Skip to main content Accessibility help
×
Hostname: page-component-788cddb947-kc5xb Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-10-10T20:24:17.876Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

References

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  05 March 2016

Rudolf Botha
Affiliation:
University of Stellenbosch, South Africa
Get access

Summary

Image of the first page of this content. For PDF version, please use the ‘Save PDF’ preceeding this image.'
Type
Chapter
Information
Language Evolution
The Windows Approach
, pp. 284 - 311
Publisher: Cambridge University Press
Print publication year: 2016

Access options

Get access to the full version of this content by using one of the access options below. (Log in options will check for institutional or personal access. Content may require purchase if you do not have access.)

References

Abe, K. and Wanatabe, D. (2011) Songbirds possess the ability to discriminate syntactic rules. Nature Neuroscience 14: 1067–74.Google Scholar
Adair, J. (1944) The Navajo and Pueblo silversmith. Norman, OK: University of Oklahoma Press.
Adger, D. (2013) Constructions and grammatical explanation: Comments on Goldberg. Mind & Language 28: 466–78.Google Scholar
Aiello, L.C. (1998) The foundation of human language. In Jablonski, N.G. and Aiello, L.C. (eds.), The origin and diversification of language. San Francisco, CA: California Academy of Sciences, 21–34.
Aiello, R. (1994) Music and language: Parallels and contrasts. In Aiello, R. and Sloboda, J.A. (eds.), Musical perceptions. Oxford University Press, 40–63.
Allen, M.R. (1978) Morphological investigations. Ph.D. diss., University of Connecticut.
Anderson, S.R. (2013) What is special about the human language faculty and how did it get that way? In Botha, R. and Everaert, M. (eds.), The evolutionary emergence of language: Evidence and inference. Oxford University Press, 18–41.
Arbib, M. (2005a) From monkey-like action recognition to human language: An evolutionary framework for neuro-linguistics. Behavioral and Brain Sciences 28: 105–67.Google Scholar
Arbib, M. (2005b) An action-oriented neurolinguistic framework for the evolution of protolanguage. In Tallerman, M. (ed.), Language origins: Perspectives on evolution. Oxford University Press, 182–200.
Arbib, M.A. and Iriki, A. (2013) Evolving the language- and music-ready brain. In Arbib, M.A. (ed.), Language, music, and the brain: A mysterious relationship. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press, 481–97.
Armstrong, E. (1982) Mosaic evolution in the primate brain: Differences and similarities in the hominid thalamus. In Armstrong, E. and Falk, D. (eds.), Primate brain evolution. New York: Plenum Press, 131–62.
Armstrong, E. (1986) Enlarged limbic structures in the human brain: The anterior thalamus and medial mammilliary body. Brain Research 362: 394–7.Google Scholar
Aronoff, M., Meir, I., Padden, C. and Sandler, W. (2004) Morphological universals and the sign language type. In Booij, J. and Marle, J.van (eds.), Yearbook of Morphology. Dordrecht/Boston: Kluwer Academic, 19–40.
Aronoff, M., Meir, I., Padden, C. and Sandler, W. (2008) The roots of linguistic organization in a new language. Interaction Studies: A Special Issue on Holophrasis vs. Compositionality in the Emergence of Protolanguage 9: 131–50.Google Scholar
Arthur, W. (2002) The emerging conceptual framework of evolutionary developmental biology. Nature 415: 757–64.Google Scholar
Baker, P. (1990) Off target. Journal of Pidgin and Creole Languages 5: 107–19.Google Scholar
Baker, P. (1994) Creativity in creole genesis. In Adone, D. and Plag, I. (eds.), Creolization and language change. Tübingen: Max Niemeyer, 65–84.
Baker, P. (1995) Motivation in creole genesis. In Baker, P. (ed.), From contact to creole and beyond. London: University of Westminister Press, 3–15.
Baker, P. (1997) Directionality in pidginization and creolization. In Spears, A.K. and Winford, D. (eds.), The structure and status of pidgins and creoles. Amsterdam: John Benjamins, 91–109.
Baker, P. (2000) Theories of creolization and the degree and nature of restructuring. In Neumann-Holzschuh, I. and Schneider, E.W. (eds.), Degrees of restructuring in creole languages. Amsterdam: John Benjamins, 41–63.
Bakker, P. (1987) Autonomous languages: Signed and spoken languages created by children in the light of Bickerton's Bioprogram Hypothesis. Publikaties van het Instituut voor Algemene Taalwetenschap, No. 53. University of Amsterdam.
Bakker, P. (1995) Pidgins. In Arends, J., Muysken, P. and Smith, N. (eds.), Pidgins and creoles: An introduction. Amsterdam: John Benjamins Publishing Company, 25–39.
Bakker, P. (2003) Pidgins as a window on the genesis of language. Paper presented at the Seminar on Windows on Language Genesis, The Netherlands Institute for Advanced Study, Wassenaar, 7–8 November 2003.
Bakker, P. (2006) Twins’ languages as windows on language genesis. Paper presented at The Netherlands Institute for Advanced Study, Wassenaar, 13 June 2006.
Balari, S., Benítez-Burraco, A., Camps, M., Longa, V.C., Lorenzo, G. and Uriagereka, L. (2011) The archaeological record speaks: Bridging anthropology and linguistics. International Journal of Evolutionary Biology, Volume 2011, Article 382679, 1–17. doi: 10.4061/2011/382679Google Scholar
Baldwin, J.M. (1894) The development of the child and of the race. New York: Macmillan.
Bambini, V., Chesi, C. and Moro, A. (2012) A conversation with Noam Chomsky: New insights on old foundations. www.phenomenologyandmind.eu/wp-content/uploads/2012/12/16-Intervista-CHOMSKY.pdf, 221–39. (Also published in Phenomenology and Mind 3: 166–79.)
Barceló-Colbijn, L. and Benítez-Burraco, A. (2013) Disentangling the Neanderthal net: A comment on Johansson (2013)Biolinguistics 7: 199–216.Google Scholar
Barham, L.S. (2002) Backed tools in Middle Pleistocene central Africa and their evolutionary significance. Journal of Human Evolution 43: 583–603.Google Scholar
Bartha, P.F. (2010) By parallel reasoning: The construction and evaluation of analogical arguments. Oxford University Press.
Bartha, P.F. (2013) Analogy and analogical reasoning. In E.N. Zalta (ed.), The Stanford Encyclopedia of Philosophy, Fall edn. http://plato.stanford.edu/ archives/fall2013/entries/reasoning-analogy/.
Baynes, K. and Gazzaniga, M.S. (2005) Lateralization of language: Toward a biologically based model of language. The Linguistic Review 22: 303–26.Google Scholar
Beerbohm, M. (1946) Fenestralia. Mainly on the air. London: William Heineman.
Benazzo, S. (2009) The emergence of temporality: From restricted linguistic systems to early human language. In Botha, R. and Swart, H.de (eds.), Language evolution: The view from restricted linguistic systems. Utrecht: LOT Occasional Series, 21–57.
Benítez-Burraco, A. and Boeckx, C. (2013) Language disorders and language evolution: Constraints on hypotheses. Biological Theory 9: 1–6.Google Scholar
Benzon, W.L. (2001) Beethoven's anvil: Music in mind and culture. Oxford University Press.
Benzon, W.L. (2005) Synch, song, and society. Review of S. Mithen, The singing Neanderthals: The origins of music, language, mind and body, London: Weidenfeld & Nicholson, 2005. Human Nature Review 5: 66–8.Google Scholar
Bertossa, R.C. (2011) Morphology and behaviour: Functional links in development and evolution. Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society B. Biological Sciences 366: 2056–68.Google Scholar
Berwick, R.C. (2012) Me Tarzan, you Jane. Review of J.R. Hurford, The origins of grammar: Language in the light of evolution, Oxford University Press, 2012. Science 336: 158.Google Scholar
Berwick, R.C. and Chomsky, N. (2011) The biolinguistic program: The current state of its development. In Di Sciullo, A.M. and Boeckx, C. (eds.), The biolinguistic enterprise: New perspectives on the evolution and nature of the human language faculty. Oxford University Press, 19–41.
Berwick, R.C., Hauser, M.D., and Tattersall, I. (2013a). Neanderthal language? Just-so stories take center stage. Frontiers in Psychology 4, Article 671, 1–2.Google Scholar
Berwick, R.C., Friederici, A.D., Chomsky, N., and Bolhuis, J.J. (2013b). Evolution, brain, and the nature of language. Trends in Cognitive Sciences 17: 89–98.Google Scholar
Berwick, R.C., Pietroski, P., Yankama, B. and Chomsky, N. (2011) Poverty of the stimulus. Cognitive Science 35: 1207–42Google Scholar
Besson, M. and Schön, D. (2001) Comparison between language and music. In Peretz, I. and Zatorre, R.J. (eds.), The cognitive neuroscience of music. Oxford University Press, 269–93.
Bickerton, D. (1981) Roots of language. Ann Arbor: Karoma.
Bickerton, D. (1984) The language biogram hypothesis. Behavioral and Brain Sciences 7: 173–88, 212–21.Google Scholar
Bickerton, D. (1990) Language and species. University of Chicago Press.
Bickerton, D. (1995) Language and human behavior. Seattle: University of Washington Press.
Bickerton, D. (1998) Catastrophic evolution: The case for a single step from protolanguage to full human language. In Hurford, J., Studdert-Kennedy, M. and Knight, C. (eds.), Approaches to the evolution of language: Social and cognitive bases. Cambridge University Press, 341–58.
Bickerton, D. (2002) From protolanguage to language. In Crow, T.J. (ed.), The speciation of modern Homo Sapiens. Oxford University Press, 103–20.
Bickerton, D. (2003) Symbol and structure: A comprehensive framework for language evolution. In Tallerman, M. (ed.), Language origins: Perspectives on evolution. Oxford University Press, 77–93.
Bickerton, D. (2004) Mothering plus vocalization doesn't equal language. Behavioral and Brain Sciences 27: 504–5.Google Scholar
Bickerton, D. (2005) Language first, then shared intentionality, then a beneficent spiral. Behavioral and Brain Sciences 28: 691–2.Google Scholar
Bickerton, D. (2007a) Language evolution: A brief guide for linguists. Lingua 117: 510–26.Google Scholar
Bickerton, D. (2007b) Review of M. Tallerman (ed.), Language origins: perspectives on evolution. Oxford University Press, 2005. Journal of Linguistics 43: 259–64.Google Scholar
Bickerton, D. (2009a) Adam's tongue: How humans made language, how language made humans. New York: Hill and Wang.
Bickerton, D. (2009b) Syntax for non-syntacticians: A brief primer. In Bickerton, D. and Szathmáry, E. (eds.), Biological foundations and origin of syntax. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press, 3–13.
Bickerton, D. (2009c) Bastard tongues: A trailblazing linguist finds clues to our common humanity in the world's lowliest languages. New York: Hill and Wang.
Bickerton, D. (2010) On two incompatible theories of language evolution. In Larson, R., Deprez, V. and Yamakido, H. (eds.), The evolution of human language: biolinguistic perspectives. Cambridge University Press, 199–210.
Bickerton, D. (2012) The origins of syntactic language. In Tallerman, M. and Gibson, K.R. (eds.), The Oxford handbook of language evolution. Oxford University Press, 456–68.
Binford, L.R. (1981) Bones: ancient men and modern myths. Studies in archaeology. New York: Academic Press.
Binford, S.R. (1968) Ethnographic data and understanding the Pleistocene. In Lee, R.B. and DeVore, I. (eds.), Man the hunter. Chicago: Aldine Publishing Company, 274–5.
Blaxland, B. (2012) Hominid and hominin – what's the difference? http://australianmuseum.net.au/Hominid-and-hominin-whats-the-difference/.
Bloom, L. (1976) Discussion paper: Child language and the origins of language. Annals of the New York Academy of Sciences 280: 170–2.Google Scholar
Bloomfield, T.C., Gentner, T.Q. and Margoliash, D. (2011) What birds say about language. Nature Neuroscience 14: 947–8.Google Scholar
Blurton Jones, N.G. and Konner, M. (1976) !Kung knowledge of animal behaviour. In Lee, R.B. and deVore, I. (eds.), Kalahari hunter-gatherers. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press, 325–48.
Boeckx, C. and Benítez-Burraco, A. (2014) The shape of the human language-ready brain. Frontiers in Psychology 5: 1–23.Google Scholar
Bolhuis, J., Tattersall, I., Chomsky, N. and Berwick, R.C. (2014) How could language have evolved? PLOS Biology. doi: 10:1371/journal.pbio.1001934
Borseley, R.D. and Newmeyer, F.J. (2009) On Subject-Auxiliary-Inversion and the notion ‘purely formal generalization’. Cognitive Linguistics 20: 35–143.Google Scholar
Botha, R. (2000) Discussing the evolution of the assorted beasts called ‘language’. Language & Communication 20: 149–60.Google Scholar
Botha, R. (2001) On the role of bridge theories in accounts of the evolution of human language. Language & Communication 21: 61–71.Google Scholar
Botha, R. (2003) Unravelling the evolution of language. Amsterdam: Elsevier.
Botha, R. (2006a) Pidgin languages as a putative window on language evolution. Language & Communication 26: 1–14.Google Scholar
Botha, R. (2006b) On the Windows Approach to language evolution. Language & Communication 26: 129–43.Google Scholar
Botha, R. (2007) On homesign systems as a window on language evolution. Language & Communication 27: 41–52.Google Scholar
Botha, R. (2008a) Prehistoric shell beads as a window on language evolution. Language & Communication 28: 197–212.Google Scholar
Botha, R. (2008b) On modelling prelinguistic evolution in early hominins. Language & Communication 28: 258–75.Google Scholar
Botha, R. (2009a) What are windows on language evolution? In Botha, R. and Swart, H.de (eds.), Language evolution: The view from restricted linguistic systems. Utrecht: LOT Occasional Series, 1–20.
Botha, R. (2009b) Theoretical underpinnings of inferences about language evolution. In Botha, R. and Knight, C. (eds.), The cradle of language. Oxford University Press, 93–111.
Botha, R. (2009c) On ‘musilanguage’/‘hmmmmm’ as an evolutionary precursor to language. Language & Communication 29: 61–76.Google Scholar
Botha, R. (2010) On the soundness of inferring modern language from symbolic behaviour. Cambridge Archaeological Journal 20: 345–56.Google Scholar
Botha, R. (2011) Constraining the arbitrariness of exaptationist accounts of the evolution of language. Lingua 121: 1552–63.Google Scholar
Botha, R. (2012a) Inferring modern language from ancient objects. In Tallerman, M. and Gibson, K.R. (eds.), The Oxford handbook of language evolution. Oxford University Press, 303–12.
Botha, R. (2012b) Protolanguage and the ‘God particle’. Lingua 122: 1308–24.Google Scholar
Bouissac, P. (2004) How plausible is the motherese hypothesis?Behavioral and Brain Sciences 27: 506–7.Google Scholar
Brainerd, C.J. (1979) Recapitulationism, Piaget, and the evolution of intelligence: Déjà vu. The Behavioral and Brain Sciences 2: 381–2.Google Scholar
Brown, S. (2001) The ‘musilanguage’ model of music evolution. In Wallin, N.L., Merker, B. and Brown, S. (eds.), The origins of music. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press, 271–300.
Bouzouggar, A., Barton, N. and Vanhaeren, M.et al. (2007) 82,000-year-old shell beads from North Africa and implications for the origin of modern behavior. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America 104: 9964–9.Google Scholar
Burling, R. (2005) The talking ape: How language evolved. Oxford University Press.
Bybee, J. (2012) Domain-general processes as the basis for grammar. In Tallerman, M. and Gibson, K.R. (eds.), The Oxford handbook of language evolution. Oxford University Press, 528–36.
Calvin, W.H. and Bickerton, D. (2000) Lingua ex machina: Reconciling Darwin and Chomsky with the human brain. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.
Cameron, D.W. (1993) Uniformitarianism and prehistoric archaeology. Australian Archaeology 36: 42–9.Google Scholar
Camps, M. and Uriagereka, J. (2006) The Gordian knot of linguistic fossils. In Rossellό, J. and Martín, J. (eds.), The biolinguistic turn: Issues on language and biology. Barcelona: PPU, Promociones y Publicasiones Universitarias, SA, 34–65.
Caplan, D. (2003) Neurolinguistics. In Aronoff, M. and Rees-Miller, J. (eds.), The handbook of linguistics. Oxford: Blackwell Publishers, 582–607.
Caplan, D. (2009) Neural organization for syntactic processing as determined by effects of lesions: Logic, data, and difficult questions. In Bickerton, D. and Szathmáry, E. (eds.), Biological foundations and origin of syntax. Cambridge, MA and London: MIT Press, 253–77.
Case, R. (1985) Intellectual development: Birth and adulthood. London: Academic Press.
Chase, P.G. (1999) Symbolism as reference and symbolism as culture. In Dunbar, R., Knight, C. and Power, C. (eds.), The evolution of culture. An interdisciplinary view. Edinburgh University Press, 34–49.
Chase, P.G. and Dibble, L.K. (1987) Middle Palaeolithic symbolism: A review of current evidence and interpretation. Journal of Anthropological Archaeology 6: 263–96.Google Scholar
Chater, N. and Christiansen, M.H. (2012) A solution to the logical problem of language evolution: Language as an adaptation to the human brain. In Tallerman, M. and Gibson, K.R. (eds.), The Oxford handbook of language evolution. Oxford University Press, 626–39.
Chomsky, N. (1975) Reflections on language. Glasgow: Fontana/Collins.
Chomsky, N. (1980) Rules and representations. New York: Columbia University Press.
Chomsky, N. (1986) Knowledge of language: Its nature, origin and use. New York: Praeger.
Chomsky, N. (1988) Language and problems of knowledge: The Managua Lectures. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.
Chomsky, N. (2007) Biolinguistic explorations: Design, development, evolution. International Journal of Philosophical Studies 15: 1–21.Google Scholar
Chomsky, N. (2009) Opening remarks. In Piattelli-Palmarini, M., Uriagereka, J. and Salaburo, P. (eds.), On mind and language: A dialogue with Noam Chomsky in the Basque Country. Oxford University Press, 13–41.
Chomsky, N. (2011) Language and other cognitive systems. What is special about language?Language Learning and Development 7: 263–78.Google Scholar
Chomsky, N. (2012) The minimalist program and language acquisition. Lectio Magistralis given on the occasion of the Honorary Doctorate Degree in Neuroscience awarded by the Scuola Internazionale Superiore di Studi Avanzati, 17 September 2012, Trieste. (Page references to typescript.)
Chomsky, N. (2014) Language architecture and its import for evolution. Paper presented at the Academy Colloquium on ‘Biology of Language: Evolution, Brain, Development’, Amsterdam, 10–13 December 2014. (Page references to the manuscript available on the internet.)
Chomsky, N. and Halle, M. (1965) Some controversial questions in phonological theory. Journal of Linguistics 1: 97–138.Google Scholar
Chomsky, N. and Halle, M. (1968) The sound pattern of English. New York: Harper and Row.
Chomsky, N., Halle, M. and Lukoff, F. (1956) On accent and juncture in English. In Halle, M., Lunt, H., McClean, H. and Schooneveld, C.van (eds.), For Roman Jakobson: Essays on the occasion of his sixtieth birthday. The Hague: Mouton, 65–80.
Christiansen, M.H. (2013) Language has evolved to depend on multiple-cue integration. In Botha, R. and Everaert, M. (eds.), The evolutionary emergence of language: Evidence and inference. Oxford University Press, 42–61.
Christy, C. (1983) Uniformitarianism in linguistics. Amsterdam/Philadelphia: John Benjamins.
Clune, J., Pennock, R.T., Ofria, C. and Lenski, R.E. (2012) Ontogeny tends to recapitulate phylogeny in digital organisms. The American Naturalist 180: E54–E63.Google Scholar
Comrie, B. (2003) Reconstruction, typology and reality. In Hickey, R. (ed.), Motives for language change. Cambridge University Press, 243–57.
Coolidge, F.L. and Wynn, T. (2011) Commentary on Henshilwood and Dubreuil (2011). Current Anthropology 52: 380–2.Google Scholar
Copi, I.M. (1968) Introduction to logic, . New York: Macmillan.
Craine, S., Thornton, R. and Khlentzos, D. (2009) The case of the missing generalizations. Cognitive Linguistics 20: 145–55.Google Scholar
Cross, I. (2001)Review of N.L. Wallin, B. Merker, and S. Brown (eds.), The origins of music. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press, 2000. Music Perception 18: 513–21.Google Scholar
Culicover, P.W. (2009) Natural language syntax. Oxford University Press.
Darwin, C. (1871) The descent of man and selection in relation to sex. London: John Murray.
Darwin, C. (1903) On the origins of species by natural selection, or, the preservation of favoured races in the struggle for life. London: Watts.
Davidson, I. (2003) The archaeological evidence of language origins: State of the art. In Christiansen, M.H. and Kirby, S. (eds.), Language evolution. Oxford University Press, 140–57.
Davidson, I. (2011) Commentary on Henshilwood and Dubreuil (2011). Current Anthropology 52: 382–3.Google Scholar
Deacon, T.W. (1991) Anatomy of hierarchical information processing. Behavioral and Brain Sciences 14: 555–7.Google Scholar
Deacon, T.W. (1997) The symbolic species: The co-evolution of language and the brain. New York: Norton.
de Boer, B. (2012) Infant-directed speech and language evolution. In Tallerman, M. and Gibson, K.R. (eds.), The Oxford handbook of language evolution. Oxford University Press, 322–7.
Dediu, D. and Levinson, S.C. (2013) On the antiquity of language: The reinterpretation of Neandertal linguistic capacities and its consequences. Frontiers in Psychology 4, Article 397, 1–17.Google Scholar
den Besten, H. (1986) Double negation and the genesis of Afrikaans. In Muysken, P. and Smith, N. (eds.), Substrata versus universals in Creole genesis. Amsterdam: John Benjamins, 185–230.
den Besten, H. (1987) Die niederländische Pidgins der alten Kapkolonie. In Boretzky, N., Enninger, W. and Stolz, T. (eds.), Beiträge zum 3. Essener Kolloquium über Sprachwandel und seine bestimmenden Faktoren vom 30.9 – 2.10.1986 an der Universität Essen. Bochum: Studienverlag Dr. N. Bockmeyer, 9–40.
den Besten, H. (1996) Double negation and the genesis of Afrikaans. In Muysken, P. and Smith, N. (eds.), Substrata versus universals in creole genesis. Amsterdam: John Benjamins, 185–230.
d'Errico, F. and Henshilwood, C.S. (2007) Additional evidence for bone technology in the southern African Middle Stone Age. Journal of Human Evolution 52: 142–63.Google Scholar
d'Errico, F. and Vanhaeren, M. (2009) Earliest personal ornaments and their significance for the origins of language. In Botha, R. and Knight, C. (eds.), The cradle of language. Oxford University Press, 1–40.
d'Errico, F. and Vanhaeren, M. (2012.) Linguistic implications of the earliest personal ornaments. In Tallerman, M. and Gibson, K.R. (eds.), The Oxford handbook of language evolution. Oxford University Press, 299–302.
d'Errico, F., Henshilwood, C.F. and Nilsen, P. (2001) An engraved bone fragment from ca. 75 Kya Middle Stone Age levels at Blombos Cave, South Africa: Implications for the origins of symbolism. Antiquity 75: 309–18.Google Scholar
d'Errico, F., Vanhaeren, M. and Wadley, L. (2008) Possible shell beads from the Middle Stone Age layers of Sibudu Cave, South Africa. Journal of Archaeological Science 35: 2675–85.Google Scholar
d'Errico, F., Henshilwood, C., Vanhaeren, M. and van Niekerk, K. (2005) Nassarius kraussianus shell beads from Blombos Cave: Evidence for symbolic behaviour in the Middle Stone Age. Journal of Human Evolution 48: 3–24.Google Scholar
d'Errico, F., Vanhaeren, M., Barton, N., Bouzouggar, A., Mienis, H., Richter, D., Hublin, J.-J., Mcpherron, P. and Lozouet, P. (2009a) Additional evidence on the use of personal ornaments in the Middle Paleolithic of North Africa. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America 106 (38): 16051–6.Google Scholar
d'Errico, F., Vanhaeren, M., Henshilwood, C., Lawson, G., Maureille, B., Gambier, D., Tillier, A.-M., Soressi, M. and van Niekerk, K. (2009b) From the origin of language to the diversification of languages: What can anthropology and palaeoanthropology say? In d'Errico, F. and Hombert, J.-M. (eds.), Becoming eloquent: Advances in the emergence of language, human cognition, and modern cultures. Amsterdam/Philadelphia: John Benjamins, 13–67.
d'Errico, F., Henshilwood, C., Lawson, G., Vanhaeren, M., Tllier, A.-M., Soressi, M., Bresson, F., Maureille, B., Nowell, A., Lakkara, J., Blackwell, C. and Julien, M. (2003) Archaeological evidence for the emergence of language, symbolism and music – An alternative multidisciplinary perspective. Journal of World Prehistory 17: 1–70.Google Scholar
de Sousa, A. and Cunha, E. (2012) Hominins and the emergence of the modern human brain. Progress in Brain Research 195: 293–321.Google Scholar
de Waal, F.B.M. and Pollick, A.S. (2012) Gesture as the most flexible modality of primate communication. In Tallerman, M. and Gibson, K.R. (eds.), The Oxford handbook of language evolution. Oxford University Press, 82–9.
Deyhle, D. (1986) Success and failure: A micro-ethnographic comparison of Navajo and Anglo students’ perceptions of testing. Curriculum Inquiry 16: 365–89.Google Scholar
Diller, K.C. and Cann, R.L. (2013) Genetics, evolution, and the innateness of language. In Botha, R. and Everaert, M. (eds.), The evolutionary emergence of language: Evidence and inference. Oxford University Press, 244–58.
Dincauze, D.F. (2000) Environmental archaeology: Principles and practice. Cambridge University Press.
Dingwall, W.O. (1995) Complex behaviors: Evolution and the brain. Behavioral and Brain Sciences 18: 186–8.Google Scholar
Dissanayake, E. (2001) Antecedents of the temporal arts in early mother-infant interaction. In Wallin, N.L., Merker, B. and Brown, S. (eds.), The origins of music. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press, 394–410.
Donald, M. (1991) Origins of the modern mind: Three stages in the evolution of culture and cognition. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press.
Donald, M. (1995) Neurolinguistic models and fossil reconstructions. Behavioral and Brain Sciences 18: 188–9.Google Scholar
Doupe, A.J. and Kuhl, P.K. (1999) Birdsong and human speech: Common themes and mechanisms. Annual Review of Neuroscience 22: 567–631.Google Scholar
Douven, I. (2011) Peirce on abduction. In E.N. Zalta (ed.), The Stanford Encyclopedia of Philosophy, Spring 2011 edn. http://plato.stanford.edu/archives/spr2011/ entries/abduction/.
Dubin, L.S. (1987) The history of beads, from 30,000 B.C. to the present. New York: Harry N. Abrams.
Dubreuil, B. (2010) Human evolution and the origin of hierarchies. Cambridge University Press. (Referred to as ‘Dubreuil 2010a’ by Henshilwood and Dubreuil (2011) in Chapter 3.)
Dunbar, R. (2005) Play the ape. Review of S. Mithen, The singing Neanderthals: The origins of music, language, mind and body. London: Weidenfeld & Nicholson, 2005. Times Literary Supplement, 29 July, 5–6.
Erwin, D.H. (2010) Microevolution and macroevolution are not governed by the same process. In Ayala, F.J. and Arp, R. (eds.), Contemporary debates in philosophy and biology. Malden, MA: Wiley-Blackwell, 190–8.
Erwin, D.H. (2011) Evolutionary uniformitarianism. Developmental Biology 357: 27–34.Google Scholar
Ettinger, G. (1979) Does development tell us about evolution?Behavioral and Brain Sciences 2: 384–5.Google Scholar
Everett, D. (2005) Cultural constraints on grammar and cognition in Pirahã: Another look at the design features of human language. Current Anthropology 46: 621–46.Google Scholar
Everett, D. (2009) Pirahã culture and grammar: A response to some criticisms. Language 85: 405–42.Google Scholar
Falk, D. (1980) A reanalysis of the South African australopithecine natural endocasts. American Journal of Physical Anthropology 53: 525–39.Google Scholar
Falk, D. (1983a) The Taung endocast: A reply to Holloway. American Journal of Physical Anthropology 60: 479–80.Google Scholar
Falk, D. (1983b) Cerebral cortex of East African early hominids. Science 221: 1072–4.Google Scholar
Falk, D. (1985) Apples, oranges and the lunate sulcus. American Journal of Physical Anthropology 67: 313–15.Google Scholar
Falk, D. (2001) Hominid brain evolution and the origin of music. In Wallin, N.L., Merker, B. and Brown, S. (eds.), The origins of music. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press, 197–216.
Falk, D. (2004) Prelinguistic evolution in early hominins: Whence motherese?Behavioral and Brain Sciences 27: 491–503, 526–41.Google Scholar
Falk, D. (2007) Evolution of the primate brain. In Henke, W. and Tattersall, I. (eds.), Handbook of palaeoanthropology, Volume 2: Primate evolution and human origins. Heidelberg: Springer-Verlag, 1133–62.
Falk, D. (2009) Finding our tongues: Mothers, infants and the origins of language. New York: Basic Books.
Falk, D. (2011) The fossil chronicles: How two controversial discoveries changed our view of human evolution. Berkeley: University of California Press.
Falk, D. (2012a) Hominin paleoneurology: Where are we now?Progress in Brain Research 195: 255–72.Google Scholar
Falk, D. (2012b) The role of hominin mothers and infants in prelinguistic evolution. In Tallerman, M. and Gibson, K.R. (eds.), The Oxford handbook of language evolution. Oxford University Press, 318–21.
Falk, D. (2014) Interpreting sulci on hominin endocasts: Old hypotheses and new findings. Frontiers in human neuroscience 8: 1–12. doi: 10.3389/fnhum.2014.00134Google Scholar
Fann, K.T. (1970) Peirce's theory of abduction. The Hague: Martinus Nijhoff.
Farah, M.J. (1994) Neuropsychological inference with an interactive brain: A critique of the ‘locality’ assumption. Behavioral and Brain Sciences 17: 43–104.Google Scholar
Fernald, A. (1994) Human maternal vocalizations to infants as biologically relevant signals: An evolutionary perspective. In Bloom, P. (ed.), Language acquisition core readings. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press, 51–94.
Fitch, W.T. (2000) The evolution of speech: A comparative review. Trends in Cognitive Science 4: 258–67.Google Scholar
Fitch, W.T. (2005a) The evolution of language: A comparative review. Biology and Philosophy 20: 193–230.Google Scholar
Fitch, W.T. (2005b) Dancing to Darwin's tune. Nature 438: 288.Google Scholar
Fitch, W.T. (2010a) The evolution of language. Cambridge University Press.
Fitch, W.T. (2010b) Three meanings of ‘recursion’: Key distinctions for biolinguistics. In Larson, R., Deprez, V. and Yamakido, H. (eds.), The evolution of human language: Biolinguistic perspectives. Cambridge University Press, 73–90.
Fitch, W.T. (2012a) The biology and evolution of rhythm: Unravelling a paradox. In Rebuschat, P., Rohrmeier, M. and Cross, I. (eds.), Language and music as cognitive systems. Oxford University Press, 73–95.
Fitch, W.T. (2012b) Reweaving the strands: Welcoming diverse perspectives on the biology of music. In Rebuschat, P., Rohrmeier, M. and Cross, I. (eds.), Language and music as cognitive systems. Oxford University Press, 128–34.
Fitch, W.T. (2012c) Evolutionary developmental biology and human evolution: Constraints on adaptation. Evolutionary Biology 39: 613–37.Google Scholar
Fitch, W.T. (2013) Musical protolanguage: Darwin's theory of language evolution revisited. In Bolhuis, J.J. and Everaert, M. (eds.), Birdsong, speech, and language: Exploring the evolution of mind and brain. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press, 489–503.
Fitch, W.T. and Hauser, M.D. (2004) Computational constraints on syntactic processing in a nonhuman primate. Science 303: 377–80.Google Scholar
Fitch, W.T. and Jarvis, E.D. (2013) Birdsong and other animal models for human speech, song, and vocal learning. In Arbib, M.A. (ed.), Language, music, and the brain: A mysterious relationship. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press, 499–539.
Fitch, W.T. and Mietchen, D. (2013) Convergence and deep homology in the evolution of spoken language. In Bolhuis, J.J. and Everaert, M. (eds.) Birdsong, speech, and language: Exploring the evolution of mind and brain. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press, 45–62.
Fitch, W.T., Hauser, M.D. and Chomsky, N. (2005) The evolution of the language faculty: Clarifications and implications. Cognition 97(2): 179–210.Google Scholar
Fodor, J. (2000) The mind doesn't work that way: The scope and limits of computational psychology. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.
Fodor, J. (2005) Give me that juicy bit over there. Review of S. Mithen, The singing Neanderthals: The origins of music, language, mind and body, London: Weidenfeld & Nicholson, 2005. London Review of Books, 6 October, 28–9.
Fogel, A. (1993) Developing through relationships: Origins of communication, self and culture. Harvester Wheatsheaf Press/University of Chicago Press.
Foley, R. (1991) Hominids, humans and hunter-gatherers: An evolutionary perspective. In Ingold, T., Riches, D. and Woodburn, J. (eds.), Hunters and gatherers 1: History, evolution and social change. New York/Oxford: Berg, 207–21.
Friederici, A.D. (2011) The brain basis of language processing: From structure to function. Physiological Reviews 91: 1357–92.Google Scholar
Gamble, C. (2006) Musical chairs. Commentary on S. Mithen, The singing Neanderthals: The origins of music, language, mind and body, London: Weidenfeld & Nicholson, 2005. Cambridge Archaeological Journal 16: 108–9.Google Scholar
Gärdenfors, P. (2011) Commentary on Henshilwood and Dubreuil (2011). Current Anthropology 52: 383–4.Google Scholar
Gärdenfors, P. (2013) The evolution of semantics: Sharing conceptual domains. In Botha, R. and Everaert, M. (eds.), The evolutionary emergence of language: Evidence and inference. Oxford University Press, 139–59.
Gentner, T.Q., Fenn, K.M., Margoliash, D. and Nusbaum, H.C. (2006) Recursive syntactic pattern learning by songbirds. Nature 440: 1204–7.Google Scholar
Gibson, K. (1999) The ontogeny and evolution of the brain, cognition, and language. In Lock, A. and Peters, C.R. (eds.), Handbook of symbolic evolution. Malden, MA: Blackwell Publishers, 407–31.
Gibson, K. (2012) Not the cortex alone: Other brain structures also contribute to speech and language. In Tallerman, M. and Gibson, K.R. (eds.), The Oxford handbook of language evolution. Oxford University Press, 176–9.
Gil, D. (2005) Early human language was isolating-monocategorial-associational. In Cohen, H. and Lefebre, C. (eds.), Categorization in cognitive science. Oxford: Elsevier, 347–79.
Gil, D. (2008a) How much grammar does it take to sail a boat? (Or, what can material artefacts tell us about the evolution of language?) In Smith, A.D.M., Smith, K. and Cancho, R.Ferrer I (eds.), The evolution of language. Hackensack, NJ: World Scientific, 123–30.
Gil, D. (2008b) How complex are isolating languages? In Miestamo, M., Sinnemäki, K. and Karlsson, F. (eds.), Language complexity: Typology, contact, change. Amsterdam: John Benjamins, 109–31.
Gil, D. (2010) How much grammar does it take to sail a boat? In Sampson, G., Gil, D. and Trudgill, P. (eds.), Language complexity as an evolutionary variable. Oxford University Press, 19–33.
Gilbert, S.F. (2006) Ernst Haeckel and the Biogenetic Law. Developmental Biology, online. Sinauer Asssociates. http://9e.devbio.com/article.php?id=219
Givón, T. (1995) Functionalism and grammar. Amsterdam: John Benjamins Publishing Company.
Givón, T. (1998) On the co-evolution of language, mind and brain. Evolution of Communication 2: 45–116.Google Scholar
Givón, T. (2002) Bio-Linguistics: The Santa Barbara Lectures. Amsterdam: John Benjamins.
Givón, T. (2009) The genesis of syntactic complexity: Diachrony, ontogeny, neuro-cognition, evolution. Amsterdam/Philadelphia: John Benjamins.
Goldberg, A.E. (2006) Constructions at work: The nature of generalization in language. Oxford University Press.
Goldberg, A.E. (2009a) The nature of generalization in language. Cognitive Linguistics 20: 93–127.Google Scholar
Goldberg, A.E. (2009b) Constructions work. Cognitive Linguistics 20: 201–24.Google Scholar
Goldberg, A.E. (2013a) Constructionist approaches. In Hoffmann, T. and Trousdale, G. (eds.), The Oxford handbook of construction grammar. Oxford University Press, 15–31.
Goldberg, A.E. (2013b) Explanation and constructions: Response to Adger. Mind & Language 28: 479–91.Google Scholar
Goldin-Meadow, S. (1993) When does gesture become language? A study of gesture used as a primary communication system by deaf children of hearing parents. In Gibson, K.R. and Ingold, T. (eds.), Tools, language, and cognition in human evolution. New York: Cambridge University Press, 63–85.
Goldin-Meadow, S. (2002) Getting a handle on language creation. In Givόn, T. and Malle, B.F. (eds.), The evolution of language out of pre-language. Amsterdam/Philadelphia: John Benjamins, 343–74.
Goldin-Meadow, S. (2003) The resilience of language: What gesture creation in deaf children can tell us about how all children learn language. New York and Hove: Psychology Press.
Goldin-Meadow, S. (2005) What language creation in the manual modality tells us about the foundations of language. The Linguistic Review 22: 199–225.Google Scholar
Goldin-Meadow, S. (2007) On inventing language. Dædalus, Summer: 1–4.
Goldin-Meadow, S. (2012) What modern-day gesture can tell us about the evolution of language. In Tallerman, M. and Gibson, K.R. (eds.), The Oxford handbook of language evolution. Oxford University Press, 545–57.
Goldin-Meadow, S. and Feldman, H. (1977) The development of language-like communication without a language model. Science 197: 401–3.Google Scholar
Goldin-Meadow, S. and Mylander, C. (1998) Spontaneous sign systems created by deaf children in two cultures. Nature 391: 279–81.Google Scholar
Gontier, N. (2008) On the different applications of Haeckel's biogenetic law in language origin and evolution studies. In Kern, S.. Gayraud, F. and Marsico, E. (eds.), Emergence of linguistic abilities. Newcastle: Cambridge Scholars Publishing Ltd., 12–29
Goodale, J.C. (1971) Tiwi wives: A study of the women of Melville Island, North Australia. Seattle, WA: University of Washington Press.
Gould, R.A. (1978) Beyond analogy in ethnoarchaeology. In Gould, R.A. (ed.), Explorations in ethnoarchaeology. Albuquerque: University of New Mexico Press, 249–93.
Gould, S.J. (1965) Is uniformitarianism necessary?American Journal of Science 263: 223–8.Google Scholar
Gould, S.J. (1977) Ontogeny and phylogeny. Cambridge, MA: Belknap Press of Harvard University Press.
Gould, S.J. (1987) Time's arrow, time's cycle: Myth and metaphor in the discovery of geological time. Cambridge, MA and London: Harvard University Press.
Gould, S.J. and Vrba, E.S. (1982) Exaptation – a missing term in the science of form. Paleobiology 8: 4–15.Google Scholar
Graves, P. (1994) Flakes and ladders: What the archaeological record cannot tell us about the origins of language. World Archaeology 26: 158–71.Google Scholar
Greenfield, P.M. (1991) Language, tools and brain: The ontogeny and phylogeny of hierarchically organized sequential behaviour. Behavioral and Brain Sciences 14: 531–51, 577–95.Google Scholar
Grodzinsky, Y. and Santi, A. (2008) The battle for Broca's region. Trends in Cognitive Sciences 12: 474–80.Google Scholar
Haeckel, E. (1866) Generelle Morphologie der Organismen: Allgemeine Grudzüge der organischen Formen-Wissenschaft, mechanisch begründet durch die von Charles Darwin formierte Descendenz-Theorie. Berlin: Georg Reimer.
Hagoort, P. (2005) On Broca, brain, and binding: A new framework. Trends in Cognitive Sciences 9: 416–23.Google Scholar
Hagoort, P. (2009) Reflections on the neurobiology of syntax. In Bickerton, D. and Szathmáry, E. (eds.), Biological foundations and origin of syntax. Cambridge, MA and London: MIT Press, 279–89.
Hagoort, P. and Poeppel, D. (2013) The infrastructure of the language-ready brain. In Arbib, M. (ed.), Language, music and the brain, Cambridge, MA: MIT Press, 239–55.
Hall, B.K. and Hallgrímsson, B. (2008) Evolution: The integration of genes, organisms and populations, . Sudbury, MA: Jones and Bartlett.
Harvey, P.H. and Pagel, M.D. (1995) The comparative method in evolutionary biology. Oxford University Press.
Hauser, M.D. (1996) The evolution of communication. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.
Hauser, M.D. (2009) Evolingo: The nature of the language faculty. In Piattelli-Palmarini, M., Uriagereka, J. and Salaburu, P. (eds.), Of minds and language: A dialogue with Noam Chomsky in the Basque Country. Oxford University Press, 74–84.
Hauser, M.D., Barner, D. and O'Donnell, T. (2007) Evolutionary linguistics: A new look at an old landscape. Language Learning and Development 3: 101–32.Google Scholar
Hauser, M., Chomsky, N. and Fitch, W.T. (2002) The faculty of language: What is it, who has it, and how did it evolve?Science 298: 1569–79.Google Scholar
Hauser, M.D., Yang, C., Berwick, R.C., Tattersall, I., Ryan, M.J., Watumull, J., Chomsky, N. and Lewontin, R.C. (2014) The mystery of language evolution. Frontiers in Psychology 5, Article 401, 1–12.Google Scholar
Healey, C. (1990) Maring hunters and traders: Production and exchange in the Papua New Guinea Highlands. Berkeley, CA: University of California Press.
Heine, B. and Kuteva, T. (2002) Personal deixis: Some rhetorical aspects of grammaticalization. University of Cologne. (Page references to typescript.)
Heine, B. and Kuteva, T. (2004) Grammaticalization theory: Preliminary observations on pidgins. Paper presented at the Seminar on Windows on Language Genesis, The Netherlands Institute for Advanced Study, Wassenaar, 7–8 November 2003.
Heine, B. and Kuteva, T. (2007) The genesis of grammar: A reconstruction. Oxford University Press.
Heine, B. and Kuteva, T. (2012) Grammaticalization theory as a tool for reconstructing language evolution. In Tallerman, M. and Gibson, K.R. (eds.), The Oxford handbook of language evolution. Oxford University Press, 512–27.
Henshilwood, C.S. (2007) Fully symbolic sapiens behaviour: Innovations in the Middle Stone Age at Blombos Cave, South Africa. In Mellars, P., Boyle, K., Bar-Yosef, O. and Stringer, C. (eds.), Rethinking the human revolution: New behavioural and biological perspectives on the origin and dispersal of modern humans, McDonald Institute Monographs. Cambridge: McDonald Institute for Archaeological Research, 123–32.
Henshilwood, C.S. and Dubreuil, B. (2009) Reading the artefacts: Gleaning language skills from the Middle Stone Age in southern Africa. In Botha, R. and Knight, C. (eds.), The cradle of language. Oxford University Press, 41–61.
Henshilwood, C.S. and Dubreuil, B. (2011) The Still Bay and Howiesons Poort, 77–59 ka: Symbolic material culture and the evolution of the mind during the Middle Stone Age. Current Anthropology 52: 361–80, 389–400.Google Scholar
Henshilwood, C.S. and Marean, C.W. (2003) The origin of modern behaviour: Critique of models and their test implication. Current Anthropology 44: 627–51.Google Scholar
Henshilwood, C.S. and Sealy, J. (1997) Bone artefacts from the Middle Stone Age at Blombos Cave, Southern Cape. Current Anthropology 38: 890–5.Google Scholar
Henshilwood, C.S., d'Errico, F., Marean, C.W., Milo, R.G. and Yates, R. (2001) An early bone tool industry from the Middle Stone Age at Blombos Cave, South Africa: Implications for the origins of modern behaviour, symbolism and language. Journal of Human Evolution 41: 631–78.Google Scholar
Henshilwood, C., d'Errico, F., Vanhaeren, M., van Niekerk, K. and Jacobs, Z. (2004) Middle Stone Age shell beads from South Africa. Science 304: 404.Google Scholar
Henshilwood, C.S., d'Errico, F., Yates, R., Jacobs, R., Tribolo, C., Duller, C., Sealy, J.C., Valladas, H., Watts, J. and Wintle, A.G. (2002) Emergence of modern human behaviour: Middle Stone Age engravings from South Africa. Science 295: 1278–80.Google Scholar
Hesse, M. (1963) Models and analogies in science. London and New York: Sheed and Ward.
Hewlett, B.S., Fouts, H.N., Boyette, A.H. and Hewlett, B.L. (2011) Social learning among Congo Basin hunter-gatherers. Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society B. Biological Sciences 366: 1168–78.Google Scholar
Hilpinen, R. (1968) Rules of acceptance in inductive logic. Acta Philosophica Fennica, Fasc. XXII. Amsterdam: North Holland.
Hirst, K.K. (s.a.) What is a hominin? (References to the text available at http:archaeology.about.com/od/hterms/ hominin.htm.)
Hockett, C.F. (1958) A course in modern linguistics. New York: MacMillan.
Hockett, C.F. (1959) Animal ‘languages’ and human language. In Spuhler, J.N. (ed.), The evolution of man's capacity for culture. Detroit: Wayne State University Press, 32–9.
Hockett, C.F. (1960a) Logical considerations in the study of animal communication. In Lanyon, W.E. and Tavolga, W.N. (eds.), Animal sounds and communication. Washington: American Institute of Biological Sciences, 392–430.
Hockett, C.F. (1960b) The origins of speech. Scientific American 203: 88–111.Google Scholar
Hockett, C.F. (1963) The problem of universals in language. In Greenberg, J.H. (ed.). Universals of language. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press, 1–29.
Hockett, C.F. (1977) The view from language: Selected essays 1948–74. Athens, GA: University of Georgia Press.
Hockett, C.F. and Altmann, S.A. (1968) A note on design features. In Sebeok, T.A. (ed.), Animal communication: Techniques of study and results of research. Bloomington and London: Indiana University Press, 61–72.
Holden, C. (2004) Oldest beads suggest early symbolic behaviour. Science 304: 369.Google Scholar
Holloway, R. (1978) Problems of brain endocast interpretation and African hominid evolution. In Jolly, C. (ed.), Early hominids in Africa. London: Duckworth, 379–401.
Holloway, R. (1979) Brain size, allometry and reorganization: A synthesis. In Hahn, M.E., Dudek, B.C. and Jensen, C. (eds.), Development and evolution of brain size. New York: Academic Press, 59–88.
Holloway, R. (1981) Revisiting the South African Taung australopithecine endocast: The position of the lunate sulcus as determined by stereoplotting technique. American Journal of Physical Anthropology 56: 43–58.Google Scholar
Holloway, R. (1983) Human paleontological evidence relevant to language behavior. Human Neurobiology 2: 105–14.Google Scholar
Holloway, R. (1984) The Taung endocast and the lunate sulcus: A rejection of the hypothesis of its anterior position. American Journal of Physical Anthropology 64: 285–7.Google Scholar
Holloway, R. (1995) The evidence for POT expansion in early Homo: A pretty theory with ugly (or no) paleoneurological facts. Behavioral and Brain Sciences 18: 191–3.Google Scholar
Holloway, R. (2008) The human brain evolving: A personal retrospective. Annual Review of Anthropology 37: 1–19.Google Scholar
Holloway, R.L., Broadfield, D.C. and Yuan, M.S. (2004) Brain endocasts: The paleoneurological evidence. Volume 3, The human fossil record. Ed. Schwartz, J.H. and Tattersall, I.. New York: Wiley-Liss.
Holm, J.A. (2000) An introduction to pidgins and creoles. Cambridge University Press.
Hooykaas, R. (1956) The principle of uniformity in geology, biology, and theology. The Journal of the Transactions of the Victoria Institute 88: 101–16.Google Scholar
Hooykaas, R. (1963) The principle of uniformity. Leiden: Brill.
Hoppitt, W.J.E., Brown, G.R., Kendal, R., Rendell, L., Thornton, A., Webster, M.M. and Laland, K.N. (2008) Lessons from animal teaching. Trends in Ecology and Evolution 23: 486–93.Google Scholar
Hoßfeld, U. and Olsson, L. (2003) The road from Haeckel: The Jena Tradition in evolutionary morphology and the origins of ‘evo-devo’. Biology and Philosophy 18: 285–307.Google Scholar
Hovers, E., Ilani, S., Bar-Yosef, O. and Vandermeersch, B. (2003). An early case of color symbolism: Ochre use by modern humans Qafzeh Cave. Current Anthropology 44: 491–522.Google Scholar
Howell, N. (1976) Towards a uniformitarian theory of human paleodemography. Journal of Human Evolution 5: 25–40.Google Scholar
Huijbregts, R. (to appear) Constructing an external language or growing an internal language? (Page references to typescript.)
Hurford, J.R. (2012) The origins of grammar: Language in the light of evolution II. Oxford University Press.
Jackendoff, R. (1983) Semantics and cognition. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.
Jackendoff, R. (1999) Possible stages in the evolution of the language faculty. Trends in Cognitive Science 3(7): 272–9.Google Scholar
Jackendoff, R. (2002) Foundations of language, brain, meaning, grammar, evolution. Oxford University Press.
Jackendoff, R. (2007) Linguistics in cognitive science: The state of the art. The Linguistic Review 24: 347–402.Google Scholar
Jackendoff, R. (2009) Parallels and non-parallels between language and music. Music Perception 26: 195–204.Google Scholar
Jackendoff, R. and Wittenberg, E. (2014) What you can say without syntax: A hierarchy of grammatical complexity. In Newmeyer, F.J. and Preston, L.B. (eds.), Measuring grammatical complexity. Oxford University Press. (Page references to typescript.)
Jacobs, B. and Horner, M.J. (1995) Language as a multimodal sensory enhancement system. Behavioral and Brain Sciences 18: 194–5.Google Scholar
Jerison, H.J. (1995) Issues in neo- and paleoneurology of language. Behavioral and Brain Sciences 18: 195–6.Google Scholar
Jerison, H.J. (2001) Paleoneurology and the biology of music. In Wallin, N.L., Merker, B. and Brown, S. (eds.), The origins of music. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press, 177–96.
Jespersen, O. (1922) Language: its nature, development and origin. London: Allen & Unwin.
Johansson, S. (2013a) The talking Neanderthals: What do fossils, genetics, and archaeology say?Biolinguistics 7: 35–74.Google Scholar
Johansson, S. (2013b) Neanderthals between man and beast: A comment on the comments by Barcelό-Coblijn & Benítez-Burraco (2013)Biolinguistics 7: 217–27.Google Scholar
Kalmar, I. (1985) Are there really no primitive languages? In Olson, D.R., Torrance, N. and Hildyard, A. (eds.), Literacy, language, and learning: The nature and consequences of reading and writing. Cambridge University Press, 148–66.
Kamei, N. (2005) Play among Baka children in Cameroon. In Hewlett, B.S. and Lamb, M.E. (eds.), Hunter-gatherer childhoods: Evolutionary, developmental, and cultural perspectives. New Brunswick, N.J: Alddine Transaction, 343–59.
Kano, T. (1992) The last ape: Pigmy chimpanzee behaviour and ecology. University of Stanford Press.
Karmiloff, K. and Karmiloff-Smith, A. (2001) Pathways to language: From fetus to adolescent. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press.
Kastner, I., Meir, I., Sandler, W. and Dachkovsky, S. (2014) The emergence of embedded structure: Insights from Kafr Qasem Sign Language. Frontiers in Psychology 5, Article 525, 1–15.Google Scholar
Kegl, J. and McWhorter, J. (1997) Perspectives on emerging language. In Clark, E.V. (ed.), Proceedings of the twenty-eighth annual child language research forum. Published for the Stanford Linguistics Association by the Center for the Study of Language and Information, 15–38.
Kegl, J., Senghas, A. and Coppola, M. (1999) Creation through contact: Sign language emergence and sign language change in Nicaragua. In DeGraff, M. (ed.), Language creation and language change. Creolization, diachrony, and development. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press, 179–237.
Kendon, A. (1991) Some considerations for a theory of language origins. Man (N.S.) 26: 199–226.Google Scholar
Kimura, M. (1993) Neuromotor mechanisms in human communication. Oxford University Press.
King, B. (2005) The singing Neanderthal. Review of S. Mithen, The singing Neanderthals: The origins of music, language, mind and body. London: Weidenfeld & Nicholson, 2005. Bookslut, October.
Kirby, S. (2000) Syntax without natural selection: How compositionality emerges from vocabulary in a population of learners. In Knight, C., Studdert-Kennedy, M. and Hurford, J.R. (eds.), The evolutionary emergence of language: Social function and the origins of linguistic form. Cambridge University Press, 303–23.
Kirby, S. (2012) Darwin's musical protolanguage. In Rebuschat, P., Rohrmeier, M. and Cross, I. (eds.), Language and music as cognitive systems. Oxford University Press, 96–102.
Kitahara-Frisch, J. (1979) The evolution of intelligence: Making assumptions explicit and hypotheses testable. Behavioral and Brain Sciences 2: 390–1.Google Scholar
Klein, R.G. (2000) Archeology and the evolution of human behaviour. Evolutionary Anthropology 9: 17–36.Google Scholar
Klein, R.G. and Elgar, B. (2002) The dawn of human culture. New York, NJ: John Wiley.
Klein, W. and Perdue, C. (1992) Utterance structure: Developing grammars. Amsterdam: John Bejamins.
Klein, W. and Perdue, C. (1997) The basic variety (or Couldn't language be much simpler?)Second Language Research 13: 301–47.Google Scholar
Kluender, K.R., Diehl, R.L. and Killeen, P.R. (1987) Japanese quail can learn phonetic categories. Science 237: 1195–7.Google Scholar
Knight, J. and Harrison, S. (2014) Limitations of uniformitarianism in the Anthropocene. Anthropocene 5: 71–5.Google Scholar
Kuhl, P.K. and Miller, J.D. (1975) Speech perception in the chinchilla: Voiced-voiceless distinction in alveolar plosive consonants. Science 190: 69–72.Google Scholar
Kuhl, P.K. and Padden, D.M. (1982) Enhanced discriminability at the phonetic boundaries for the voicing feature in macaques. Perception & Psychophysics 32: 542–50.Google Scholar
Laland, K.N. and Hoppitt, W. (2003) Do animals have culture?Evolutionary Anthropology 12: 150–9.Google Scholar
Lamendella, J.T. (1976) Relations between the ontogeny and phylogeny of language: A neorecapitulationist view. Annals of the New York Academy of Sciences 280: 396–412.Google Scholar
Lamendella, J.T. (1979) Assumptions about hominid ‘intelligence’ and ‘language’. Behavioral and Brain Sciences 2: 391–2.Google Scholar
Lancy, D.F. (2008) The anthropology of culture: Cherubs, chattels, changelings. Cambridge University Press.
Lancy, D.F. and Grove, M.A. (2010) The role of adults in children's learning. In Lancy, D.F., Bock, J. and Gaskins, S. (eds.), The anthropology of learning in childhood. Plymouth, NH: AltaMira, 145–79.
Lee, R.B. (1979) The !Kung San: Men, women, and work in a foraging society. Cambridge University Press.
Legate, J.A. and Pesetsky, D. (2014) Recursive misrepresentations: A reply to Levinson (2013). Language 90: 515–28.Google Scholar
Lemasson, A., Ouattara, K. and Zuberbühler, K. (2013) Exploring the gaps between primate calls and human language. In Botha, R. and Everaert, M. (eds.), The evolutionary emergence of language: evidence and inference. Oxford University Press, 181–203.
Levinson, S.C. (2013) Recursion in pragmatics. Language 89: 149–62.Google Scholar
Levman, B.G. (1992) The genesis of music and language. Ethnomusicology 36: 147–70.Google Scholar
Li, C.N. (2002) Missing links, issues and hypotheses in the evolutionary origin of language. In Givόn, T. and Malle, B.F. (eds.), The evolution of language out of pre-language. Amsterdam: John Benjamins Publishing Company, 83–106.
Lidz, J. and Williams, A. (2009) Constructions on holiday. Cognitive Linguistics 20: 177–89.Google Scholar
Lieberman, P. (1984) The biology and evolution of language. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press.
Lieberman, P. (1995) Manual versus speech motor control and the evolution of language. Behavioral and Brain Sciences 18: 197–8.Google Scholar
Lieberman, P. (2007) The evolution of human speech: Its anatomical and neural bases. Current Anthropology 48: 39–66.Google Scholar
Livingstone, F.B. (1973) Did the Australopithecines sing?Current Anthropology 14: 25–9.Google Scholar
Lobina, D.J. (2012) All tied in knots. Biolinguistics 6: 70–8.Google Scholar
Lobina, D.J. and Brenchley, M. (2012) Knots, language, and computation. Biolinguistics 6: 176–204.Google Scholar
Lock, A. (1983) ‘Recapitulation’ in the ontogeny and phylogeny of language. In de Grolier, E. (ed.), Glossogenetics: The origin and evolution of language. Paris: Harwood Academic Publishers, 255–73.
Locke, J.L. (2009) Evolutionary developmental linguistics: Naturalization of the faculty of language. Language Sciences 31: 33–59.Google Scholar
Locke, J.L. and Bogin, B. (2006) Language and life history: A new perspective on the development and evolution of human language. Behavioral and Brain Sciences 29: 259–325.Google Scholar
London, J. (2012) Schemas, not syntax: A reply to Patel. In Rebuschat, P., Rohrmeier, M. and Cross, I. (eds.), Language and music as cognitive systems. Oxford University Press, 243–7.
Longhi, E. and Karmiloff-Smith, A. (2004) In the beginning was the song: The complex multimodal timing of mother-infant musical interaction. Behavioral and Brain Sciences 27: 516–17.Google Scholar
Lyell, C. (1830) Principles of geology: Being an attempt to explain the former changes of the earth's surface by reference to causes now in operation. London: John Murray.
MacDonald, K. (2007) Cross-cultural comparison of learning in human hunting: Implications for life history evolution. Human Nature 18: 386–402.Google Scholar
MacDonald, K. and Roebroeks, W. (2013) Neanderthal linguistic abilities: an alternative view. In Botha, R. and Everaert, M. (eds.), The evolutionary emergence of language: Evidence and inference. Oxford University Press, 97–117.
MacNeilage, P.F. (2008) The origins of speech. Oxford University Press.
MacNeilage, P.F. (2012) The evolution of phonology. In Tallerman, M. and Gibson, K.R. (eds.), The Oxford handbook of language evolution, Oxford University Press, 423–34.
MacNeilage, P.F. and Davis, B.L. (2000) On the origin of the internal structure of word forms. Science 288: 527–31.Google Scholar
MacWhinney, B. (2002) The gradual emergence of language. In Givόn, T. and Malle, B.F. (eds.), The evolution of language out of pre-language. Amsterdam: John Benjamins, 233–63.
Malafouris, L. (2007) Before and beyond representation: Towards an enactive conception of the Palaeolithic image. In Renfrew, C. and Morley, I. (eds.), Image and imagination: A global history of figurative representation. Cambridge: McDonald Institute for Archaeological Research, 289–302.
Malafouris, L. (2008) Beads for a plastic mind: The ‘blind man's stick’ (BMS) hypothesis and the active nature of material culture. Cambridge Archaeological Journal 18: 401–14.Google Scholar
Malafouris, L. (2010) Grasping the concept of number: How did the sapient mind move beyond approximation? In Morley, I. and Renfrew, C. (eds.), The archaeology of measurement: Comprehending heaven, earth and time in ancient societies. Cambridge University Press, 35–42. (Referred to as ‘Malafouris (2010b)’ by Malafouris in Chapter 3.)
Malafouris, L. (2011) Commentary on Henshilwood and Dubreuil (2011). Current Anthropology 52: 385–6.Google Scholar
Marler, P. (1970) Birdsong and speech development: Could there be parallels?American Scientist 58: 669–73.Google Scholar
Marshall, L. (1976) The !Kung of Nyae Nyae. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press.
Martins, M.D. (2012) Distinctive signatures of recursion. Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society B 367: 2055–64.Google Scholar
Matsaka, N. (1998) Perception of motherese in Japanese Sign Language by 6-month-old hearing infants. Developmental Psychology 34: 241–6.Google Scholar
Matsaka, N. (2007) Music, evolution and language. Developmental Studies 10: 35–9.Google Scholar
Mayr, E. (1982) The growth of biological thought: Diversity, evolution, and inheritance. Cambridge, MA: Belknap Press of Harvard University Press.
Mayr, E. (1994) Recapitulation reinterpreted: The somatic program. The Quarterly Review of Biology 69: 223–32.Google Scholar
McBrearty, S. and Brooks, A.S. (2000) The revolution that wasn't: A new interpretation of the origin of modern human behaviour. Journal of Human Evolution 39: 453–563.Google Scholar
Meir, I., Sandler, W., Padden, C. and Aronoff, M. (2012) Emerging sign languages. In Nathan, P., Marschark, M. and Spencer, P.E. (eds.), The Oxford handbook of deaf studies, Language and education, Volume 2. Oxford University Press. (Page references to the text available at: http://sandlersignlab.haifa.ac.il/html/html_eng/pdf/ EMERGING_SIGN_LANGUAGES.pdf.)
Mellars, P. (1998a) Neanderthals, modern humans and the archaeological evidence for language. In Jablonski, N.G. and Aiello, L.C. (eds.), The origin and diversification of language. Memoirs of the California Academy of Science, No. 24, San Francisco, CA, 98–115.
Mellars, P. (1998b) Modern humans, language and the ‘symbolic explosion’. Cambridge Archaeological Journal 8: 88–90.Google Scholar
Mellars, P. (2006) Why did modern human populations disperse from Africa c. 60,000 years ago?Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America 103: 9381–6.Google Scholar
Merker, B. (2001) Synchronous chorusing and human origins. In Wallin, N.L., Merker, B. and Brown, S. (eds.), The origins of music. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press, 315–27.
Messinger, D.S. and Fogel, A. (1998) Give and take: The development of conventional infant gestures. Merrill-Palmer Quarterly 44: 566–90.Google Scholar
Mithen, S. (1999) Symbolism and the supernatural. In Dunbar, R.I.M., Knight, C. and Power, C. (eds.), The evolution of culture: An interdisciplinary view. Edinburgh University Press, 147–69.
Mithen, S. (2005) The singing Neanderthals. The origins of music, language, mind and body. London: Weidenfeld & Nicholson.
Mithen, S. (2006) Overview and reply. Review feature of S. Mithen, The singing Neanderthals. The origins of music, language, mind and body, London: Weidenfeld & Nicholson, 2005. Cambridge Archaeological Journal 16: 98–100, 109–12.Google Scholar
Mithen, S. (2012a) Musicality and language. In Tallerman, M. and Gibson, K.R. (eds.), The Oxford handbook of language evolution. Oxford University Press, 296–8.
Mithen, S. (2012b) The significance of stones and bones: Understanding the biology and evolution of rhythm requires attention to the archaeological and fossil record. In Rebuschat, P., Rohrmeier, M. and Cross, I. (eds.), Language and music as cognitive systems. Oxford University Press, 103–9.
Mithun, M. (1984) How to avoid subordination. Berkeley Linguistic Society 10: 493–509.Google Scholar
Monnot, M. (1998) Mother-infant communication and infant health: In Monnot, M. (ed.), Biological Anthropology. Cambridge University Press, 216.
Monnot, M. (1999) Function of infant-directed speech. Human Nature: An Interdisciplinary Biosocial Perspective 10: 415–43.Google Scholar
Moore, J. (1996) Savanna chimpanzees: Referential models and the last common ancestor. In McGrew, W.C., Marchant, L.F. and Nishida, T. (eds.), Great ape societies. Cambridge University Press, 275–92.
Moorman, S. and Bolhuis, J.J. (2013) Behavioral similarities between birdsong and spoken language. In Bolhuis, J.J. and Everaert, M. (eds.), Birdsong, speech, and language: Exploring the evolution of mind and brain. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press, 112–23.
Morford, J.P. (1996) Insights to language from the study of gesture: A review of the research on the gestural communication of non-signing deaf people. Language & Communication 16: 165–78.Google Scholar
Morford, J.P. (2002) Why does exposure to language matter? In Givόn, T. and Malle, B.F. (eds.), The evolution of language out of pre-language. Amsterdam: John Benjamins, 329–41.
Morley, I. (2006) Name that tune. Cambridge Archaeological Journal 16: 101–3.Google Scholar
Mufwene, S.S. (2007) What do creoles and pidgins tell us about the evolution of language? In Laks, B., Cleuziou, S., Demoule, J-P. and Encrevé, P. (eds.), The origin and evolution of languages: Approaches, models, paradigms. London: Equinox. (Page references to the text available at: http://humanties.uchicago.edu/faculty/mufwene/publications.CREOLES-LGEVOLUTION-Revisions-1.pdf.)
Mufwene, S.S. (2008) Language evolution: Contact, competition and change. London: Continuum International.
Müller, G.B. (2007a) Six memos for evo-devo. In Laubichler, M.D. and Maienschein, J. (eds.), A history of developmental evolution. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press, 499–524.
Müller, G.B. (2007b) Evo-devo: Extending the evolutionary synthesis. Nature Reviews: Genetics 8: 943–9.Google Scholar
Müller, G.B. (2008) Evo-devo as a discipline. In Minelli, A. and Fusco, G. (eds.), Evolving pathways: Key themes in evolutionary developmental biology. Cambridge University Press, 5–30.
Nelson, G. (1978) Ontogeny, phylogeny, paleontology, and the biogenetic law. Systematic Zoology 27: 324–45.Google Scholar
Nelson, K. (1981) Individual differences in language development: Implications for development and language. Developmental Psychology 17: 170–87.Google Scholar
Nettle, D. (1999) Is the rate of linguistic change constant?Lingua 108: 119–36.Google Scholar
Nevins, A.D., Pesetsky, D. and Rodrigues, C. (2009a) Pirahã exceptionality: A reassessment. Language 85: 355–404.Google Scholar
Nevins, A.D., Pesetsky, D. and Rodrigues, C. (2009b) Evidence and argumentation: A reply to Everett (2009)Language 85: 671–81.Google Scholar
Newman, J.D. (2004) Motherese by any other name: Mother–infant communication in non-hominin mammals. Behavioral and Brain Sciences 27: 519–20.Google Scholar
Newmeyer, F. (1998) On the supposed ‘counterfactuality’ of universal grammar: Some evolutionary implications. In Hurford, J.R., Studdert-Kennedy, M. and Knight, C. (eds.), Approaches to the evolution of language. Cambridge University Press, 305–19.
Newmeyer, F. (2002) Uniformitarian assumptions and language evolution research. In Wray, A. (ed.), The transition to language. Oxford University Press, 359–75.
Newmeyer, F. (2003) What can the field of linguistics tell us about the origins of language? In Christiansen, M.H. and Kirby, S. (eds.), Language evolution. Oxford University Press, 58–76.
Newmeyer, F. (2004) Cognitive and functional factors in the evolution of grammar. (Page references to the text available at: www.interdisciplines.org/coevolution/papers/3.)
Newmeyer, F. (2006) What can grammaticalization tell us about the origins of language? Paper presented at the Sixth International Conference on the Evolution of Language, Rome, 12–15 April. (Page references to the typescript.)
Newport, E.L., Gleitman, H. and Gleitman, L.R. (1977) ‘Mother, I'D rather do it myself’: Some effects and non-effects of maternal speech style. In Snow, C. and Ferguson, C.A. (eds.), Talking to children: Language input and acquisition. Cambridge University Press, 109–49.
Nottebohm, F. (1999) The anatomy and timing of vocal learning in birds. In Hauser, M.D. and Konishi, M. (eds.), The design of animal communication. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press, 63–110.
Nowak, M.A., Plotkin, J.B. and Jansen, V.A.A. (2000) The evolution of syntactic communication. Nature 404: 495–8.Google Scholar
Ochs, E. and Schieffelin, B. (1984) Language acquisition and socialization: Three developmental stories. In Schweder, R. and LeVine, R. (eds.), Culture theory: Mind, self, and emotion. Cambridge University Press, 276–320.
O'Grady, W. (1997) Syntactic development. University of Chicago Press.
Ojemann, G.A. and Mateer, C. (1979) Human language cortex: Localization of memory, syntax, and sequential motor-phoneme identification systems. Science 205: 401–3.Google Scholar
Ojemann, G.A., Ojemann, F., Lettich, E. and Berger, M. (1989) Cortical language localization in left dominant hemisphere: An electrical stimulation mapping investigation in 117 patients. Journal of Neurosurgery 71: 316–26.Google Scholar
Paavola, S. (2005) Peircean abduction: Instinct, or inference?Semiotica 153: 131–54.Google Scholar
Padden, C., Meir, I., Sandler, W. and Aronoff, M. (2010) Against all expectations: Encoding subjects and objects in a new language. In Gerdts, D.B., Moore, J.C. and Polinsky, M. (eds.), Hypothesis A/Hypothesis B: Linguistic explorations in honor of David M. Perlmutter. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press, 383–400.
Pagel, M. (2002) Comparative method. In Pagel, M. (ed.), Encyclopedia of evolution, Volume 1. Oxford University Press, 183–90.
Parisi, D. (1983) A three-stage model of language evolution: From pantomime to syntax. In de Grolier, E. (ed.), Glossogenetics: The origin and evolution of language. Paris: Harwood Academic Publishers, 419–34.
Parker, A.R. (2005) Evolving the Narrow Language Faculty: Was recursion the pivotal step? Unpublished paper, Language Evolution and Computational research Unit, University of Edinburgh.
Parker, S.T. (1985) A social-technological model for the evolution of language. Current Anthropology 26: 617–39.Google Scholar
Parker, S.T. and Gibson, K.R. (1979) A model of the evolution of language and intelligence in early hominids. Behavioral and Brain Sciences 2: 367–407.Google Scholar
Patel, A. (2003) Language, music, syntax, and the brain. Nature Neuroscience 6: 674–81.Google Scholar
Patel, A. (2008) Music, language, and the brain. Oxford University Press.
Patel, A. (2012a) Language, music, and the brain: A resource sharing framework. In Rebuschat, P., Rohrmeier, M. and Cross, I. (eds.), Language and music as cognitive systems. Oxford University Press, 204–23.
Patel, A. (2012b) Advancing the comparative study of linguistic and musical syntactic processing. In Rebuschat, P., Rohrmeier, M. and Cross, I. (eds.) Language and music as cognitive systems. Oxford University Press, 248–53.
Patel, A. (2013). Sharing and nonsharing of brain resources for language and music. In Arbib, M.A. (ed.), Language, music, and the brain: A mysterious relationship. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press, 329–55.
Pearl, L. and Strouse, J. (2012) Syntactic islands and learning biases: Combining experimental syntax and computational modeling. Cognitive Science, University of California at Irvine. Typescript.
Peirce, C.S. (1931–5) Collected Papers. (CP) Volumes I–VI re-issued by Hartshorne, C. and Weiss, P. (eds.), Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press.
Peirce, C.S (1958) Collected Papers. Volumes VII and VIII re-issued by Burks, W. (ed.), Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press.
Pesetsky, D. (1985) Morphology and logical form. Linguistic Inquiry 16: 193–246.Google Scholar
Petraglia, M., Shipton, C. and Paddayya, K. (2005) Life and mind in the Acheulan: A case study from India. In Gamble, C. and Porr, M. (eds.), The hominid individual in context: Archaeological investigations of lower and middle Palaeolithic landscapes. Locales, and artefacts. London: Routledge, 197–219.
Phillips, C. (2006) The real-time status of island phenomena. Language 82: 795–823.Google Scholar
Piaget, J. (1969) Genetic epistemology. Columbia Forum 12: 4–11.Google Scholar
Piaget, J. (1978) Behavior and evolution. New York: Pantheon.
Pinker, S. and Bloom, P. (1990) Natural language and natural selection. Behavioral and Brain Sciences 13: 707–84.Google Scholar
Pinker, S. and Jackendoff, R. (2005) The faculty of language: What's special about it?Cognition 95: 201–36.Google Scholar
Plutynski, A. (2011) Four problems of abduction: a brief history. HOPOS: The Journal of the International Society for the History of Philosophy of Science 1: 227–48.Google Scholar
Poeppel, D., Emmorey, K., Hickok, G. and Pylkkänen, L. (2012) Towards a new neurobiology of language. Journal of Neuroscience 32: 14125–31.Google Scholar
Psillos, S. (2011) An explorer upon untrodden ground: Peirce on abduction. In Gabbay, D.M., Hartmann, S. and Woods, J. (eds.), Handbook of the history of logic, Volume 10: Inductive Logic. Oxford: North Holland, 117–52.
Puri, R.K. (1997) Hunting knowledge of the Penan Benalui of East Kalimantan, Indonesia. Ph.D. thesis, University of Hawaii.
Ragir, S. (2002) Constraints on communities with indigenous sign languages: Clues to the dynamics of language genesis. In Wray, A. (ed.), The transition to language. Oxford University Press, 272–94.
Reichard, G.A. (1934) Spider woman: A story of Navajo weavers and chanters. Glorietta, NM: Rio Grande Press.
Rey, A., Perruche, P. and Fagot, J. (2012) Centre-embedded structures are a by-product of associative learning and working memory constraints: Evidence from baboons (Papio papio)Cognition 123: 180–4.Google Scholar
Richards, R.J. (2008) The tragic sense of life: Ernst Haeckel and the struggle over evolutionary thought. University of Chicago Press.
Richman, B. (1993) On the evolution of speech: Singing as the middle term. Current Anthropology 34: 721–2.Google Scholar
Richman, B. (2001) How music fixed ‘nonsense’ into significant formulas: On rhythm, repetition, and meaning. In Wallin, N.L., Merker, B. and Brown, S. (eds.), The origins of music. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press, 301–14.
Roberge, P.T. (2009) The creation of pidgins as a possible window on language evolution. In Botha, R. and Swart, H.de (eds.), Language evolution: The view from restricted linguistic systems., Utrecht: LOT Occasional Series, 101–37.
Roberge, P.T. (2012) Pidgins, creoles, and the creation of language. In Tallerman, M. and Gibson, K.R. (eds.), The Oxford handbook of language evolution. Oxford University Press, 537–44.
Roberts, J.M. (1995a) Pidgin Hawaiin: A sociohistorical study. Journal of Pidgin and Creole Languages 10: 1–56.Google Scholar
Roberts, J.M. (1995b) A structural sketch of Pidgin Hawaiin. Amsterdam Creole Studies 12: 97–126.Google Scholar
Roberts, J.M. (to appear) The role of diffusion in creole genesis. (Previously published in Language 47(1), 1998: 1–39.)Google Scholar
Roebroeks, W. and Verpoorte, A. (2009) A ‘language-free’ explanation for differences between the European Middle and Upper Paleolithic record. In Botha, R. and Knight, C. (eds.), The cradle of language. Oxford University Press.
Roy, A.C., Curie, A., Nazir, T., Paulignan, Y., Des Portes, V., Fourneret, P. and Deprez, V. (2013), Syntax at hand: Common syntactic structures for actions and language. PLOS ONE e72677: 1–11.Google Scholar
Rudwick, M.J.S. (1972) The meaning of fossils. London: Macdonald.
Salmon, W.C. (1967) The foundations of scientific inference. Pittsburgh: University of Pittsburgh Press.
Sandler, W., Meir, I., Padden, C. and Aronoff, M. (2005) The emergence of grammar: Systematic structure in a new language. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences 102 (7): 2661–5.Google Scholar
Sankoff, G. (1979) The genesis of language. In Hill, K.C. (ed.), The genesis of language. Ann Arbor: Karoma, 23–47.
Scharff, C. and Petri, J. (2011) Evo-devo, deep homology and FoxP2: Implications for the evolution of speech and language. Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society B 366: 2124–40.Google Scholar
Schiffer, M.B. (1978) Methodological issues in ethnoarchaeology. In Gould, R.A. (ed.), Explorations in ethnoarchaeology. Albuquerque: University of New Mexico Press, 229–47.
Schoenemann, P.T. (2006) Evolution of the size and functional areas of the human brain. Annual Review of Anthropology 35: 379–406.Google Scholar
Schurz, G. (2008) Patterns of abduction. Synthese 164: 201–34.Google Scholar
Seeley, T.D. and Sherman, P.W. (2014) Animal behaviour. Encyclopaedia Brittanica Online. Retrieved 17 June 2014 from www.brittanica.com/EBchecked/topic/25597/animal-behaviour.
Selkirk, E.O. (1984) Phonology and syntax: The relation between sound and structure. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.
Senghas, A. (1995) Children's contribution to the birth of Nicaraguan Sign Language. Ph.D. thesis, MIT.
Senghas, A. (2000) The development of early spatial morphology in Nicaraguan Sign Language. In Holwell, S.C., Fish, S.A. and Keith-Lucas, T. (eds.), Proceedings of the Boston Conference on Language Development, 19. Boston: Cascadilla Press, 696–707.
Senghas, A. (2005) Language emergence: Clues from a new Bedouin Sign Language. Current Biology 15: 463–5.Google Scholar
Senghas, A. and Coppola, M. (2001) Children creating language: How Nicaraguan Sign Language acquired a spatial grammar. Psychological Science 12: 323–8.Google Scholar
Senghas, A., Kita, S. and Ӧzyürek, A. (2004) Children creating core properties of language: Evidence from an emerging sign language in Nicaragua. Science 305: 1779–82.Google Scholar
Senghas, A., Ӧzyürek, A. and Goldin-Meadow, S. (2013) Homesign as a way-station between co-speech gesture and sign language: The evolution of segmentation and sequencing. In Botha, R. and Everaert, M. (eds.), The evolutionary emergence of language: evidence and inference. Oxford University Press, 62–76.
Senghas, A.Coppola, M., Newport, M. and Suppala, T. (1997) Argument structure in Nicaraguan Sign Language: The emergence of grammatical devices. In Hughes, E., Hughes, A. and Greenhill, A. (eds.), Proceedings of the Boston University Conference on Language Development, 21. Boston: Cascadilla Press, 550–61.
Shea, J.H. (1982) Twelve fallacies of uniformitarianism. Geology 10: 455–60.Google Scholar
Shipley, E.F., Smith, C.S. and Gleitman, L.R. (1969) A study in the acquisition of language: Free responses to commands. Language 45: 322–42.Google Scholar
Shubin, T., Tabin, C. and Carroll, S. (1997) Fossils, genes and the evolution of the human body. Nature 388: 639–48.Google Scholar
Simpson, G.G. (1970) Uniformitarianism: An inquiry into principle, theory, and method in geohistory and biohistory. In Hecht, M.K. and Steere, W.C. (eds.), Essays in evolution and genetics in honor of Theodosius Dobhanzky: A supplement to evolutionary biology. New York: Appleton-Century-Crofts, 43–96.
Slobin, D. (2002) Language evolution, acquisition and diachrony. In Givόn, T. and Malle, B.F. (eds.), The evolution of language out of pre-language. Amsterdam: John Benjamins, 375–92.
Slobin, D. (2004) From ontogenesis to phylogenesis: What can child language tell us about language evolution? In Langer, J., Parker, S.T. and Milbrath, G. (eds.), Biology and knowledge revisited: From neurogenesis to psychogenesis. Mahwah, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates. (Page references to the text available at: http://ihd.berkeley.edu/Slobin-Language%20Acquisition/(2004)%20Slobin%20.)
Slocombe, K. (2012) Have we underestimated great ape vocal capacities? In Tallerman, M. and Gibson, K.R. (eds.), The Oxford handbook of language evolution. Oxford University Press, 90–5.
Smith, K. (2006) The protolanguage debate: Bridging the gap? Paper presented at EVOLANG 6, Rome, April. (Page references to the text available at: www.tech.plymouth.ac.uk/socce/evolang6/smithk.pdf.)
Smith, K. (2008) Is a holistic protolanguage a plausible precursor to language? A test case for a modern evolutionary linguistics. Interaction Studies 1: 1–17.Google Scholar
Snowdon, C.T. and French, J.A. (1979) Ontogeny does not always recapitulate phylogeny. Behavioral and Brain Sciences 2: 397–8.Google Scholar
Spiezio, C. and Lunardelli, A. (2004) Is it always really mother's fault?Behavioral and Brain Sciences 27: 523–4.Google Scholar
Stam, J.H. (1976) Inquiries into the origin of language: The fate of a question. New York: Harper and Row Publishers.
Steele, J., Ferrari, P.F. and Fogassi, L. (2012) From action to language: Comparative perspectives on primate tool use, gesture and the evolution of human language. Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society B 367: 4–9.Google Scholar
Stiles, D. (2001) Hunter-gatherer studies: The importance of context. African Studies Monographs, Supplement 26: 41–65.Google Scholar
Studdert-Kennedy, M. (1990) Language development from an evolutionary perspective. Haskins Laboratories Status Report on Speech Research 1990, SR-101, 14–27.
Studdert-Kennedy, M. (2012) The emergence of phonetic form. In Tallerman, M. and Gibson, K.R. (eds.), The Oxford handbook of language evolution. Oxford University Press, 417–22.
Studdert-Kennedy, M. and Goldstein, L. (2003) Launching language: The gestural origin of discrete infinity. In Christiansen, M.H. and Kirby, S. (eds.), Language evolution. Oxford University Press, 235–54.
Swisher, K. and Deyhle, D. (1989) The styles of learning are different, but the teaching is the same: Suggestions for teachers of American Indian youth. Journal of American Indian Education 29: 1–14.Google Scholar
Számadó, S., Hurford, J., Bishop, D., Deacon, T., d'Errico, F., Fischer, F., Okanoya, K., Szathmáry, E. and White, S. (2009) What are the possible biological and genetic foundations for syntactic phenomena? In Bickerton, D. and Szathmáry, E. (eds.), Biological foundations and origin of syntax. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press, 207–36.
Tallerman, M. (2006) Abracadabra! Early Hominin for ‘I think my humming's out of tune with the rest of the world!’Cambridge Archaeological Journal 16: 106–7.Google Scholar
Tallerman, M. (2007) Did our ancestors speak a holistic protolanguage?Lingua 117: 579–604.Google Scholar
Tallerman, M. (2008) Holophrastic protolanguage: Planning, processing, storage, and retrieval. Interaction Studies 9: 84–99.Google Scholar
Tallerman, M. (2010). Holophrastic protolanguage: Planning, processing, storage and retrieval. In Bickerton, D. and Arbib, M. (eds.), The emergence of protolanguage. Amsterdam: John Benjamins, 83–99.
Tallerman, M. (2012a) What is syntax? In Tallerman, M. and Gibson, K.R. (eds.), The Oxford handbook of language evolution. Oxford University Press, 442–55.
Tallerman, M. (2012b) Protolanguage. In Tallerman, M. and Gibson, K.R. (eds.), The Oxford handbook of language evolution. Oxford University Press, 479–91.
Tallerman, M. (2013a) Kin selection, pedagogy, and linguistic complexity: Whence protolanguage? In Botha, R. and Everaert, M. (eds.), The evolutionary emergence of language: evidence and inference. Oxford University Press, 77–96.
Tallerman, M. (2013b) Join the dots: A musical interlude in the evolution of language. A review of T. Fitch, The evolution of language, Cambridge University Press, 2010. Journal of Linguistics 49: 455–87.Google Scholar
Tallerman, M. and Gibson, K.R. (2012) Introduction: The evolution of language. In Tallerman, M. and Gibson, K.R. (eds.), The Oxford handbook of language evolution. Oxford University Press, 1–35.
Tattersall, I. (1998a) Becoming human. Evolution and human uniqueness. New York: Harcourt Brace.
Tattersall, I. (1998b) The origin of the human capacity. James Arthur Lecture Series 86. New York: American Museum of Natural History.
Tattersall, I. (2014) Communication and human uniqueness. In Pina, M. and Gontier, N. (eds.), Evolution of social communication in primates: A multidisciplinary approach. Heidelberg: Springer, 219–28.
Tehrani, J.J. and Riede, F. (2008) Towards an archaeology of pedagogy: Learning, teaching and the generation of material culture traditions. World Archaeology 40: 316–31.Google Scholar
Thomason, S.G. (2001) Language contact. Edinburgh University Press.
Tobias, P.V. (1987) The brain of Homo habilis: A new level of organization in cerebral evolution. Journal of Human Evolution 16: 741–61.Google Scholar
Tobias, P.V. (1996) The dating of linguistic beginnings. Behavioral and Brain Sciences 9: 789–91.Google Scholar
Tolbert, E. (2012) An ethnomusicological perspective on animal ‘music’ and human music: The paradox of ‘the paradox of rhythm’. In Rebuschat, P., Rohrmeier, M. and Cross, I. (eds.), Language and music as cognitive systems. Oxford University Press, 121–7.
Tomasello, M. (2003) Constructing a language: A usage-based theory of language acquisition. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press.
Tomasello, M. (2004) What kind of evidence could refute the UG hypothesis?Studies in Language 28: 642–5.Google Scholar
Tomasello, M. (2005) Beyond formalities: The case of language acquisition. The Linguistic Review 22: 183–97.Google Scholar
Tomasello, M. (2006) Acquiring linguistic constructions. In Kuhn, D. and Siegler, R. (eds.), Handbook of child psychology, Volume 2. New York: Wiley, 255–98.
Trudgill, P. (1992). Dialect typology and social structure. In Jahr, E.H. (ed.), Language contact. New York: Mouton de Gruyter, 195–211.
Uttal, W.R. (2001) The new phrenology: The limits of localizing cognitive processes in the brain. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.
van Beek, A.G. (1978) The way of all flesh: Hunting and ideology of the Bedamuni of the Great Papuan Plateau (Papua New Guinea). Ph.D. diss., University of Leiden.
van Heijningen, C.A.A., de Visser, J., Zuidema, W. and ten Cate, C. (2009) Simple rules can explain discrimination of putative recursive syntactic structures by songbird species. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America 106: 20538–43.Google Scholar
van der Lely, H.K.J. and Pinker, S. (2014) The biological basis of language: Insights from developmental grammatical impairments. Trends in Cognitive Sciences 18: 586–95.Google Scholar
Vanhaeren, M., d'Errico, F., Stringer, C., James, S.L., Todd, J.A. and Mienis, H.K. (2006) Middle Paleolithic shell beads in Israel and Algeria. Science 312: 1785–8.Google Scholar
Vauclair, J. and Cochet, H. (2013) Speech-gesture links in the ontogeny and phylogeny of gestural communication. In Botha, R. and Everaert, M. (eds.), The evolutionary emergence of language: evidence and inference. Oxford University Press, 160–80.
Wadley, L. (2001) What is cultural modernity? A general view and a South African perspective from Rose Cottage Cave. Cambridge Archaeological Journal 11: 201–22.Google Scholar
Wadley, L. (2011) Commentary on Henshilwood and Dubreuil (2011). Current Anthropology 52: 388–9.Google Scholar
Wagner, G.P. (2002) Homology. In Pagel, M. (ed.), Encyclopedia of evolution, Volume 1. Oxford University Press, 497–501.
Welker, W. (1990) Why does cerebral cortex fissure and fold? A review of the determinants of gyri and sulci. In Jones, E.G. and Peters, A. (eds.), Comparative structure and evolution of cerebral cortex. New York: Plenum Press, 3–136.
Wexler, K. and Culicover, P. (1980) Formal principles of language acquisition. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.
Whitcombe, E. (1995) Paleoneurology of language: Grounds for scepticism. Behavioral and Brain Sciences 18: 204–5.Google Scholar
White, J.P., Modjeska, N. and Hipuya, I. (1977) Group definitions and mental templates. In Wright, R.V.S. (ed.), Stone tools as cultural markers. Atlantic Highlands, NJ: Humanities Press, 380–90.
Whiten, A. (2011) The scope of culture in chimpanzees, humans and ancestral apes. Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society B. Biological Sciences 366: 997–1007.Google Scholar
Whitney, W. (1873) Oriental and linguistic studies,Vol. 1. New York: Charles Scribner's Sons.
Wilkins, W.K. (2005) Anatomy matters. The Linguistic Review 22: 271–88.Google Scholar
Wilkins, W.K. (2007) Layers, mosaic pieces, and tiers. The Linguistic Review 24: 475–84.Google Scholar
Wilkins, W.K. (2009) Mosaic neurobiology and anatomical plausibility. In Botha, R. and Knight, C. (eds.), The prehistory of language. Oxford University Press, 266–85.
Wilkins, W.K. (2012) Towards an evolutionary biology of language through comparative neuroanatomy. In Tallerman, M. and Gibson, K.R. (eds.), The Oxford handbook of language evolution. Oxford University Press, 198–206.
Wilkins, W.K. and Wakefield, J.L. (1995a) Brain evolution and neurolinguistic preconditions. Behavioral and Brain Sciences 18: 161–82.Google Scholar
Wilkins, W.K. and Wakefield, J.L. (1995b) Issues and non-issues in the origin of language. Behavioral and Brain Sciences 18: 205–6.Google Scholar
Wilkins, W.K. and Wakefield, J.L. (1996) Further issues in neurolinguistic preconditions. Behavioral and Brain Sciences 19: 793–8.Google Scholar
Wilmsen, E.N. (1970) Lithic analysis and cultural inference: A Paleo-Indian case. Anthropological Papers of the University of Arizona, No. 16. Tucson: University of Arizona Press.
Winford, D. (1997) Introduction: On the structure and status of pidgins and creoles. In Spears, A.K. and Winford, D. (eds.), The structure and status of pidgins and creoles. Amsterdam: John Benjamins, 1–31.
Winford, D. (2003) An introduction to contact linguistics. Malden, Oxford: Blackwell.
Wray, A. (1998) Protolanguage as a holistic system for social interaction. Language & Communication 18: 47–67.Google Scholar
Wray, A. (2000) Holistic utterances in protolanguage: The link from primates to humans. In Knight, C., Studdert-Kennedy, M. and Hurford, J.R. (eds.), The evolutionary emergence of language: Social function and the origins of linguistic form. Cambridge University Press, 285–302.
Wray, A. (2002a) Dual processing in protolanguage: Performance without competence. In Wray, A. (ed.), The transition to language. Oxford University Press, 113–37.
Wray, A. (2002b) Holistic utterances in protolanguage: the link from primates to humans. In Knight, C., Studdert-Kennedy, M. and Hurford, J.R. (eds.), The evolutionary emergence of language: Social function and the origins of linguistic form. Cambridge University Press, 285–302.
Wray, A. (2002c) Formulaic language and the lexicon. Cambridge University Press.
Wray, A. (2006) Joining the dots: The evolutionary picture of language and music. Cambridge Archaeological Journal 16: 103–5.Google Scholar
Wurz, S. (2000) The Middle Stone Age at Klasies River, South Africa. Ph.D. thesis, University of Stellenbosch.
Wylie, A. (2002) Thinking from things: Essays in the philosophy of archaeology. Berkley: University of California Press.
Wynn, T. (1991) Tools, grammar and the archaeology of cognition. Cambridge Archaeological Journal 1: 191–206.Google Scholar
Wynn, T. and Coolidge, F.L. (2007) Did a small but significant enhancement in working memory capacity power the evolution of modern thinking? In Mellars, P., Boyle, K., Bar-Yosef, O. and Stringer, C. (eds.), Rethinking the human revolution: New behavioural and biological perspectives on the origin and dispersal of modern humans. McDonald Institute Monographs. Cambridge: McDonald Institute for Archaeological Research, 79–90.
Wynn, T. and Coolidge, F.L. (2012) How to think like a Neandertal. Oxford University Press.
Yang, C. (2013) Ontogeny and phylogeny of language. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America 110: 6324–7.Google Scholar
Zawidski, T. (2006) Sexual selection for syntax and kin selection for semantics: Problems and prospects. Biology and Philosophy 21: 453–70.Google Scholar
Zollikofer, C.P.E. and Ponce de León, M.S. (2013) Pandora's growing box: Inferring the evolution and development of hominin brains from endocasts. Evolutionary Anthropology 22: 20–33.Google Scholar
Zuberbühler, K. (2005) The phylogenetic roots of language: Evidence from primate communication. Current Directions in Psychological Science 14: 125–30.Google Scholar

Save book to Kindle

To save this book to your Kindle, first ensure coreplatform@cambridge.org is added to your Approved Personal Document E-mail List under your Personal Document Settings on the Manage Your Content and Devices page of your Amazon account. Then enter the ‘name’ part of your Kindle email address below. Find out more about saving to your Kindle.

Note you can select to save to either the @free.kindle.com or @kindle.com variations. ‘@free.kindle.com’ emails are free but can only be saved to your device when it is connected to wi-fi. ‘@kindle.com’ emails can be delivered even when you are not connected to wi-fi, but note that service fees apply.

Find out more about the Kindle Personal Document Service.

  • References
  • Rudolf Botha, University of Stellenbosch, South Africa
  • Book: Language Evolution
  • Online publication: 05 March 2016
  • Chapter DOI: https://doi.org/10.1017/CBO9781316471449.016
Available formats
×

Save book to Dropbox

To save content items to your account, please confirm that you agree to abide by our usage policies. If this is the first time you use this feature, you will be asked to authorise Cambridge Core to connect with your account. Find out more about saving content to Dropbox.

  • References
  • Rudolf Botha, University of Stellenbosch, South Africa
  • Book: Language Evolution
  • Online publication: 05 March 2016
  • Chapter DOI: https://doi.org/10.1017/CBO9781316471449.016
Available formats
×

Save book to Google Drive

To save content items to your account, please confirm that you agree to abide by our usage policies. If this is the first time you use this feature, you will be asked to authorise Cambridge Core to connect with your account. Find out more about saving content to Google Drive.

  • References
  • Rudolf Botha, University of Stellenbosch, South Africa
  • Book: Language Evolution
  • Online publication: 05 March 2016
  • Chapter DOI: https://doi.org/10.1017/CBO9781316471449.016
Available formats
×