes a través de la conservación y cambio de los fenotipos ontogénicos. Publicación Ocasional n. 46/1992. Santiago, Chile: Museo Nacional de Historia Natural.Google Scholar
Maturana, H. and Varela, F. 1980 [1972]. Autopoiesis and Cognition. The Realization of the Living. Dordrecht: D. Reidel.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Maturana, H. 1992 [1987]. The Tree of Knowledge, The Biological Roots of Human Understanding, revised edn., trans. Paolucci, R.. Boston: Shambhala.Google Scholar
Mayr, E. 1963. Animal Species and Evolution. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Mayr, E. 1982. The Growth of Biological Thought: Diversity, Evolution, and Inheritance. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press.Google Scholar
McGurk, H. and MacDonald, J. 1976. Hearing Lips and Seeing Voices. Nature 264: 746–48.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
McIntosh, A. R. and Lobaugh, N. J. 2003. When Is a Word Not a Word?Science 301: 322–23.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
McMurray, B. 2007. Defusing the Childhood Vocabulary Explosion. Science 317: 631.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
McNeill, D. 2005. Gesture and Thought. Chicago, IL: University of Chicago Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
McNeill, D. 2012. How Language Began: Gesture and Speech in Human Evolution. Cambridge University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Melser, D. 2004. The Act of Thinking. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.Google Scholar
Meltzoff, A., Kuhl, P., Movellan, J. and Sejnowski, T. 2009. Foundations for a New Science of Learning. Science 325: 284–88.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Mendoza-Denton, N. 2007. Homegirls: The Making of Latina Youth Styles. Oxford: Blackwell.Google Scholar
Mingers, J. 2002. Can Social Systems Be Autopoietic? Assessing Luhmann’s Social Theory. The Sociological Review 50.2: 278–99.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Morsella, E. and Krauss, R. 2004. The Role of Gestures in Spatial Working Memory and Speech. The American Journal of Psychology 117.3: 411–24.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Mufwene, S. 2001. The Ecology of Language Evolution. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Müller, (F.) M. 1965 [1861]. Lectures on the Science of Language, delivered at the Royal Institution of Great Britain. Delhi, India: Munshi Ram Manohar Lal.Google Scholar
Neisser, U. 1976. Cognition and Reality: Principles and Implications of Cognitive Psychology. San Francisco, CA: W. H. Freeman.Google Scholar
Nelson, K. 1996. Language in Cognitive Development: Emergence of the Mediated Mind. Cambridge University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Nelson, K. 2007. Young Minds in Social Worlds. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Nevins, A., Pesetsky, D. and Rodrigues, C. 2009. Pirahã Exceptionality: A Reassessment. Language 85.2: 355–404.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Newman, J. 2004. Motherese by Any Other Name: Mother-Infant Communication in Non-Hominin Mammals. Response to Falk (2004), Behavioral and Brain Sciences 27.4: 519–20.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Newmeyer, F. 1998. On the Supposed “Counterfunctionality” of Universal Grammar: Some Evolutionary Implications. In Hurford, et al., pp. 305–19.
Newmeyer, F. 2003. Grammar Is Grammar and Usage Is Usage. Language 79.4: 682–707.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Nunberg, G. 1997. Verbed Off. In The Way We Talk Now (2001), pp. 151–53. Boston, MA: Houghton Mifflin.Google Scholar
Núñez, R. and Freeman, W. J. 1999. Reclaiming Cognition: The Primacy of Action, Intention and Emotion. Thorverton, UK and Bowling Green, OH: Imprint Academic.Google Scholar
Núñez, R. and Sweetser, E. 2006. With the Future Behind Them: Convergent Evidence From Aymara Language and Gesture in the Crosslinguistic Comparison of Spatial Construals of Time. Cognitive Science 30: 401–50.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
O’Grady, W, Archibald, J., Aronoff, M. and Rees-Miller, J. 2001. Contemporary Linguistics: An Introduction. Boston: Bedford/St. Martin’s.Google Scholar
O’Higgins, P. and Elton, S. 2007. Walking on Trees. Science 316: 1292–94.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Oozera, C., Noongwook, C., Alowa, C. and Krupnik, I. 2004. Watching Ice and Weather Our Way. Washington DC: Arctic Studies Center, Smithsonian.Google Scholar
Oshima-Takane, Y., Goodz, E. and Derevensky, J. L. 1996. Birth Order Effects on Early Language Development: Do Secondborn Children Learn from Overheard Speech?Child Development 67: 621–34.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Oyama, S. 1985. The Ontogeny of Information: Developmental Systems and Evolution. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
Oyama, S. 1988. Stasis, Development and Heredity. In Evolutionary Processes and Metaphors, ed. Ho, M.-W. and Fox, S.W., pp. 255–74. London: Wiley.Google Scholar
Oyama, S. 1989. Ontogeny and the Central Dogma: Do We Need the Concept of Genetic Programming in Order to Have an Evolutionary Perspective? In Systems and Development. The Minnesota Symposia on Child Psychology, vol. XXII, ed. Gunnar, M. and Thelen, E., pp. 1–13. Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum.Google Scholar
Oyama, S. 1995. The Accidental Chordate: Contingency in Developmental Systems. South Atlantic Quarterly 94.2: 509–26.Google Scholar
Oyama, S. 2000. Evolution’s Eye. A Systems View of the Biology-Culture Divide. Durham, NC: Duke University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Özyürek, A., Allen, S., Furman, R. et al. 2008. Development of Cross-Linguistic Variation in Speech and Gesture: Motion Events in English and Turkish. Developmental Psychology 44.4: 1040–54.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Packman, A., Code, C. and Onslow, M. 2007. On the Cause of Stuttering: Integrating Theory with Brain and Behavioral Research. Journal of Neurolinguistics 20: 353–62.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Pennebaker, J. 1997. Opening Up: The Healing Power of Expressing Emotions. New York:Guildford Press.Google Scholar
Peters, A. 1983. The Units of Language Acquisition. Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
Peters, J. D. 1989. John Locke, The Individual, and the Origin of Communication. Quarterly Journal of Speech 75.4: 387–99.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Pica, P., Lerner, C., Izard, V. and Dehaene, S. 2004. Exact and Approximate Arithmetic in an Amazonian Indigene Group. Science 306: 499–503.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Pinker, S. 1994. The Language Instinct: How the Mind Creates Language. New York: William Morrow.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Pinker, S. 1997. How the Mind Works. New York: Norton.Google Scholar
Pinker, S. 2002. Why the Nature/Nurture Debate Won’t Go Away. Boston Globe. October 13.
Pinker, S. 2003. Language as an Adaptation to the Cognitive Niche. In Language Evolution: States of the Art, ed. Christiansen, M. H. and Kirby, S.. New York: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Pinker, S. 2007. The Stuff of Thought. Language as a Window into Human Nature. Viking/Penguin: New York.Google Scholar
Pinker, S. 2008. The Moral Instinct. New York Times Sunday Magazine, January 13: 32ff.
Pinker, S. 2010. The Cognitive Niche: Coevolution of Intelligence, Sociality, and Language. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences 107 (Supplement 2): 8993–99.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Pinker, S. and Bloom, P. 1990. Natural Language and Natural Selection. Behavioral and Brain Sciences 13.4: 707–27, with Open Peer Commentary 727–84.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Pinker, S. and Bloom, P. 1992. Natural Language and Natural Selection, reprinted in Barkow, et al., pp. 451–93.
Pinker, S. and Jackendoff, R. 2005. The Faculty of Language: What’s Special About It?Cognition 95: 201–36.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Pollick, A. and de Waal, F. 2007. Ape Gestures and Language Evolution. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences 104.19: 8184–89.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Poplack, S. 1979/80. Sometimes I’ll Start a Sentence in Spanish y Termino En Espanol. Linguistics 18: 581–618.Google Scholar
Port, R. 2010. Rich Memory and Distributed Phonology. Language Sciences 32: 43–55.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Premack, D. 2004. Is Language the Key to Human Intelligence?Science 303: 318–20.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Pullum, G. 1996. Learnability, Hyperlearning, and the Poverty of the Stimulus. Proceedings of the Berkeley Linguistics Society 22: 498–513.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Pullum, G. and Scholz, B. 2002. Empirical Assessment of Stimulus Poverty Arguments. Linguistic Review 19: 9–50.Google Scholar
Purves, D. and Lotto, B. 2003. Why We See What We Do: An Empirical Theory of Vision. Sunderland, MA: Sinauer Associates.Google Scholar
Putnam, H. 1967. The ‘Innateness Hypothesis’ and Explanatory Models in Linguistics. Synthese 17.1: 12–22.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Quine, W. V. 1940. Mathematical Logic. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press.Google Scholar
Reddy, M. 1979. The Conduit Metaphor: A Case of Frame Conflict in Our Language About Language. In Metaphor and Thought, ed. Ortony, A., pp. 284–324. Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
Rickford, J. R., Wasow, T., Mendoza-Denton, N. and Espinoza, J. 1995. Syntactic Variation and Change in Progress: Loss of the Verbal Coda in Topic-Restricting As Far As Constructions. Language 71.1: 102–31.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Riegler, A. 2005. Constructive Memory. Kybernetes 34.1/2: 89–104.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Rizzolatti, G. and Arbib, M. 1998. Language Within Our Grasp. Trends in Neurosciences 12: 188–94.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Rizzolatti, G. and Craighero, L. 2004. The Mirror-Neuron System. Annual Review of Neuroscience 27: 169–92.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Rockwell, T. 2005. Neither Brain Nor Ghost. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.Google Scholar
Rowe, M. and Goldin-Meadow, S. 2009. Differences in Early Gesture Explain SES Disparities in Child Vocabulary Size at School Entry. Science 323: 951–53.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Sampson, G., Gil, D. and Trudgill, P., eds. 2009. Language Complexity as an Evolving Variable. Oxford University Press.
Sapir, E. 1921. Language. New York: Harcourt, Brace & World.Google Scholar
Sapir, E. 1949 [1929]. The Status of Linguistics as a Science. Language 5: 207–14. Reprinted in Selected Writings of Edward Sapir in Language, Culture, and Personality (1949), ed. Mandelbaum, D. G., pp. 160–66. Berkeley, CA: University of California Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Saussure, F. de 1985 [1916]. Cours de linguistique générale, edition prepared by de Mauro, T.. Paris: Payot.Google Scholar
Saussure, F. de 1959 [1916]. Course in General Linguistics, translated and with an introduction by Baskin, W.. New York: McGraw-Hill.Google Scholar
Savage-Rumbaugh, S. 1987. Communication, Symbolic Communication, and Language: Reply to Seidenberg and Petitto. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General 116.3: 228–92.Google ScholarPubMed
Savage-Rumbaugh, S., Shanker, S. and Taylor, T. 1996. Apes with Language. Critical Quarterly 38.3: 45–57.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Schiffrin, D. 1987. Discourse Markers. Cambridge University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Schlaggar, B., Brown, T., Lugar, H. et al. 2002. Functional Neuroanatomical Differences Between Adults and School-Age Children in the Processing of Single Words. Science 296: 1476–79.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Searle, J. 1995. The Construction of Social Reality. New York: The Free Press.Google Scholar
Searle, J. 1998. Mind, Language and Society. New York: Basic Books.Google Scholar
Seidenberg, M. and Petitto, L. 1987. Communication, Symbolic Communication, and Language: Comment on Savage-Rumbaugh, McDonald, Sevcik, Hopkins, and Rupert (1986). Journal of Experimental Psychology: General 116.3: 279–87.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Sheets-Johnstone, M. 1990. The Roots of Thinking. Philadelphia: Temple University Press.Google Scholar
Silverman, L., Bennetto, L., Campana, E. and Tanenhaus, M. 2007. Speech-and-Gesture Integration in High Functioning Autism. Cognition 11.3: 380–93.Google Scholar
Skinner, B. F. 1938. Behavior of Organisms. New York: Appleton-Century Company.Google Scholar
Skinner, B. F. 1945. The Operational Analysis of Psychological Terms. In Cumulative Record. Acton, MA: Copley.Google Scholar
Skinner, B. F. 1957. Verbal Behavior. Acton, MA: Copley Publishing Group.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Skinner, B. F. 1966. The Phylogeny and Ontogeny of Behavior. Science 153: 1205–13.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Skinner, B. F. 1972. A Lecture on ‘Having’ a Poem. Cumulative Record: A Selection of Papers by B.F. Skinner, 3rd edn., pp. 345–55. New York: Appleton-Century-Crofts.Google Scholar
Skinner, B. F. 1981. Selection by Consequences. Science 213: 501–504.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Skinner, B. F. 1985. Cognitive Science and Behaviourism. British Journal of Psychology 76: 291–301.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Skinner, B. F. 1990. Can Psychology Be a Science of Mind?American Psychologist 45: 1206–10.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Slobin, D. 1990. The Development from Child Speaker to Native Speaker. In Cultural Psychology. Essays on Comparative Human Development, ed. Stigler, J., Schweder, R. and Herdt, G.. Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
Slobin, D. 1996. From “Thought to Language” to “Thinking for Speaking”. In Rethinking Linguistic Relativity, ed. Gumperz, J. J. and Levinson, S. C., pp. 70–96. Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
Smith, B. H. 1978. On the Margins of Discourse: The Relation of Literature to Language. University of Chicago Press.Google Scholar
Smith, B. H. 1988. Contingencies of Value: Alternative Perspectives for Critical Theory. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press.Google Scholar
Smith, B. H. 1997. Belief and Resistance. Dynamics of Contemporary Intellectual Dynamics. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press.Google Scholar
Smith, B. H. 2005. Scandalous Knowledge. Edinburgh: University of Edinburgh Press.Google Scholar
Steier, F., ed. 1991. Research and Reflexivity. London: Sage.
Stephan, K., Marshall, J. C., Friston, K. J. et al. 2003. Lateralized Cognitive Processes and Lateralized Task Control in the Human Brain. Science 301: 384–86.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Stephens, G., Silbert, L. and Hasson, U. 2010. Speaker–Listener Neural Coupling Underlies Successful Communication. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences 107.32: 14425–30.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Strand, E. 1999. Uncovering the Role of Gender Stereotypes in Speech Perception. Journal of Langue and Social Psychology 18.1: 86–99.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Sundberg, M. L. and Michael, J. 2001. The Benefits of Skinner’s Analysis of Verbal Behavior for Children with Autism. Behavior Modification 25: 698–24.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Sundberg, M. L., Michael, J., Partington, J. W. and Sundberg, C. A. 1996. The Role of Automatic Reinforcement in Early Language Acquisition. Analysis of Verbal Behavior 13: 21–37.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Symons, D. 1987. If We’re All Darwinians, What’s the Fuss About? In Sociobiology and Psychology, ed. Crawford, C., Smith, M. and Krebs, D.. Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates.Google Scholar
Tallal, P. and Benasich, A. A. 2002. Developmental Language Learning Impairments. Development and Psychopathology 14: 559–79.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Tanenhaus, M., Spivey-Knowlton, M. J., Eberhard, K. and Sedivy, J. 1995. Integration of Visual and Linguistic Information in Spoken Language Comprehension. Science 268: 1632–35.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Tanenhaus, M., Magnuson, J. S., Dahan, D. and Chambers, C. 2000. Eye Movements and Lexical Access in Spoken-Language Comprehension: Evaluating a Linking Hypothesis Between Fixations and Linguistic Processing. Journal of Psycholinguistic Research 29.6: 557–80.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Taylor, T. 1997. Theorizing Language. Amsterdam: Pergamon.Google Scholar
Thierry, G., Athanasopoulos, P., Wiggett, A., Dering, B. and Kuipers, J.-R. 2009. Unconscious Effects of Language-Specific Terminology on Preattentive Color Perception. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences 106.11: 4567–70.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Thomas, M. 2002. Development of the Concept of “the Poverty of the Stimulus.”The Linguistic Review 19: 51–71.Google Scholar
Tobin, Y. 1990. Semiotics and Linguistics. London/New York: Longman.Google Scholar
Tomasello, M. 1995. Language Is Not an Instinct. Review of The Language Instinct. Cognitive Development 10: 131–56.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Tomasello, M. 1999. The Cultural Origins of Human Cognition. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press.Google Scholar
Tomasello, M. 2001. Could We Please Lose the Mapping Metaphor Please?Behavioral and Brain Sciences 24.6: 1119–20.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Tomasello, M. 2003. Constructing a Language: A Usage-Based Theory of Language Acquisition. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press.Google Scholar
Tomasello, M. 2008. Origins of Human Communication. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.Google Scholar
Tomasello, M. 2009. Why We Cooperate. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.Google Scholar
Tooby, J. and Cosmides, L. 1992. The Psychological Foundations of Culture. In Barkow, et al., pp. 19–136.
Tsuang, M., Stone, W. and Faraone, S. 2001. Genes, Environment and Schizophrenia. British Journal of Psychiatry 178 (suppl. 40): s18–s24.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Uexküll, J. von 1909. Umwelt und Innenwelt der Tiere. Berlin: Springer.Google Scholar
Venezia, J. and Hickok, G. 2009. Mirror Neurons, the Motor System and Language: From the Motor Theory to Embodied Cognition and Beyond. Language and Linguistics Compass 3/6: 1403–16.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Vološinov, V. N. 1973. Marxism and the Philosophy of Language, trans. Matejka, L. and Titunik, I. R.. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.Google Scholar
Vygotsky, L. 1962 [1934]. Thought and Language, ed. and trans. Hanfmann, E. and Vakar, G.. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Vygotsky, L. 1986 [1934]. Thought and Language, revised and edited by Kozulin, A.. Cambridge MA: MIT Press.Google Scholar
Waddington, C. 1957.The Strategy of the Genes: A Discussion of Some Aspects of Theoretical Biology. London: Allen & Unwin.Google Scholar
Waddington, C. 1960. The Ethical Animal. London: George Allen.Google Scholar
Waddington, C. 1975. The Evolution of an Evolutionist. Ithaca, NY: Cornell University Press.Google Scholar
Watson, J. B. 1931. Behaviorism, 2nd edn. London: Kegan Paul.Google Scholar
Whitney, W. D. 1869. On the Present Condition of the Question as to the Origin of Language. Transactions of the American Philological Society 1: 84–94.Google Scholar
Whorf, B. L. 1956. Language, Thought, and Reality. Selected Writings of Benjamin L. Whorf, edited and with an introduction by Carroll, J. B.. New York: John Wiley & Sons and The Technology Press of Massachusetts Institute of Technology.Google Scholar
Wickelgren, I. 2005. Autistic Brains Out of Sync?Science 308: 1856–58.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Wierzbicka, A. 1998. “Sadness” and “Anger” in Russian: The Non-Universality of So-Called “Basic Human Emotions.” In Speaking of Emotions: Conceptualization and Expression, ed. Athanasiadou, A. and Tabakowska, E., pp. 3–27. Berlin, New York: Mouton de Gruyter.Google Scholar
Wilson, R. and Keil, F. 1999. The MIT Encyclopedia of the Cognitive Sciences. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.Google Scholar
Winawer, J., Witthoft, N., Frank, M. et al. 2007. Russian Blues Reveal Effects of Language on Color Discrimination. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences 104.19: 7780–85.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Wittgenstein, L. 1997 [1953]. Philosophical Investigations, 2nd German/English edn., trans. Anscombe, G. E. M.. Oxford: Blackwell.Google Scholar
Woodward, J. and Cowie, F. 2004. The Mind Is Not (Just) a System of Modules Shaped (Just) by Natural Selection. In Contemporary Debates in Philosophy of Science, ed. Hitchcock, C., pp. 312–34. Malden, MA: Blackwell.Google Scholar

Save book to Kindle

To save this book to your Kindle, first ensure coreplatform@cambridge.org is added to your Approved Personal Document E-mail List under your Personal Document Settings on the Manage Your Content and Devices page of your Amazon account. Then enter the ‘name’ part of your Kindle email address below. Find out more about saving to your Kindle.

Note you can select to save to either the @free.kindle.com or @kindle.com variations. ‘@free.kindle.com’ emails are free but can only be saved to your device when it is connected to wi-fi. ‘@kindle.com’ emails can be delivered even when you are not connected to wi-fi, but note that service fees apply.

Find out more about the Kindle Personal Document Service.

  • Bibliography
  • Julie Tetel Andresen, Duke University, North Carolina
  • Book: Linguistics and Evolution
  • Online publication: 05 June 2014
  • Chapter DOI: https://doi.org/10.1017/CBO9781107325364.014
Available formats
×

Save book to Dropbox

To save content items to your account, please confirm that you agree to abide by our usage policies. If this is the first time you use this feature, you will be asked to authorise Cambridge Core to connect with your account. Find out more about saving content to Dropbox.

  • Bibliography
  • Julie Tetel Andresen, Duke University, North Carolina
  • Book: Linguistics and Evolution
  • Online publication: 05 June 2014
  • Chapter DOI: https://doi.org/10.1017/CBO9781107325364.014
Available formats
×

Save book to Google Drive

To save content items to your account, please confirm that you agree to abide by our usage policies. If this is the first time you use this feature, you will be asked to authorise Cambridge Core to connect with your account. Find out more about saving content to Google Drive.

  • Bibliography
  • Julie Tetel Andresen, Duke University, North Carolina
  • Book: Linguistics and Evolution
  • Online publication: 05 June 2014
  • Chapter DOI: https://doi.org/10.1017/CBO9781107325364.014
Available formats
×