Book contents
- The Cambridge Handbook of the Imagination
- The Cambridge Handbook of the Imagination
- Copyright page
- Dedication
- Contents
- Figures
- Contributors
- Acknowledgments
- 1 Surveying the Imagination Landscape
- Part I Theoretical Perspectives on the Imagination
- Part II Imagery-Based Forms of the Imagination
- Part III Intentionality-Based Forms of the Imagination
- Part IV Novel Combinatorial Forms of the Imagination
- Part V Phenomenology-Based Forms of the Imagination
- Part VI Altered States of the Imagination
- Name Index
- Subject Index
- References
Part II - Imagery-Based Forms of the Imagination
Published online by Cambridge University Press: 26 May 2020
Edited by
Book contents
- The Cambridge Handbook of the Imagination
- The Cambridge Handbook of the Imagination
- Copyright page
- Dedication
- Contents
- Figures
- Contributors
- Acknowledgments
- 1 Surveying the Imagination Landscape
- Part I Theoretical Perspectives on the Imagination
- Part II Imagery-Based Forms of the Imagination
- Part III Intentionality-Based Forms of the Imagination
- Part IV Novel Combinatorial Forms of the Imagination
- Part V Phenomenology-Based Forms of the Imagination
- Part VI Altered States of the Imagination
- Name Index
- Subject Index
- References
Summary
A summary is not available for this content so a preview has been provided. Please use the Get access link above for information on how to access this content.

- Type
- Chapter
- Information
- The Cambridge Handbook of the Imagination , pp. 173 - 290Publisher: Cambridge University PressPrint publication year: 2020
References
References
Albright, T. D. (2012). On the Perception of Probable Things: Neural Substrates of Associative Memory, Imagery, and Perception. Neuron, 74(2), 227–245.
Allport, G. W. (1924). Eidetic Imagery. British Journal of Psychology, 15, 99–120.
Bergmann, J., Genc, E., Kohler, A., Singer, W., and Pearson, J. (2016). Smaller Primary Visual Cortex Is Associated with Stronger, but Less Precise Mental Imagery. Cerebral Cortex, 26(9), 3838–3850.
Brascamp, J. W., Knapen, T. H. J., Kanai, R., van Ee, R., and van den Berg, A. V. (2007). Flash Suppression and Flash Facilitation in Binocular Rivalry. Journal of Vision, 7(12), 12–12.
Carrasco, M., Ling, S., and Read, S. (2004). Attention Alters Appearance. Nature Neuroscience, 7(3), 308–313.
Chang, S., and Pearson, J. (2018). The Functional Effects of Prior Motion Imagery and Motion Perception. Cortex, 105, 83–96.
Chiou, R., Rich, A. N., Rogers, S., and Pearson, J. (2018). Exploring the Functional Nature of Synaesthetic Colour – Dissociations from Colour Perception and Imagery. Cognition, 177, 107–121.
Egeth, H. E., and Yantis, S. (1997). Visual Attention: Control, Representation, and Time Course. Annual Review of Psychology, 48(1), 269–297.
Galton, F. (1880). I.—Statistics of Mental Imagery. Mind, 19, 301–318.
Goebel, R., Khorram-Sefat, D., Muckli, L., Hacker, H., and Singer, W. (1998). The Constructive Nature of Vision: Direct Evidence from Functional Magnetic Resonance Imaging Studies of Apparent Motion and Motion Imagery. The European Journal of Neuroscience, 10(5), 1563–1573.
Gray, C. R., and Gummerman, K. (1975). The Enigmatic Eidetic Image: A Critical Examination of Methods, Data, and Theories. Psychological Bulletin, 82(3), 383–407.
Haber, R. N. (1979). Twenty Years of Haunting Eidetic Imagery: Where’s the Ghost? Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 2(4), 583–594.
Ishai, A., and Sagi, D. (1995). Common Mechanisms of Visual Imagery and Perception. Science, 268(5218), 1772–1774.
Ishai, A., Ungerleider, L. G., and Haxby, J. V. (2000). Distributed Neural Systems for the Generation of Visual Images. Neuron, 28(3), 979–990.
Jacobs, C., Schwarzkopf, D. S., and Silvanto, J. (2018). Visual Working Memory Performance in Aphantasia. Cortex, 105, 61–73.
Keogh, R., and Pearson, J. (2011). Mental Imagery and Visual Working Memory. PLoS ONE, 6(12), e29221.
Keogh, R., and Pearson, J. (2014). The Sensory Strength of Voluntary Visual Imagery Predicts Visual Working Memory Capacity. Journal of Vision, 14(12), 7–7.
Keogh, R., and Pearson, J. (2018). The Blind Mind: No Sensory Visual Imagery in Aphantasia. Cortex, 105, 53–60.
Koenig-Robert, R., and Pearson, J. (2019). Decoding the Contents and Strength of Imagery before Volitional Engagement. Scientific Reports, 9(1), 3504.
Kosslyn, S. M. (1973). Scanning Visual Images: Some Structural Implications. Perception & Psychophysics, 14(1), 90–94.
Kosslyn, S. M. (2005). Mental Images and the Brain. Cognitive Neuropsychology, 22(3), 333–347.
Laeng, B., and Sulutvedt, U. (2014). The Eye Pupil Adjusts to Imaginary Light. Psychological Science: a Journal of the American Psychological Society / APS, 25(1), 188–197.
Lewis, D. E., O’Reilly, M. J., Khuu, S. K., and Pearson, J. (2013). Conditioning the Mind’s Eye: Associative Learning with Voluntary Mental Imagery. Clinical Psychological Science, 1(4), 390–400.
Maróthi, R., and Kéri, S. (2018). Enhanced Mental Imagery and Intact Perceptual Organization in Schizotypal Personality Disorder. Psychiatry Research, 259, 433–438.
Meng, M., Remus, D. A., and Tong, F. (2005). Filling-in of Visual Phantoms in the Human Brain. Nature Neuroscience, 8(9), 1248–1254.
Mohr, H. M., Linder, N. S., Dennis, H., and Sireteanu, R. (2011). Orientation-Specific Aftereffects to Mentally Generated Lines. Perception, 40(3), 272–290.
Morina, N., Leibold, E., and Ehring, T. (2013). Vividness of General Mental Imagery Is Associated with the Occurrence of Intrusive Memories. Journal of Behavior Therapy and Experimental Psychiatry, 44(2), 221–226.
Morris, T., Spittle, M., and Watt, A. P. (2005). Imagery in Sport. Champaign, IL: Human Kinetics Books.
Naselaris, T., Olman, C. A., Stansbury, D. E., Ugurbil, K., and Gallant, J. L. (2015). A Voxel-Wise Encoding Model for Early Visual Areas Decodes Mental Images of Remembered Scenes. NeuroImage, 105, 215–228.
Pearson, J. (2014). New Directions in Mental-Imagery Research: The Binocular-Rivalry Technique and Decoding fMRI Patterns. Current Directions in Psychological Science, 23(3), 178–183.
Pearson, J., Clifford, C. W. G., and Tong, F. (2008). The Functional Impact of Mental Imagery on Conscious Perception. Current Biology: CB, 18(13), 982–986.
Pearson, J., and Keogh, R. (2019). Redefining Visual Working Memory: A Cognitive-Strategy, Brain-Region Approach. Current Directions in Psychological Science, 28(3), 266–273.
Pearson, J., and Kosslyn, S. M. (2015). The Heterogeneity of Mental Representation: Ending the Imagery Debate. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences, 112(33), 10089–10092.
Pearson, J., Naselaris, T., Holmes, E. A., and Kosslyn, S. M. (2015). Mental Imagery: Functional Mechanisms and Clinical Applications. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 19(10), 590–602.
Pearson, J., Rademaker, R. L., and Tong, F. (2011). Evaluating the Mind’s Eye: The Metacognition of Visual Imagery. Psychological Science, 22(12), 1535–1542.
Pearson, J., and Westbrook, F. (2015). Phantom Perception: Voluntary and Involuntary Non-Retinal Vision. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 19(5), 278–284.
Perky, C. W. (1910). An Experimental Study of Imagination. The American Journal of Psychology, 21(3), 422–452.
Pylyshyn, Z. (2001). Is the Imagery Debate Over? If So, What Was It About? In Depoux, E (ed.), Language, Brain, and Cognitive Development: Essays in Honor of Jacques Mehler. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press, 59–83.
Ranganath, C., and D’Esposito, M. (2005). Directing the Mind’s Eye: Prefrontal, Inferior and Medial Temporal Mechanisms for Visual Working Memory. Current Opinion in Neurobiology, 15(2), 175–182.
Sasaki, Y., and Watanabe, T. (2004). The Primary Visual Cortex Fills in Color. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America, 101(52), 18251–18256.
Schlack, A., and Albright, T. D. (2007). Remembering Visual Motion: Neural Correlates of Associative Plasticity and Motion Recall in Cortical Area MT. Neuron, 53(6), 881–890.
Schlegel, A., Kohler, P. J., Fogelson, S. V., et al. (2013). Network Structure and Dynamics of the Mental Workspace. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences, 110(40), 16277–16282.
Shepard, R. N., and Metzler, J. (1971). Mental Rotation of Three-Dimensional Objects. Science, 171(3972), 701–703.
Shine, J. M., Keogh, R., O’Callaghan, C., et al. (2014). Imagine That: Elevated Sensory Strength of Mental Imagery in Individuals with Parkinson’s Disease and Visual Hallucinations. Proceedings of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences, 282(1798), 20142047.
Slotnick, S. D., Thompson, W. L., and Kosslyn, S. M. (2005). Visual Mental Imagery Induces Retinotopically Organized Activation of Early Visual Areas. Cerebral Cortex, 15(10), 1570–1583.
Stokes, M., Thompson, R., Cusack, R., and Duncan, J. (2009). Top-Down Activation of Shape-Specific Population Codes in Visual Cortex During Mental Imagery. Journal of Neuroscience, 29(5), 1565–1572.
Stromeyer, C. F., and Psotka, J. (1970). The Detailed Texture of Eidetic Images. Nature, 225(5230), 346–349.
Tanaka, Y., and Sagi, D. (1998). A Perceptual Memory for Low-Contrast Visual Signals. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America, 95(21), 12729–12733.
Tartaglia, E. M., Bamert, L., Mast, F. W., and Herzog, M. H. (2009). Human Perceptual Learning by Mental Imagery. Current Biology: CB, 19(24), 2081–2085.
Thirion, B., Duchesnay, E., Hubbard, E., et al. (2006). Inverse Retinotopy: Inferring the Visual Content of Images from Brain Activation Patterns. NeuroImage, 33(4), 1104–1116.
Yomogida, Y. (2004). Mental Visual Synthesis Is Originated in the Fronto-temporal Network of the Left Hemisphere. Cerebral Cortex, 14(12), 1376–1383.
References
Agnew, M. (1922). The Auditory Imagery of Great Composers. Psychological Monographs, 31(1), 279.
Aranosian, C. M. (1981). Musical Creativity: The Stream of Consciousness in Composition, Improvisation, and Education. Imagination, Cognition and Personality, 1(1), 67–88.
Arora, S., Aggarwal, R., Sirimanna, P., et al. (2011). Mental Practice Enhances Surgical Technical Skills: A Randomized Controlled Study. Annals of Surgery, 253(2), 265–270.
Bailes, F. A. (2002). Musical Imagery: Hearing and Imagining Music. Sheffield, UK: University of Sheffield, PhD thesis.
Bailes, F. A. (2006). The Use of Experience-Sampling Methods to Monitor Musical Imagery in everyday life. Musicae Scientiae, 10, 173–190.
Bailes, F. A. (2007a). Timbre as an Elusive Component of Imagery for Music. Empirical Musicology Review, 2, 21–34.
Bailes, F. A. (2007b). The Prevalence and Nature of Imagined Music in the Everyday Lives of Music Students. Psychology of Music, 35, 555–570.
Bailes, F. A. (2015). Music in Mind? An Experience Sampling Study of What and When, Towards an Understanding of Why. Psychomusicology: Music, Mind and Brain, 25(1), 58–68.
Bailes, F., Bishop, L., Stevens, C. J., and Dean, R. T. (2012). Mental Imagery for Musical Changes in Loudness. Frontiers in Psychology, 3 (Dec).
Baird, B., Smallwood, J., Mrazek, M. D., et al. (2012). Inspired by Distraction: Mind-Wandering Facilitates Creative Incubation. Psychological Science, 23, 1117–1122.
Bangert, M., Peschel, T., Schlaug, G., et al. (2006). Shared Networks for Auditory and Motor Processing in Professional Pianists: Evidence from fMRI Conjunction. NeuroImage, 30(3), 917–926.
Baruss, I., and Wammes, M. (2009). Characteristics of Spontaneous Musical Imagery. Journal Of Consciousness Studies, 16(1), 37–61.
Beaman, C. P., and Williams, T. I. (2010). Earworms (Stuck Song Syndrome): Towards a Natural History of Intrusive Thoughts. British Journal of Psychology, 101, 637–653.
Beaman, C. P., and Williams, T. I. (2013). Individual Differences in Mental Control Predict Involuntary Musical Imagery. Musicae Scientiae, 17, 398–409.
Beaty, R. E., Burgin, C. J., Nusbaum, E. C., et al. (2013). Music to the Inner Ears: Exploring Individual Differences in Musical Imagery. Consciousness and Cognition, 22(4), 1163–1173.
Bishop, L., Bailes, F., and Dean, R. T. (2013a). Musical Expertise and the Ability to Imagine Loudness. PLoS ONE, 8(2).
Bishop, L., Bailes, F., and Dean, R. T. (2013b). Musical Imagery and the Planning of Dynamics and Articulation During Performance. Music Perception: An Interdisciplinary Journal, 31(2), 97–117.
Blood, A. J., and Zatorre, R. J. (2001). Intensely Pleasurable Responses to Music Correlate with Activity in Brain Regions Implicated in Reward and Emotion. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences, 98(20), 11818–11823.
Brown, S. (2006). The Perpetual Music Track: The Phenomenon of Constant Musical Imagery. Journal of Consciousness Studies, 13(6), 43–62.
Byron, T. P., and Fowles, L. C. (2015). Repetition and Recency Increases Involuntary Musical Imagery of Previously Unfamiliar Songs. Psychology of Music, 43(3), 375–389.
Chamorro-Premuzic, T., and Furnham, A. (2007). Personality and Music: Can Traits Explain How People Use Music in Everyday Life? British Journal of Psychology, 98(2), 175–185.
Chen, J. L., Penhune, V. B., and Zatorre, R. J. (2008). Moving on Time: Brain Network for Auditory-Motor Synchronization Is Modulated by Rhythm Complexity and Musical Training. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 20(2), 226–239.
Clynes, M., and Walker, J. (1982). Neurobiologic Functions of Rhythm, Time, and Pulse in Music. In Clynes, M (ed.), Music, Mind, and Brain: The Neuropsychology of Music. New York: Plenum, 171–216.
Coffman, D. D. (1990). Effects of Mental Practice, Physical Practice, and Knowledge of Results on Piano Performance. Journal of Research in Music Education, 38(3), 187.
Connolly, C., and Williamon, A. (2004). Mental Skills Training. In Williamon, A (ed.), Music Excellence: Strategies and Techniques to Enhance Performance. Oxford, UK: Oxford University Press, 221–242.
Cotter, K. N., Christensen, A. P., and Silvia, P. J. (2018). Understanding Inner Music: A Dimensional Approach to Musical Imagery. Psychology of Aesthetics, Creativity, and the Arts. dx.doi.org/10.1037/aca0000195.
Cotter, K. N., and Silvia, P. J. (2017). Measuring Mental Music: Comparing Retrospective and Experience Sampling Methods for Assessing Musical Imagery. Psychology of Aesthetics, Creativity, and the Arts, 11(3), 335–343.
Crowder, R. G. (1989). Imagery for Musical Timbre. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 15(3), 472.
Davidson-Kelly, K., Schaefer, R. S., Moran, N., and Overy, K. (2015). “Total Inner Memory”: Deliberate Uses of Multimodal Musical Imagery During Performance Preparation. Psychomusicology: Music, Mind, and Brain, 25(1), 83.
Driskell, J. E., Copper, C., and Moran, A. (1994). Does Mental Practice Enhance Performance? Journal of Applied Psychology, 79(4), 481–492.
Dunbar, R. (2012). On the Evolutionary Function of Song and Dance. In Bannan, N (ed.), Music, Language and Human Evolution. Oxford, UK: Oxford University Press, 201–214.
Eerola, T., Friberg, A., and Bresin, R. (2013). Emotional Expression in Music: Contribution, Linearity, and Additivity of Primary Musical Cues. Frontiers in Psychology, 4, 487.
Farah, M. J., and Smith, A. F. (1983). Perceptual Interference and Facilitation with Auditory Imagery. Perception & Psychophysics, 33(5), 475–478.
Farrugia, N., Jakubowski, K., Cusack, R., and Stewart, L. (2015). Tunes Stuck in Your Brain: The Frequency and Affective Evaluation of Involuntary Musical Imagery Correlate with Cortical Structure. Consciousness and Cognition, 35, 66–77.
Fine, P. A., Wise, K. J., Goldemberg, R., and Bravo, A. (2015). Performing Musicians’ Understanding of the Terms “Mental Practice” and “Score Analysis”. Psychomusicology: Music, Mind, and Brain, 25(1), 69–82.
Fitch, W. T. (2015). Four Principles of Bio-Musicology. Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society of London. Series B, Biological Sciences, 370(1664), 20140091.
Floridou, G. A. (2016). Investigating the Relationship between Involuntary Musical Imagery and Other Forms of Spontaneous Cognition. London, UK: Goldsmiths, University of London, PhD thesis.
Floridou, G. A., and Müllensiefen, D. (2015). Environmental and Mental Conditions Predicting the Experience of Involuntary Musical Imagery: An Experience Sampling Method Study. Consciousness and Cognition, 33, 472–486.
Floridou, G., Williamson, V. J., and Müllensiefen, D. (2012). Contracting Earworms: The Roles of Personality and Musicality. In Cambouropoulos, E, Tsougras, C, Mavromatis, K, and Pastiadis, K, (eds.), Proceedings of ICMPC-ESCOM 12, Thessaloniki, Greece, 302–310.
Floridou, G. A., Williamson, V. J., and Stewart, L. (2017). A Novel Indirect Method for Capturing Involuntary Musical Imagery under Varying Cognitive Load. Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 70(11), 2189–2199.
Floridou, G. A., Williamson, V. J., Stewart, L., and Müllensiefen, D. (2015). The Involuntary Musical Imagery Scale (IMIS). Psychomusicology: Music, Mind and Brain, 25(1), 28–36.
Forster, S., and Lavie, N. (2009). Harnessing the Wandering Mind: The Role of Perceptual Load. Cognition, 111(3), 345–355.
Gabrielsson, A., and Lindström, E. (2010). The Role of Structure in the Musical Expression of Emotions. In Juslin, P. N. and Sloboda, J. A. (eds.), Handbook of Music and Emotion: Theory, Research, and Applications. Oxford, UK: Oxford University Press, 367–400.
Gelding, R. W., Thompson, W. F., and Johnson, B. W. (2015). The Pitch Imagery Arrow Task: Effects of Musical Training, Vividness, and Mental Control. PLoS ONE, 10(3), e0121809.
Gordon, E. E. (1999). All About Audiation and Music Aptitudes. Music Educators Journal, 86(2), 41–44.
Gordon, E. E. (2003). Learning Sequences in Music: Skill, Content, and Patterns. Chicago, IL: GIA Publications.
Gould, D., Voelker, D. F., Damarjian, N., and Greenleaf, C. (2014). Imagery Training for Peak Performance. In van Raalte, J. L. and Brewer, B. W. (eds.), Exploring Sport and Exercise Psychology. 3rd edition. Washington, DC: American Psychological Association, 55–82.
Griffiths, T. D. (2000). Musical Hallucinosis in Acquired Deafness: Phenomenology and Brain Substrate. Brain, 123(10), 2065–2076.
Hallam, S. (2010). The Power of Music: Its Impact on the Intellectual, Social and Personal Development of Children and Young People. International Journal of Music Education, 28(3), 269–289.
Halpern, A. R. (1988). Perceived and Imagined Tempos of Familiar Songs. Music Perception, 6(2), 193–202.
Halpern, A. R. (1989). Memory for the Absolute Pitch of Familiar Songs. Memory & Cognition, 17(5), 572–581.
Halpern, A. R. (1992). Musical Aspects of Auditory Imagery. In Reisberg, D (ed.), Auditory Imagery. Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum, 1–27.
Halpern, A. R. (2015). Differences in Auditory Imagery Self-Report Predict Neural and Behavioral Outcomes. Psychomusicology: Music, Mind, and Brain, 25(1), 37–47.
Halpern, A. R., and Bartlett, J. C. (2011). The Persistence of Musical Memories: A Descriptive Study of Earworms. Music Perception, 28(4), 425–432.
Halpern, A. R., Zatorre, R. J., Bouffard, M., and Johnson, J. A. (2004). Behavioral and Neural Correlates of Perceived and Imagined Musical Timbre. Neuropsychologia, 42(9), 1281–1292.
Herholz, S. C., Halpern, A. R., and Zatorre, R. J. (2012). Neuronal Correlates of Perception, Imagery, and Memory for Familiar Tunes. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 24(6), 1382–1397.
Herholz, S. C., Lappe, C., Knief, A., and Pantev, C. (2008). Neural Basis of Music Imagery and the Effect of Musical Expertise. The European Journal of Neuroscience, 28(11), 2352–2360.
Highben, Z., and Palmer, C. (2004). Effects of Auditory and Motor Mental Practice in Memorized Piano Performance. Bulletin of the Council for Research in Music Education, 159, 58–65.
Holmes, P. (2005). Imagination in Practice: A Study of the Integrated Roles of Interpretation, Imagery and Technique in the Learning and Memorisation Processes of Two Experienced Solo Performers. British Journal of Music Education, 22(3), 217–235.
Hyde, K. L., Lerch, J., Norton, A., et al. (2009). The Effects of Musical Training on Structural Brain Development: A Longitudinal Study. Annals of the New York Academy of Sciences, 1169(1), 182–186.
Hyman, I. E., Burland, N. K., Duskin, H. M., et al. (2013). Going Gaga: Investigating, Creating, and Manipulating the Song Stuck in My Head. Applied Cognitive Psychology, 27, 204–215.
Hyman, I. E., Cutshaw, K. I., Hall, C. M., et al. (2015). Involuntary to Intrusive: Using Involuntary Musical Imagery to Explore Individual Differences and the Nature of Intrusive Thoughts. Psychomusicology: Music, Mind and Brain, 25(1), 14–27.
Intons-Peterson, M. J. (1980). The Role of Loudness in Auditory Imagery. Memory & Cognition, 8(5), 385–393. dx.doi.org/10.3758/BF03211134.
Intons-Peterson, M. J. (1992). Components of Auditory Imagery. In Reisberg, D (ed.), Auditory Imagery. Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates, 45–72.
Jakubowski, K., Bashir, Z., Farrugia, N., and Stewart, L. (2018). Involuntary and Voluntary Recall of Musical Memories: A Comparison of Temporal Accuracy and Emotional Responses. Memory and Cognition, 46(5), 741–756.
Jakubowski, K., Farrugia, N., Halpern, A. R., Sankarpandi, S. K., and Stewart, L. (2015). The Speed of Our Mental Soundtracks: Tracking the Tempo of Involuntary Musical Imagery in Everyday Life. Memory and Cognition, 43(8), 1229–1242.
Jakubowski, K., Farrugia, N., and Stewart, L. (2016). Probing Imagined Tempo for Music: Effects of Motor Engagement and Musical Experience. Psychology of Music, 44(6), 1274–1288.
Jakubowski, K., Finkel, S., Stewart, L., and Müllensiefen, D. (2017). Dissecting an Earworm: Melodic Features and Song Popularity Predict Involuntary Musical Imagery. Psychology of Aesthetics, Creativity, and the Arts, 11(2), 122–135.
Janata, P., and Paroo, K. (2006). Acuity of Auditory Images in Pitch and Time. Perception & Psychophysics, 68(5), 829–844.
Keller, P. E. (2012). Mental Imagery in Music Performance: Underlying Mechanisms and Potential Benefits. Annals of the New York Academy of Sciences, 1252, 206–213.
Keller, P. E., and Appel, M. (2010). Individual Differences, Auditory Imagery, and the Coordination of Body Movements and Sounds in Musical Ensembles. Music Perception, 28(1), 27–46.
Keller, P. E., Dalla Bella, S., and Koch, I. (2010). Auditory Imagery Shapes Movement Timing and Kinematics: Evidence from a Musical Task. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 36(2), 508–513.
Keller, P. E., and Koch, I. (2006). The Planning and Execution of Short Auditory Sequences. Psychonomic Bulletin & Review, 13(4), 711–716.
Keller, P. E., and Koch, I. (2008). Action Planning in Sequential Skills: Relations to Music Performance. Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 61(2), 275–291.
Kleber, B., Birbaumer, N., Veit, R., Trevorrow, T., and Lotze, M. (2007). Overt and Imagined Singing of an Italian Aria. NeuroImage, 36(3), 889–900.
Koelsch, S. (2014). Brain Correlates of Music-Evoked Emotions. Nature Reviews Neuroscience, 15, 170–180.
Kraemer, D. J. M., Macrae, C. N., Green, A. E., and Kelley, W. M. (2005). Musical Imagery: Sound of Silence Activates Auditory Cortex. Nature, 434(7030), 158.
Lancashire, R. (2017). An Experience-Sampling Study to Investigate the Role of Familiarity in Involuntary Musical Imagery Induction. In Harrison, P. M. C. (ed.), Proceedings of the 10th International Conference of Students of Systematic Musicology (SysMus17), London, UK.
Liikkanen, L. A. (2012a). Musical Activities Predispose to Involuntary Musical Imagery. Psychology of Music, 40, 236–256.
Liikkanen, L. A. (2012b). Inducing Involuntary Musical Imagery: An Experimental Study. Musicae Scientiae, 16(2), 217–234.
Liikkanen, L. A., Jakubowski, K., and Toivanen, J. M. (2015). Catching Earworms on Twitter: Using Big Data to Study Involuntary Musical Imagery. Music Perception, 33(2), 199–216.
Limb, C. J., and Braun, A. R. (2008). Neural Substrates of Spontaneous Musical Performance: An fMRI Study of Jazz Improvisation. PLoS one, 3(2), e1679.
Lucas, B. J., Schubert, E., and Halpern, A. R. (2010). Perception of Emotion in Sounded and Imagined Music. Music Perception, 27(5), 399–412.
McAdams, S., Winsberg, S., Donnadieu, S., de Soete, G., and Krimphoff, J. (1995). Perceptual Scaling of Synthesized Musical Timbres: Common Dimensions, Specificities, and Latent Subject Classes. Psychological Research, 58(3), 177–192.
McCullough Campbell, S., and Margulis, E. H. (2015). Catching an Earworm Through Movement. Journal of New Music Research, 44(4), 347–358.
McDermott, J., and Hauser, M. (2005). The Origins of Music: Innateness, Uniqueness, and Evolution. Music Perception, 23(1), 29–59.
McNally-Gagnon, A. (2016). Imagerie musicale involontaire: caractéristiques phénoménologiques et mnésiques. Montreal, Canada: University of Montreal, PhD thesis.
Mountain, R. (2001). Composers and Imagery: Myths and Realities. In Godøy, R. I. and Jörgensen, H (eds.), Musical Imagery. New York, NY: Taylor & Francis, 271–288.
Müllensiefen, D., Jones, R., Jilka, S., Stewart, L., and Williamson, V. J. (2014). Individual Differences Predict Patterns in Spontaneous Involuntary Musical Imagery. Music Perception, 31(4), 323–338.
North, A. C., Hargreaves, D. J., and Hargreaves, J. J. (2004). Uses of Music in Everyday Life. Music Perception: An Interdisciplinary Journal, 22(1), 41–77.
Okada, H., and Matsuoka, K. (1992). Effects of Auditory Imagery on the Detection of a Pure Tone in White Noise: Experimental Evidence of the Auditory Perky Effect. Perceptual and Motor Skills, 74(2), 443–448.
Pecenka, N., and Keller, P. E. (2009). Auditory Pitch Imagery and Its Relationship to Musical Synchronization. Annals of the New York Academy of Sciences, 1169, 282–286.
Pitt, M. A., and Crowder, R. G. (1992). The Role of Spectral and Dynamic Cues in Imagery for Musical Timbre. Journal of Experimental Psychology. Human Perception and Performance, 18(3), 728–738.
Reybrouck, M. (2001). Musical Imagery between Sensory Processing and Ideomotor Simulation. In Godøy, R. I. and Jörgensen, H (eds.), Musical imagery. New York: Taylor & Francis, 117–136.
Ross, S. L. (1985). The Effectiveness of Mental Practice in Improving the Performance of College Trombonists. Journal of Research in Music Education, 33(4), 221.
Schaefer, R. S., Desain, P., and Farquhar, J. (2013). Shared Processing of Perception and Imagery of Music in Decomposed EEG. NeuroImage, 70, 317–326. dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.neuroimage.2012.12.064.
Sloboda, J. A., O’Neill, S. A., and Ivaldi, A. (2001). Functions of Music in Everyday Life: An Exploratory Study Using the Experience Sampling Method. Musicae Scientiae, 5(1), 9–32.
Stewart, L., von Kriegstein, K., Warren, J. D., and Griffiths, T. D. (2006). Music and the Brain: Disorders of Musical Listening. Brain, 129(10), 2533–2553.
Taylor, S., McKay, D., Miguel, E. C., et al. (2014). Musical Obsessions: A Comprehensive Review of Neglected Clinical Phenomena. Journal of Anxiety Disorders, 28(6), 580–589.
Teasdale, J. D., Dritschel, B. H., Taylor, M. J., et al. (1995). Stimulus-Independent Thought Depends on Central Executive Resources. Memory & Cognition, 23(5), 551–559.
Trusheim, W. H. (1991). Audiation and Mental Imagery: Implications for Artistic Performance. Quarterly Journal of Music Teaching and Learning, 2, 138–147.
Weber, R. J., and Brown, S. (1986). Musical Imagery. Music Perception, 3(4), 411–426.
Weir, G., Williamson, V. J., and Müllensiefen, D. (2015). Increased Involuntary Musical Mental Activity Is Not Associated with More Accurate Voluntary Musical Imagery. Psychomusicology: Music, Mind, and Brain, 25(1), 48–57.
Williams, T. I. (2015). The Classification of Involuntary Musical Imagery: The Case for Earworms. Psychomusicology: Music, Mind and Brain, 25(1), 5–13.
Williamson, V. J., and Jilka, S. R. (2013). Experiencing Earworms: An Interview Study of Involuntary Musical Imagery. Psychology of Music, 42 (5), 653–670.
Williamson, V. J., Jilka, S. R., Fry, J., et al. (2012). How Do Earworms Start? Classifying the Everyday Circumstances of Involuntary Musical Imagery. Psychology of Music, 40(3), 259–284.
Williamson, V. J., and Müllensiefen, D. (2012). Earworms from Three Angles. In Cambouropoulos, E, Tsougras, C, Mavromatis, K, and Pastiadis, K, (eds.), Proceedings of ICMPC-ESCOM 12, Thessaloniki, Greece, 1124–1133.
Zatorre, R. J., and Halpern, A. R. (2005). Mental Concerts: Musical Imagery and Auditory Cortex. Neuron 47(1), 9–12.
Zatorre, R. J., Halpern, A. R., and Bouffard, M. (2010). Mental Reversal of Imagined Melodies: A Role for the Posterior Parietal Cortex. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 22(4), 775–789.
Zatorre, R. J., Halpern, A. R., Perry, D. W., Meyer, E., and Evans, A. C. (1996). Hearing in the Mind’s Ear: A PET Investigation of Musical Imagery and Perception. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 8(1), 29–46.
References
Adam, H., and Galinsky, A. D. (2012). Enclothed Cognition. Journal of Experimental Social Psychology, 48(4), 918–925.
Beyer, L., Weiss, T., Hansen, E., Wolf, A., and Seidel, A. (1990). Dynamics of Central Nervous Activation during Motor Imagination. International Journal of Psychophysiology, 9, 75–80.
Borst, G. (2013). Neural Underpinning of Object Mental Imagery, Spatial Imagery, and Motor Imagery. In Oschner, K. N. and Kosslyn, S (eds.), The Oxford Handbook of Cognitive Neuroscience, Vol 2: The Cutting Edges. Oxford, UK: Oxford University Press, 74–87.
Boulton, H., and Mitra, S. (2013). Body Posture Modulates Imagined Arm Movements and Responds to Them. Journal of Neurophysiology, 110, 2617–2626.
Callow, N., Hardy, L., and Hall, C. (2001). The Effect of a Motivational General-Mastery Imagery Intervention on the Sport Confidence of High-Level Badminton Players. Research Quarterly for Exercise and Sport, 72, 389–400.
Callow, N., Roberts, R., and Fawkes, J. Z. (2006). Effects of Dynamic and Static Imagery on Vividness of Imagery, Skiing Performance, and Confidence. Journal of Imagery Research in Sport and Physical Activity, 1, 1–15.
Callow, N., and Waters, A. (2005). The Effect of Kinesthetic Imagery on the Sport Confidence of Flat-Race Horse Jockeys. Psychology of Sport and Exercise, 6, 443–459.
Calvo-Merino Glaser, D. E., Grèzes, J., Passingham, R. E., and Haggard, P. (2005). Action Observation and Acquired Motor Skills: An fMRI Study with Expert Dancers. Cerebral Cortex, 15, 1243–1249.
Chang, Y., Lee, J. J., Seo, J. H., et al. (2011). Neural Correlates of Motor Imagery for Elite Archers. NMR Biomedicine, 24, 366–372.
Collet, C., Di Rienzo, F., El Hoyek, N., and Guillot, A. (2013). Autonomic Nervous System Correlates in Movement Observation and Motor Imagery. Frontiers in Human Neuroscience, 7, 415.
Collet, C., Guillot, A., Lebon, F., MacIntyre, T., and Moran, A. (2011). Measuring Motor Imagery Using Psychometric, Behavioural, and Psychophysiological Tools. Exercise and Sport Sciences Reviews, 39, 85–92.
Cumming, J., and Williams, S. E. (2012). Imagery: The Role of Imagery in Performance. In Murphy, S (ed.), Handbook of Sport and Performance Psychology. New York, NY: Oxford University Press, 213–232.
Debarnot, U., Castellani, E., and Guillot, A. (2012). Selective Delayed Gains Following Motor Imagery of Complex Movements. Archives Italiennes de Biologie, 150, 238–250.
Debarnot, U., Castellani, E., Valenza, G., Sebastiani, L., and Guillot, A. (2011). Daytime Naps Improve Motor Imagery Learning. Cognitive and Affective Behavioral Neuroscience, 11, 541–550.
Debarnot, U., Creveaux, T., Collet, C., Doyon, J., and Guillot, A. (2009). Sleep Contribution to Motor Memory Consolidation: A Motor Imagery Study. Sleep, 32, 1559–1565.
Debarnot, U., Maley, L., Rossi, D. D., and Guillot, A. (2010). Motor Interference Does Not Impair the Memory Consolidation of Imagined Movements. Brain and Cognition, 74, 52–57.
Debarnot, U., Sperduti, M., Di Rienzo, F., and Guillot, A. (2014). Experts’ Bodies, Experts’ Minds: How Physical and Mental Training Shape the Brain. Frontiers in Human Neuroscience, 8, 280.
Decety, J., Jeannerod, M., Germain, M., and Pastene, J. (1991). Vegetative Response during Imagined Movement Is Proportional to Mental Effort. Behavioural Brain Research, 42, 1–5.
Decety, J., Perani, D., Jeannerod, M., et al. (1994). Mapping Motor Representations with Positron Emission Tomography. Nature, 371, 600–602.
Demougeot, L., and Papaxanthis, C. (2011). Muscle Fatigue Affects Mental Simulation of Action. Journal of Neuroscience, 31, 10712–10720.
Deschaumes-Molinaro, C., Dittmar, A., and Vernet-Maury, E. (1992). Autonomic Nervous System Response Patterns Correlate with Mental Imagery. Physiology and Behavior, 51, 1021–1027.
Di Rienzo, F., Collet, C., Hoyek, N., and Guillot, A. (2012). Selective Effects of Physical Fatigue on Motor Imagery Accuracy. PLoS One, 7, e47207, 1–11.
Di Rienzo, F., Collet, C., Hoyek, N., and Guillot, A. (2014). Impact of Neurologic Deficits on Motor Imagery: A Systematic Review of Clinical Evaluations. Neuropsychology Review, 24, 116–147.
Di Rienzo, F., Debarnot, U., Daligault, D., et al. (2016). Online and Offline Performance Gains Following Motor Imagery: A Comprehensive Review of Behavioral and Neuroimaging Studies. Frontiers in Human Neuroscience, 10, article 315, 1–15.
Dickstein, R., Gazit-Grunwald, M., Plax, M., Dunsky, A., and Marcovitz, E. (2005). EMG Activity in Selected Target Muscles during Imagery Rising on Tiptoes in Healthy Adults and Poststroke Hemiparetic Patients. Journal of Motor Behavior, 37, 475–483.
Doyon, J., and Benali, H. (2005). Reorganization and Plasticity in the Adult Brain during Learning of Motor Skills. Current Opinion in Neurobiology, 15, 161–167.
Driskell, J. E., Copper, C., and Moran, A. (1994). Does Mental Practice Enhance Performance? Journal of Applied Psychology, 79, 481–492.
Eaves, D. L., Riach, M., Holmes, P. S., and Wright, D. J. (2016). Motor Imagery during Action Observation: A Brief Review of Evidence, Theory and Future Research Opportunities. Frontiers in Neuroscience, 10, 514.
Ehrsson, H. H., Geyer, S., and Naito, E. (2003). Imagery of Voluntary Movement of Fingers, Toes and Tongue Activates Corresponding Body-Part-Specific Motor Representations. Journal of Neurophysiology, 90, 3304–3316.
Feltz, D. L., and Landers, D. M. (1983). The Effects of Mental Practice on Motor Skill Learning and Performance: A Meta-Analysis. Journal of Sport and Exercise Psychology, 5, 25–57.
Filgueiras, A., Quintas Conde, E. F., and Hall, C. R. (2017). The Neural Basis of Kinesthetic and Visual Imagery in Sports: An ALE Meta-Analysis. Brain Imaging Behaviour, 12, 1513–1523.
Grangeon, M., Guillot, A., and Collet, C. (2011). Postural Control during Visual and Kinesthetic Motor Imagery. Applied Psychophysiology and Biofeedback, 36, 47–56.
Guillot, A., and Collet, C. (2005). Contribution from Neurophysiological and Psychological Methods to the Study of Motor Imagery. Brain Research Review, 50, 387–397.
Guillot, A., and Collet, C. (2008). Construction of the Motor Imagery Integrative Model in Sport: A Review and Theoretical Investigation of Motor Imagery Use. International Review of Sport and Exercise Psychology, 1, 31–44.
Guillot, A., Collet, C., and Dittmar, A. (2005). Influence of Environmental Context on Motor Imagery Quality. Biology of Sport, 22, 215–226.
Guillot, A., Collet, C., Nguyen, V.A., et al. (2009). Brain Activity during Visual versus Kinesthetic Imagery: An fMRI Study. Human Brain Mapping, 30, 2157–2172.
Guillot, A., Di Rienzo, F., Macintyre, T., Moran, A., and Collet, C. (2012a). Imagining Is Not Doing but Involves Specific Motor Commands: A Review of Experimental Data Related to Motor Inhibition. Frontiers in Human Neuroscience, 6, 1–22.
Guillot, A., Haguenauer, M., Dittmar, A., and Collet, C. (2005). Effect of a Fatiguing Protocol on Motor Imagery Accuracy. European Journal of Applied Physiology, 95(2–3), 186–190.
Guillot, A., Hoyek, N., Louis, M., and Collet, C. (2012b). Understanding the Timing of Motor Imagery: Recent Findings and Future Directions. International Review of Sport and Exercise Psychology, 5, 3–22.
Guillot, A., Lebon, F., and Collet, C. (2010). Electromyographic Activity during Motor Imagery. In Guillot, A and Collet, C (eds.), The Neurophysiological Foundations of Mental and Motor Imagery. New York, NY: Oxford University Press, 83–93.
Guillot, A., Moschberger, K., and Collet, C. (2013). Coupling Movement with Imagery as a New Perspective for Motor Imagery Practice. Behavioural Brain Functions 9, 8.
Halpern, A. R. (2012). Dynamic Aspects of Musical Imagery. Annals of New York Academy of Science, 1252, 200–205.
Hardwick, R. M., Caspers, S., Eickhoff, S. B., and Swinnen, S. P. (2018). Neural Correlates of Action: Comparing Meta-Analyses of Imagery, Observation, and Execution. Neuroscience and Biobehavioral Reviews, 94, 31–44.
Hétu, S., Grégoire, M., Saimpont, A., et al. (2013). The Neural Network of Motor Imagery: An ALE Meta-Analysis. Neuroscience and Biobehavioral Reviews, 37(5), 930–949.
Holmes, P. S., and Collins, D. J. (2001). The PETTLEP Approach to Motor Imagery: A Functional Equivalence Model for Sport Psychologists. Journal of Applied Sport Psychology, 13, 60–83.
Jackson, P. L., Lafleur, M. F., Malouin, F., Richards, C. L., and Doyon, J. (2003). Functional Cerebral Reorganization Following Motor Sequence Learning through Mental Practice with Motor Imagery. NeuroImage, 20, 1171–1180.
Jackson, P. L., Meltzoff, A. L., and Decety, J. (2006). Neural Circuits Involved in Imitation and Perspective-Taking. NeuroImage, 31, 429–439.
Janssen, J. J., and Sheikh, A. A. (1994). Enhancing Athletic Performance through Imagery: An Overview. In: Sheikh, A. A. and Korn, E. R. (eds.), Imagery and Sports Physical Performance. Amityville, NY: Bayood Publishing, 175–181.
Jeannerod, M. (1994). The Representing Brain: Neural Correlates of Motor Intention and Imagery. Behavioural Brain Sciences, 17, 187–202.
Jeannerod, M. (2001). Neural Simulation of Action: A Unifying Mechanism for Motor Cognition. NeuroImage, 14, S103–109.
Jeannerod, M. (2006). Motor Cognition: What Actions Tell to the Self. New York, NY: Oxford University Press.
Jiang, D., Edwards, M. G., Mullins, P., and Callow, N. (2015). The Neural Substrates for the Different Modalities of Movement Imagery. Brain and Cognition, 97, 22–31.
Kanthack, T. F. D., Guillot, A., Altimari, L. R., et al. (2016). Selective Efficacy of Static and Dynamic Imagery in Different States of Physical Fatigue. PLoS One, 11, e0149654.
Kanthack, T. F. D., Guillot, A., Clémençon, M., and Di Rienzo, F. (2019). Effect of Physical Fatigue Elicited by Continuous and Intermittent Exercise on Motor Imagery Ability. Submitted for publication.
Lacourse, M. G., Turner, J. A., Randolph-Orr, E., Schandler, S. L., and Cohen, M. J. (2004). Cerebral and Cerebellar Sensorimotor Plasticity Following Motor Imagery-Based Mental Practice of a Sequential Movement. Journal of Rehabilitation Research and Development, 41, 505–524.
Lafleur, M. F., Jackson, P. L., Malouin, F., et al. (2002). Motor Learning Produces Parallel Dynamic Functional Changes during the Execution and Imagination of Sequential Foot Movements. NeuroImage, 16, 142–157.
Lebon, F., Horn, U., Domin, M., and Lotze, M. (2018). Motor Imagery Training: Kinesthetic Imagery Strategy and Inferior Parietal fMRI Activation. Human Brain Mapping, 39, 1805–1813.
Lemos, T., Souza, N. S., Horsczaruk, C. H., et al. (2014). Motor Imagery Modulation of Body Sway Is Task-Dependent and Relies on Imagery Ability. Frontiers in Human Neuroscience, 8, 290.
Lorey, B., Bischoff, M., Pilgramm, S., et al. (2009). The Embodied Nature of Motor Imagery: The Influence of Posture and Perspective. Experimental Brain Research, 194, 233–243.
Lotze, M., and Halsband, U. (2006). Motor Imagery. Journal of Physiology (Paris), 99, 386–395.
Lotze, M., Montoya, P., Erb, M., et al. (1999). Activation of Cortical and Cerebellar Motor Areas during Executed and Imagined Hand Movements: An fMRI Study. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 11, 491–501.
Lotze, M., Scheler, G., Tan, H. R., Braun, C., and Birbaumer, N. (2003). The Musician’s Brain: Functional Imaging of Amateurs and Professionals during Performance and Imagery. NeuroImage, 20, 1817–1829.
Louis, C., Collet, C., and Guillot, A. (2011). Differences in Motor Imagery Times during Aroused and Relaxed Conditions. Journal of Cognitive Psychology, 23, 374–382.
Louis, M., Guillot, A., Maton, S., Doyon, J., and Collet, C. (2008). Effect of Imagined Movement Speed on Subsequent Motor Performance. Journal of Motor Behavior, 40, 117–132.
MacIntyre, T. E., Madan, C. R., Moran, A. P., Collet, C., and Guillot, A. (2018). Motor Imagery, Performance and Motor Rehabilitation. Progress in Brain Research, 240, 141–159.
Macuga, K. L., and Frey, S. H. (2012). Neural Representations Involved in Observed, Imagined, and Imitated Actions Are Dissociable and Hierarchically Organized. NeuroImage, 59, 2798–2807.
Miller, E. (1994). Optimal Sports Performance Imagery. In Sheikh, A. A and Korn, E. R. (eds.), Imagery and Sports Physical Performance. Amityville, NY: Bayood Publishing, 175–181.
Milton, J., Solodkin, A., Hlustik, P., and Small, S. L. (2007). The Mind of Expert Motor Performance Is Cool and Focused. NeuroImage, 35, 804–813.
Mizuguchi, N., Nakata, H., Hayashi, T., et al. (2013). Brain Activity during Motor Imagery of an Action with an Object: A Functional Magnetic Resonance Imaging Study. Neuroscience Research, 76(3), 150–155.
Mizuguchi, N., Nakata, H., and Kanosue, K. (2014). Activity of Right Premotor-Parietal Regions Dependent upon Imagined Force Level: An fMRI Study. Frontiers in Human Neuroscience, 8, 810.
Moran, A., Guillot, A., MacIntyre, T., and Collet, C. (2012). Re-imagining Mental Imagery: Building Bridges Between Cognitive and Sport Psychology. British Journal of Psychology, 103, 224–247.
Morris, T., Spittle, M., and Perry, C. (2004). Imagery in Sport. In Morris, T and Summers, J (eds.), Sport Psychology: Theory, Applications and Issues. 2nd edition. Brisbane, Australia: Wiley, 344–383.
Mulder, T., de Vries, S., and Zijlstra, S. (2005). Observation, Imagination and Execution of an Effortful Movement: More Evidence for a Central Explanation of Motor Imagery. Experimental Brain Research, 163, 344–351.
Munroe, K. J., Giacobbi, P. R., Hall, C., and Weinberg, R. (2000). The Four Ws of Imagery Use: Where, When, Why and What. The Sport Psychologist, 14, 119–137.
Munzert, J., Lorey, B., and Zentgraf, K. (2009). Cognitive Motor Processes: The Role of Motor Imagery in the Study of Motor Representations. Brain Research Reviews, 60, 306–326.
Murphy, S., Nordin, S. M., and Cumming, J. (2008). Imagery in Sport, Exercise and Dance. In Horn, T (ed.), Advances in Sport Psychology. Champaign, IL: Human Kinetics, 306–315.
Nedelko, V., Hassa, T., Hamzei, F., Schoenfeld, M. A., and Dettmers, C. (2012). Action Imagery Combined with Action Observation Activates more Corticomotor Regions than Action Observation Alone. Journal of Neurology and Physical Therapy, 36(4), 182–188.
O, J., and Hall, C. (2009). A Quantitative Analysis of Athletes’ Voluntary Use of Slow Motion, Real Time, and Fast Motion Images. Journal of Applied Sport Psychology, 21, 15–30.
O’Shea, H., and Moran, A. (2017). Does Motor Simulation Theory Explain the Cognitive Mechanisms Underlying Motor Imagery? A Critical Review. Frontiers in Human Neuroscience, 11, 72.
Paivio, A. (1985). Cognitive and Motivational Functions of Imagery in Human Performance. Canadian Journal of Applied Sport Science, 10(4), 22S–28S.
Roure, R., Collet, C., Deschaumes-Molinaro, C., et al. (1999). Imagery Quality Estimated by Autonomic Response Is Correlated to Sporting Performance Enhancement. Physiology and Behavior, 66, 63–72.
Rozand, V., Lebon, F., Papaxanthis, C., and Lepers, R. (2014). Does a Mental Training Session Induce Neuromuscular Fatigue? Medicine Science in Sports and Exercise, 46, 1981–1989.
Ruby, P., and Decety, J. (2001). Effect of Subjective Perspective Taking during Simulation of Action: A PET Investigation of Agency. Nature Neuroscience, 4, 546–550.
Sauvage, C., Jissendi, P., Seignan, S., Manto, M., and Habas, C. (2013). Brain Areas Involved in the Control of Speed during a Motor Sequence of the Foot: Real Movement versus Mental Imagery. Journal of Neuroradiology, 40, 267–280.
Schmidt, T. T., Ostwald, D., and Blankenburg, F. (2014). Imaging Tactile Imagery: Changes in Brain Connectivity Support Perceptual Grounding of Mental Images in Primary Sensory Cortices. NeuroImage, 98, 216–224.
Schuster, C., Hilfiker, R., Amft, O., et al. (2011). Best Practice for Motor Imagery: A Systematic Literature Review on Motor Imagery Training Elements in Five Different Disciplines. BMC Medicine, 9, 75.
Seiler, B. D., Monsma, E. V., and Newman-Norlund, R. D. (2015). Biological Evidence of Imagery Abilities: Intraindividual Differences. Journal of Sport and Exercise Psychology, 37, 421–435.
Sharma, N., Jones, P. S., Carpenter, T. A., and Baron, J. C. (2008). Mapping the Involvement of BA 4a and 4p during Motor Imagery. NeuroImage, 41, 92–99.
Shirazipour, C. H., Munroe-Chandler, K. J., Loughead, T. M., and Vander Laan, A. G. (2016). The Effect of Image Speed on Novice Golfers’ Performance in a Putting Task. Journal of Imagery Research in Sport and Physical Activity, 11, 1–12.
Smith, D., Wright, C., Allsopp, A., and Westhead, H. (2007). It’s All in the Mind: PETTLEP-Based Imagery and Sport Performance. Journal of Applied Sport Psychology, 19, 80–92.
Solodkin, A., Hlustik, P., Chen, E. E., and Small, S. L. (2004). Fine Modulation in Network Activation during Motor Execution and Motor Imagery. Cerebral Cortex, 14, 1246–1255.
Stins, J. F., Schneider, I. K., Koole, S. L., and Beek, P. J. (2015). The Influence of Motor Imagery on Postural Sway: Differential Effects of Type of Body Movement and Person Perspective. Advances in Cognitive Psychology, 11(3): 77–83.
Taube, W., Mouthon, M., Leukel, C., et al. (2015). Brain Activity during Observation and Motor Imagery of Different Balance Tasks: An fMRI Study. Cortex, 64, 102–114.
van der Meulen, M., Allali, G., Rieger, S. W., Assal, F., and Vuilleumier, P. (2014). The Influence of Individual Motor Imagery Ability on Cerebral Recruitment during Gait Imagery. Human Brain Mapping, 35, 455–470.
Vogt, S., Di Rienzo, F., Collet, C., Collins, A., and Guillot, A. (2013). Multiple Roles of Motor Imagery during Action Observation. Frontiers in Human Neuroscience, 7.
Wang, Y., and Morgan, W. P. (1992). The Effect of Imagery Perspectives on the Psychophysiological Responses to Imagined Exercise. Behavioural Brain Research, 52(2), 167–174.
Wei, G., and Luo, J. (2010). Sport Expert’s Motor Imagery: Functional Imaging of Professional Motor Skills and Simple Motor Skills. Brain Research, 1341, 52–62.
Wright, C. J., and Smith, D. (2009). The Effect of PETTLEP Imagery on Strength Performance. International Journal of Sport and Exercise Psychology, 7, 18–31.
References
Alais, D., and Burr, D. (2003). The “Flash-Lag” Effect Occurs in Audition and Cross-Modally, Current Biology, 13(1), 59–63.
Bakin, J., Nakayama, K., and Gilbert, C. (2000). Visual Responses in Monkey Areas V1 and V2 to Three-Dimensional Surface Configurations. Journal of Neuroscience, 20, 8188–8198.
Ban, H., Yamamoto, H., Hanakawa, T., et al. (2013). Topographic Representation of an Occluded Object and the Effects of Spatiotemporal Context in Human Early Visual Areas. Journal of Neuroscience, 33, 16992–17007.
Beck, J. (2018). Marking the Perception-Cognition Boundary. Australasian Journal of Philosophy, 96, 319–334.
Bullier, J. (2004). Communications between Cortical Areas of the Visual System. In Chalupa, L. M. and Werner, J. S. (eds.), The Visual Neurosciences. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press, 522–540.
Bushnell, B. N., Harding, P. J., Kosai, Y., and Pasupathy, A. (2011). Partial Occlusion Modulates Contour-Based Shape Encoding in Primate Area V4. Journal of Neuroscience, 31, 4012–4024.
Cai, M., Stetson, C., Eagleman, D. M. (2012). A Neural Model for Temporal Order Judgments and their Active Recalibration: A Common Mechanism for Space and Time? Frontiers in Psychology, 3, 470.
Ekman, M., Kok, P., and de Lange, F. (2017). Time-Compressed Preplay of Anticipated Events in Human Primary Visual Cortex. Nature Communications, 8.
Emmanouil, T., and Ro, T. (2014). Amodal Completion of Unconsciously Presented Objects. Psychonomic Bulletin & Review, 21(5), 1188–1194.
Esterman, M., and Yantis, S. (2010). Perceptual Expectation Modulates Category-Selective Cortical Activity. Cerebral Cortex, 20, 1245–1253.
Fujisaki, W., Shimojo, S., Kashino, M., and Nishida, S. (2004). Recalibration of Audiovisual Simultaneity. Nature Neuroscience, 7, 773–778.
Geldard, F. A., and Sherrick, C. E. (1972). The Cutaneous “Rabbit”: A Perceptual Illusion. Science, 178, 178–179.
Grice, H. P. (1961). The Causal Theory of Perception. Proceedings of the Aristotelian Society, Supplementary Volume, 35, 121–153.
Grill-Spector, K., and Malach, R. (2004). The Human Visual Cortex. Annual Review of Neuroscience, 27, 649–677.
Grush, R. (2007). Time and Experience. In Muller, T (ed.), Philosophie der Zeit: Neue Analytische Ansatze. Frankfurt, Germany: Verlag Vittorio Klostermann.
Grush, R. (2005). Internal Models and the Construction of Time: Generalizing from State Estimation to Trajectory Estimation to Address Temporal Features of Perception, including Temporal Illusions. Journal of Neural Engineering, 2(3), S209–218.
Hazenberg, S. J., Jongsma, M. L. A., Koning, A., and van Lier, R. (2014). Differential Familiarity Effects in Amodal Completion: Support from Behavioral and Electrophysiological Measurements. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 40(2), 669–684.
Hedgé, J., Fang, F., Murray, S. O., and Kersten, D. (2008). Preferential Responses to Occluded Objects in the Human Visual Cortex. Journal of Vision, 8, 16–35.
Hoerl, C. (2009). Time and Tense in Perceptual Experience. Philosopher’s Imprint, 9(12).
James, W. (1890). The Principles of Psychology. In 2 volumes. New York, NY: Henry Holt and Company.
Komatsu, H. (2006). The Neural Mechanisms of Perceptual Filling-in. Nature Review Neuroscience, 7, 220–231.
Kosslyn, S. M., Behrmann, M., and Jeannerod, M. (1995a). The Cognitive Neuroscience of Mental Imagery. Neuropsychologia, 33, 1335–1344.
Kosslyn, S. M., Thompson, W. L., Kim, I. J., and Alpert, N. M. (1995b). Topographical Representations of Mental Images in Primary Visual Cortex. Nature, 378: 496–498.
Kösem, A., Gramfort, A., and van Wassenhove, V. (2014). Encoding of Event Timing in the Phase of Neural Oscillations. NeuroImage, 92, 274–284.
Kovacs, G., Vogels, R., and Orban, G. A. (1995). Selectivity of Macaque Inferior Temporal Neurons for Partially Occluded Shapes. Journal of Neuroscience, 15, 1984–1997.
Larsen, A., Madsen, K. H., Lund, T. E., and Bundesen, C. (2006). Images of Illusory Motion in Primary Visual Cortex. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 18(7), 1174–1180.
Leaver, A. M., van Lare, J., Zielinski, B., Halpern, A. R., and Rauschecker, J. P. (2009). Brain Activation during Anticipation of Sound Sequences. Journal of Neuroscience, 29(8), 2477–2485.
Lee, G. (2014a). Extensionalism, Atomism, and Continuity. In Oaklander, N (ed.), Debates in the Metaphysics of Time. London: Bloomsbury.
Lee, G. (2014b). Temporal Experience and the Temporal Structure of Experience. Philosopher’s Imprint 14(3).
Lee, S. H., Kwan, A. C., Zhang, S., et al. (2012). Activation of Specific Interneurons Improves V1 Feature Selectivity and Visual Perception. Nature, 488, 379–383.
Lee, T. S., and Nguyen, M. (2001). Dynamics of Subjective Contour Formation in the Early Visual Cortex. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences , 98(4), 1907–1911.
Lerner, Y., Harel, M., and Malach, R. (2004). Rapid Completion Effects in Human High-Order Visual Areas. NeuroImage, 21, 516–526.
Muckli, L., Kohler, A., Kriegeskorte, N., and Singer, W. (2005). Primary Visual Cortex Activity Along the Apparent-Motion Trace Reflects Illusory Perception. PLoS Biology, 3(8), 1501–1510.
Nanay, B. (2010). Perception and Imagination: Amodal Perception as Mental Imagery. Philosophical Studies, 150, 239–254.
Nanay, B. (2012). Perceptual Phenomenology. Philosophical Perspectives, 26, 235–246.
Nanay, B. (2018a). Multimodal Mental Imagery. Cortex, 105, 125–134.
Nanay, B. (2018b). The Importance of Amodal Completion in Everyday Perception. I-Perception. https://doi.org/10.1177/2041669518788887
Nanay, B. (Forthcoming). Seeing Things You Don’t See. Oxford, UK: Oxford University Press.
Page, J. W., Duhamel, P., and Crognale, M. A. (2011). ERP Evidence of Visualization at Early Stages of Visual Processing. Brain and Cognition, 75(2), 141–146.
Pan, Y., Chen, M., Yin, J., et al. (2012). Equivalent Representation of Real and Illusory Contours in Macaque V4. Journal of Neuroscience, 32, 6760–6770.
Pearson, J., and Westbrook, F. (2015). Phantom Perception: Voluntary and Involuntary Nonretinal Vision. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 19, 278–284.
Pearson, J., Naselaris, T., Holmes, E. A., and Kosslyn, S. M. (2015). Mental Imagery: Functional Mechanisms and Clinical Applications. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 19, 590–602.
Phillips, B. (2019). The Shifting Boundary between Perception and Cognition. Noûs, 53, 316–346.
Phillips, I. (2010). Perceiving Temporal Properties. European Journal of Philosophy, 18(2), 176–202.
Phillips, I. (2011). Perception and Iconic Memory: What Sperling Doesn’t Show. Mind & Language, 281–311. doi.org/10.1111/j.1468-0017.2011.01422.x.
Phillips, I. (2014). Experience of and in Time. Philosophy Compass, 9, 131–144.
Rauschenberger, R., Liu, T., Slotnick, S. D., and Yantis, S. (2006). Temporally Unfolding Neural Representation of Pictorial Occlusion. Psychological Science, 17, 358–364.
Rees, A., Green, G., and Kay, R. H. (1986). Steady-State Evoked Responses to Sinusoidally Amplitude-Modulated Sounds Recorded in Man. Hearing Research, 23( 2). 123–133.
Regan, D. (1966). Some Characteristics of Average Steady-State and Transient Responses Evoked by Modulated Light. Electroencephalography and Clinical Neurophysiology, 20(3), 238–248.
Rolls, E. T., and Tovee, M. J. (1994). Processing Speed in the Cerebral Cortex and the Neurophysiology of Visual Masking. Proceedings of the Royal Society: Biological Sciences, 257, 9–15.
Scherzer, T. R., and Ekroll, V. (2015). Partial Modal Completion under Occlusion: What do Modal and Amodal Percepts Represent? Journal of Vision, 15, 1–20.
Shepard, R. N. (1978). Mental Images. American Psychologist, 33, 125–137.
Shibata, K., Watanabe, T., Sasaki, Y., and Kawato, M. (2011). Perceptual Learning Incepted by Decoded fMRI Neurofeedback without Stimulus Presentation. Science, 334, 1413–1415.
Shimoji, S. (2014). Postdiction: Its Implications on Visual Awareness, Hindsight, and Sense of Agency. Frontiers in Psychology, 5, 196.
Slotnick, S. D., Thompson, W. L., and Kosslyn, S. M. (2005). Visual Mental Imagery Induces Retinotopically Organized Activation of Early Visual Areas. Cerebral Cortex, 15, 1570–1583.
Smith, F. W., and Muckli, L. (2010). Nonstimulated Early Visual Areas Carry Information About Surrounding Context. PNAS, 107, 20099–20103.
Stekelenburg, J., Sugano, Y., and Vroomen, J. (2011). Neural Correlates of Motor-Sensory Temporal Recalibration. Brain Research, 1397, 46–54.
Sterzer, P., Haynes, J. D., and Rees, G. (2006). Primary Visual Cortex Activation on the Path of Apparent Motion is Mediated by Feedback from hTM + V5. NeuroImage, 32, 1308–1316.
Stetson, C., Cui, X., Montague, P. R., and Eagleman, D. M. (2006). Motor-Sensory Recalibration Leads to an Illusory Reversal of Action and Sensation. Neuron, 51(5), 651–659.
Sugita, Y. (1999). Grouping of Image Fragments in Primary Visual Cortex. Nature, 401, 269–272.
Thorpe, S., Fize, D., and Marlot, C. (1996). Speed of Processing in the Human Visual System. Nature, 381, 520–522.
Viera, G. (2019). The Fragmentary Model of Temporal Experience and the Mirroring Constraint. Philosophical Studies, 176, 21.
Vroomen, J., Keetels, M., de Gelder, B., and Bertelson, P. (2004). Recalibration of Temporal Order Perception by Exposure to Audio–Visual Asynchrony. Cognitive Brain Research, 22, 32–35.
Vroomen, J., and Keetels, M. (2010). Perception of Intersensory Synchrony: A Tutorial Review. Attention, Perception, and Psychophysics, 72, 871–884.
Zatorre, R. J., and Halpern, A. R. (2005). Mental Concert: Musical Imagery and Auditory Cortex. Neuron, 47, 9–12.
References
Arntz, A. (2012). Imagery Rescripting as a Therapeutic Technique: Review of Clinical Trials, Basic Studies, and Research Agenda. Journal of Experimental Psychopathology, 3, 121–126.
Aynsworth, C., Nemat, N., Collerton, D., Smailes, D., and Dudley, R. (2017). Reality Monitoring Performance and the Role of Visual Imagery in Visual Hallucinations. Behaviour Research and Therapy, 97, 115–122.
Baddeley, A. D., and Andrade, J. (2000). Working Memory and the Vividness of Imagery. Journal of Experimental Psychology-General, 129, 126–145.
Barsics, C., van der Linden, M., and D’Argembeau, A. (2016). Frequency, Characteristics, and Perceived Functions of Emotional Future Thinking in Daily Life. The Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 69, 217–233.
Berntsen, D., and Jacobsen, A. S. (2008). Involuntary (Spontaneous) Mental Time Travel into the Past and Future. Consciousness and Cognition, 17, 1093–1104.
Blackwell, S. E. (2019). Mental Imagery: From Basic Research to Clinical Practice. Journal of Psychotherapy Integration, 29(3), 235–247.
Blackwell, S. E., and Holmes, E. A. (2010). Modifying Interpretation and Imagination in Clinical Depression: A Single-Case Series Using Cognitive Bias Modification. Applied Cognitive Psychology, 24, 338–350.
Carrera, P., Caballero, A., and Muñoz, D. (2012). Future-Oriented Emotions in the Prediction of Binge-Drinking Intention and Expectation: The Role of Anticipated and Anticipatory Emotions. Scandinavian Journal of Psychology, 53, 273–279.
Chan, C. K. Y., and Cameron, L. D. (2012). Promoting Physical Activity with Goal-Oriented Mental Imagery: A Randomized Controlled Trial. Journal of Behavioral Medicine, 35, 347–363.
Cole, S. N., Staugaard, S. R., and Berntsen, D. (2016). Inducing Involuntary and Voluntary Mental Time Travel using a Laboratory Paradigm. Memory & Cognition, 44, 376–389.
Conroy, D., and Hagger, M. S. (2018). Imagery Interventions in Health Behavior: A Meta-Analysis. Health Psychology, 37, 668–679.
Conway, M. A., Meares, K., and Standart, S. (2004). Images and Goals. Memory, 12, 525–531.
Conway, M. A., Singer, J. A., and Tagini, A. (2004). The Self and Autobiographical Memory: Correspondence and Coherence. Social Cognition, 22, 491–529.
Cumming, J., and Williams, S. E. (2012). The Role of Imagery in Performance. In Murphy, S. M. (ed.), Handbook of Sport and Performance Psychology. Oxford, UK: Oxford University Press, 213–232.
D’Argembeau, A., Renaud, O., and van der Linden, M. (2011). Frequency, Characteristics and Functions of Future-Oriented Thoughts in Daily Life. Applied Cognitive Psychology, 25, 96–103.
Di Simplicio, M., Renner, F., Blackwell, S. E., et al. (2016). An Investigation of Mental Imagery in Bipolar Disorder: Exploring “the Mind’s Eye”. Bipolar Disorders, 18, 669–683.
Edwards, D. (2007). Restructuring Implicational Meaning through Memory-Based Imagery: Some Historical Notes. Journal of Behavior Therapy and Experimental Psychiatry, 38, 306–316.
Ehlers, A., Clark, D. M., Hackmann, A., McManus, F., and Fennell, M. (2005). Cognitive Therapy for Post-Traumatic Stress Disorder: Development and Evaluation. Behaviour Research and Therapy, 43, 413–431.
Fritzsche, A., Schlier, B., Oettingen, G., and Lincoln, T. M. (2016). Mental Contrasting with Implementation Intentions Increases Goal-Attainment in Individuals with Mild to Moderate Depression. Cognitive Therapy and Research, 40, 557–564.
Golla, F., Hutton, E. L., and Walter, W. G. (1943). The Objective Study of Mental Imagery. Journal of Mental Science, 89, 216–223.
Görgen, S. M., Joormann, J., Hiller, W., and Witthöft, M. (2015). Implicit Affect after Mental Imagery: Introduction of a Novel Measure and Relations to Depressive Symptoms in a Non-Clinical Sample. Journal of Experimental Psychopathology, 6, 1–23.
Gregory, W. L., Cialdini, R. B., and Carpenter, K. M. (1982). Self-Relevant Scenarios as Mediators of Likelihood Estimates and Compliance – Does Imagining Make it So? Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 43, 89–99.
Hales, S. A., Blackwell, S. E., Di Simplicio, M., et al. (2014). Imagery-Based Cognitive Behavioral Assessment. In Brown, G. P. and Clark, D. A. (eds.), Assessment in Cognitive Therapy. New York, NY: Guilford Press.
Hales, S. A., Deeprose, C., Goodwin, G. M., and Holmes, E. A. (2011). Cognitions in Bipolar Disorder versus Unipolar Depression: Imagining Suicide. Bipolar Disorders, 13, 651–661.
Hanrahan, C., and Vergeer, I. (2001). Multiple Uses of Mental Imagery by Professional Modern Dancers. Imagination, Cognition and Personality, 20, 231–255.
Hirsch, C. R., Mathews, A., Clark, D. M., Williams, R., and Morrison, J. (2005). The Causal Role of Negative Imagery in Social Anxiety: A Test in Confident Public Speakers. Journal of Behavior Therapy and Experimental Psychiatry, 37, 159–170.
Hitchcock, C., Mueller, V., Hammond, E., et al. (2016). The Effects of Autobiographical Memory Flexibility (MemFlex) Training: An Uncontrolled Trial in Individuals in Remission from Depression. Journal of Behavior Therapy and Experimental Psychiatry, 52, 92–98.
Hitchcock, C., Werner-Seidler, A., Blackwell, S. E., and Dalgleish, T. (2017). Autobiographical Episodic Memory-Based Training for the Treatment of Mood, Anxiety and Stress-Related Disorders: A Systematic Review and Meta-Analysis. Clinical Psychology Review, 52, 92–107.
Holmes, E. A., Blackwell, S. E., Burnett Heyes, S., Renner, F., and Raes, F. (2016). Mental Imagery in Depression: Phenomenology, Potential Mechanisms, and Treatment Implications. Annual Review of Clinical Psychology, 12. doi:10.1146/annurev-clinpsy-021815-092925.
Holmes, E. A., James, E. L., Kilford, E. J., and Deeprose, C. (2010). Key Steps in Developing a Cognitive Vaccine against Traumatic Flashbacks: Visuospatial Tetris versus Verbal Pub Quiz. PloS One, 5, e13706.
Holmes, E. A., Lang, T. J., and Shah, D. M. (2009). Developing Interpretation Bias Modification as a “Cognitive Vaccine” for Depressed Mood – Imagining Positive Events Makes you Feel Better than Thinking about them Verbally. Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 118, 76–88.
Holmes, E. A., and Mathews, A. (2005). Mental Imagery and Emotion: A Special Relationship? Emotion, 5, 489–497.
Ivins, A., Di Simplicio, M., Close, H., Goodwin, G. M., and Holmes, E. A. (2014). Mental Imagery in Bipolar Affective Disorder versus Unipolar Depression: Investigating Cognitions at times of “Positive” Mood. Journal of Affective Disorders, 166, 234–242.
Iyadurai, L., Blackwell, S. E., Meiser-Stedman, , et al. (2017). Preventing Intrusive Memories after Trauma via a Brief Intervention Involving Tetris Computer Game Play in the Emergency Department: A Proof-of-Concept Randomized Controlled Trial. Molecular Psychiatry, 23(3), 674–682. doi:10.1038/mp.2017.23.
Ji, J. L., Burnett Heyes, S., MacLeod, C., and Holmes, E. A. (2016). Emotional Mental Imagery as Simulation of Reality: Fear and Beyond. A Tribute to Peter Lang. Behavior Therapy, 47, 702–719.
Ji, J. L., Holmes, E. A., and Blackwell, S. E. (2017). Seeing Light at the End of the Tunnel: Positive Prospective Mental Imagery and Optimism in Depression. Psychiatry Research, 247, 155–162.
Kahneman, D., and Tversky, A. (1982). The Simulation Heuristic. In Kahneman, D, Slovic, P, and Tversky, A (eds.), Judgement Under Uncertainty: Heuristics and Biases. Cambridge, UK: Cambridge University Press, 201–208.
Kavanagh, D. J., Andrade, J., and May, J. (2005). Imaginary Relish and Exquisite Torture: The Elaborated Intrusion Theory of Desire. Psychological Review, 112, 446–467.
Kessler, H., Holmes, E. A., Blackwell, S. E., et al. (2018). Reducing Intrusive Memories of Trauma using a Visuospatial Interference Intervention with Inpatients with Post-Traumatic Stress Disorder (PTSD). Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology, 86(12), 1076–1090.
Knäuper, B., McCollam, A., Rosen-Brown, A., et al. (2011). Fruitful Plans: Adding Targeted Mental Imagery to Implementation Intentions Increases Fruit Consumption. Psychology & Health, 26, 601–617.
Korrelboom, K., de Jong, M., Huijbrechts, I., and Daansen, P. (2009). Competitive Memory Training (COMET) for Treating Low Self-Esteem in Patients with Eating Disorders: A Randomized Clinical Trial. Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology, 77, 974–980.
Krans, J., Näring, G., Holmes, E. A., and Becker, E. S. (2009). “I See What You Are Saying”: Intrusive Images from Listening to a Traumatic Verbal Report. Journal of Anxiety Disorders, 24, 134–140.
Lang, P. J. (1979). A Bio-Informational Theory of Emotional Imagery. Psychophysiology, 16, 495–512.
Leer, A., Engelhard, I. M., and van den Hout, M. A. (2014). How Eye Movements in EMDR Work: Changes in Memory Vividness and Emotionality. Journal of Behavior Therapy and Experimental Psychiatry, 45, 396–401.
Libby, L. K., Shaeffer, E. M., Eibach, R. P., and Slemmer, J. A. (2007). Picture Yourself at the Polls – Visual Perspective in Mental Imagery Affects Self-Perception and Behavior. Psychological Science, 18, 199–203.
Linke, J., and Wessa, M. (2017). Mental Imagery Training Increases Wanting of Rewards and Reward Sensitivity and Reduces Depressive Symptoms. Behavior Therapy, 48, 695–706.
MacLeod, A. (2017). Prospection, Well-Being, and Mental Health. Oxford, UK: Oxford University Press.
Mathews, A. (1971). Psychophysiological Approaches to the Investigation of Desensitisation and Related Processes. Psychological Bulletin, 76, 73–91.
Mathews, A., Ridgeway, V., and Holmes, E. A. (2013). Feels like the Real Thing: Imagery is both More Realistic and Emotional than Verbal Thought. Cognition & Emotion, 27, 217–229.
Meevissen, Y. M. C., Peters, M. L., and Alberts, H. J. E. M. (2011). Become more Optimistic by Imagining a Best Possible Self: Effects of a Two-Week Intervention. Journal of Behavior Therapy and Experimental Psychiatry, 42, 371–378.
Nelis, S., Debeer, E., Holmes, E. A., and Raes, F. (2013). Dysphoric Students Show Higher use of the Observer Perspective in their Retrieval of Positive versus Negative Autobiographical Memories. Memory, 21, 423–430.
Oatley, K. (2011). Such Stuff as Dreams: The Psychology of Fiction. Oxford, UK: Wiley-Blackwell.
Pearson, J. (2014). New Directions in Mental-Imagery Research: The Binocular-Rivalry Technique and Decoding fMRI Patterns. Current Directions in Psychological Science, 23, 178–183.
Pearson, J., Naselaris, T., Holmes, E. A., and Kosslyn, S. M. (2015). Mental Imagery: Functional Mechanisms and Clinical Applications. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 19, 590–602.
Pictet, A., Jermann, F., and Ceschi, G. (2016). When Less Could Be More: Investigating the Effects of a Brief Internet-Based Imagery Cognitive Bias Modification Intervention in Depression. Behaviour Research and Therapy, 84, 45–51.
Pratt, D., Cooper, M. J., and Hackmann, A. (2004). Imagery and Its Characteristics in People Who Are Anxious about Spiders. Behavioural & Cognitive Psychotherapy, 32, 165–176.
Raes, F., Williams, J. G., and Hermans, D. (2009). Reducing Cognitive Vulnerability to Depression: A Preliminary Investigation of Memory Specificity Training (MEST) in Inpatients with Depressive Symptomatology. Journal of Behavior Therapy and Experimental Psychiatry, 40, 24–38.
Renner, F., Ji, J. L., Pictet, A., Holmes, E. A., and Blackwell, S. E. (2017). Effects of Engaging in Repeated Mental Imagery of Future Positive Events on Behavioural Activation in Individuals with Major Depressive Disorder. Cognitive Therapy and Research, 41, 369–380.
Reynolds, M., and Brewin, C. R. (1998). Intrusive Cognitions, Coping Strategies and Emotional Responses in Depression, Post-Traumatic Stress Disorder and a Non-Clinical Population. Behaviour Research and Therapy, 36, 135–147.
Schacter, D. L., Addis, D. R., and Buckner, R. L. (2008). Episodic Simulation of Future Events: Concepts, Data, and Applications. New York Academy of Sciences, 1124, 39–60.
Stopa, L. (2009). Imagery and the Threatened Self: Perspectives on Mental Imagery and the Self in Cognitive Therapy. London, UK: Routledge.
Weßlau, C., Cloos, M., Höfling, V., and Steil, R. (2015). Visual Mental Imagery and Symptoms of Depression: Results from a Large-Scale Web-Based Study. BMC Psychiatry, 15, 308.
Wolpe, J. (1961). The Systematic Desensitization Treatment of Neurosis. Journal of Nervous and Mental Disease, 132, 189–203.
Zeman, A., Dewar, M., and Della Sala, S. (2015). Lives without Imagery – Congenital Aphantasia. Cortex, 73, 378–380.
References
Alais, D., and Burr, D. (2004). The Ventriloquist Effect Results from Near-Optimal Bimodal Integration. Current Biology: CB, 14(3), 257–262.
Albright, T. D. (2012). On the Perception of Probable Things: Neural Substrates of Associative Memory, Imagery, and Perception. Neuron, 74(2), 227–245.
Allen, A. K., Wilkins, K., Gazzaley, A., and Morsella, E. (2013). Conscious Thoughts from Reflex-Like Processes: A New Experimental Paradigm for Consciousness Research. Consciousness and Cognition, 22(4), 1318–1331.
Andersen, T. S., Tiippana, K., and Sams, M. (2004). Factors Influencing Audiovisual Fission and Fusion Illusions. Cognitive Brain Research, 21(3), 301–308.
Barraclough, N. E., Xiao, D., Baker, C. I., Oram, M. W., and Perrett, D. I. (2005). Integration of Visual and Auditory Information by Superior Temporal Sulcus Neurons Responsive to the Sight of Actions. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 17(3), 377–391.
Beauchamp, M. S., Argall, B. D., Bodurka, J., Duyn, J. H., and Martin, A. (2004a). Unraveling Multisensory Integration: Patchy Organization within Human STS Multisensory Cortex. Nature Neuroscience, 7(11), 1190–1192.
Beauchamp, M. S., Lee, K. E., Argall, B. D., and Martin, A. (2004b). Integration of Auditory and Visual Information about Objects in Superior Temporal Sulcus. Neuron, 41(5), 809–823. www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pubmed/15813999.
Beauchamp, M. S., Nath, A. R., and Pasalar, S. (2010). fMRI-Guided Transcranial Magnetic Stimulation Reveals that the Superior Temporal Sulcus is a Cortical Locus of the McGurk Effect. Journal of Neuroscience, 30(7), 2414–2417.
Berger, C. C., and Ehrsson, H. H. (2013). Mental Imagery Changes Multisensory Perception. Current Biology, 23, 1367–1372.
Berger, C. C., and Ehrsson, H. H. (2014). The Fusion of Mental Imagery and Sensation in the Temporal Association Cortex. Journal of Neuroscience, 34(41), 13684–13692.
Berger, C. C., and Ehrsson, H. H. (2017). The Content of Imagined Sounds Changes Visual Motion Perception in the Cross-Bounce Illusion. Scientific Reports, 7, 40123.
Berger, C. C., and Ehrsson, H. H. (2018). Mental Imagery Induces Crossmodal Sensory Plasticity and Changes Future Auditory Perception. Psychological Science, 29(6), 926–935.
Bischoff, M., Walter, B., Blecker, C. R., et al. (2007). Utilizing the Ventriloquism-Effect to Investigate Audiovisual Binding. Neuropsychologia, 45(3), 578–586.
Bonath, B., Noesselt, T., Martinez, A., et al. (2007). Neural Basis of the Ventriloquist Illusion. Current Biology: CB, 17(19), 1697–1703.
Bruce, C., Desimone, R., and Gross, C. G. (1981). Visual Properties of Neurons in a Polysensory Area in Superior Temporal Sulcus of the Macaque. Journal of Neurophysiology, 46(2), 369–384. www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pubmed/6267219.
Calvert, G. A., Campbell, R., and Brammer, M. J. (2000). Evidence from Functional Magnetic Resonance Imaging of Crossmodal Binding in the Human Heteromodal Cortex. Current Biology: CB, 10(11), 649–657. www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pubmed/10837246.
Cichy, R. M., Heinzle, J., and Haynes, J.-D. (2011). Imagery and Perception Share Cortical Representations of Content and Location. Cerebral Cortex, 22(2), 372–380.
Craver-Lemley, C., and Reeves, A. (1987). Visual Imagery Selectively Reduces Vernier Acuity. Perception, 16(5), 599–614.
Dils, A. T., and Boroditsky, L. (2010). Visual Motion Aftereffect from Understanding Motion Language. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America, 107, 16396–16400.
Driver, J., and Noesselt, T. (2008). Multisensory Interplay Reveals Crossmodal Influences on “Sensory-Specific” Brain Regions, Neural Responses, and Judgments. Neuron, 57(1), 11–23.
Farah, M. J. (1985). Psychophysical Evidence for a Shared Representational Medium for Mental Images and Percepts. Journal of Experimental Psychology. General, 114(1), 91–103. www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pubmed/3156947.
Farah, M. J. (1989a). Mechanisms of Imagery-Perception Interaction. Journal of Experimental Psychology. Human Perception and Performance, 15(2), 203–211. www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pubmed/2525596.
Farah, M. J. (1989b). The Neural Basis of Mental Imagery. Trends in Neurosciences, 12(10), 395–399. www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pubmed/8137002.
Frissen, I., Vroomen, J., and de Gelder, B. (2012). The Aftereffects of Ventriloquism: The Time Course of the Visual Recalibration of Auditory Localization. Seeing and Perceiving, 25(1), 1–14.
Frissen, I., Vroomen, J., de Gelder, B., and Bertelson, P. (2005). The Aftereffects of Ventriloquism: Generalization Across Sound-Frequencies. Acta Psychologica, 118(1–2), 93–100.
Ghazanfar, A. A., Chandrasekaran, C., and Logothetis, N. K. (2008). Interactions Between the Superior Temporal Sulcus and Auditory Cortex Mediate Dynamic Face/Voice Integration in Rhesus Monkeys. Journal of Neuroscience, 28(17), 4457–4469.
Ghazanfar, A. A., and Schroeder, C. E. (2006). Is Neocortex Essentially Multisensory? Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 10(6), 278–285.
Grassi, M., and Casco, C. (2009). Audiovisual Bounce-Inducing Effect: Attention Alone Does Not Explain Why the Discs Are Bouncing. Journal of Experimental Psychology. Human Perception and Performance, 35(1), 235–243.
Grassi, M., and Casco, C. (2012). Revealing the Origin of the Audiovisual Bounce-Inducing Effect. Seeing and Perceiving, 25(2), 223–233.
Halpern, A. R. (1988). Mental Scanning in Auditory Imagery for Songs. Journal of Experimental Psychology. Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 14(3), 434–443. www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pubmed/2969942.
Howard, I. P., and Templeton, W. B. (1966). Human Spatial Orientation. London, UK: Wiley.
Hubbard, T. L. (2010). Auditory Imagery: Empirical Findings. Psychological Bulletin, 136(2), 302–329.
Johns, L. C., Rossell, S., Frith, C., et al. (2001). Verbal Self-Monitoring and Auditory Verbal Hallucinations in Patients with Schizophrenia. Psychological Medicine, 31, 705–715.
Kayser, C., and Logothetis, N. K. (2009). Directed Interactions between Auditory and Superior Temporal Cortices and their Role in Sensory Integration. Frontiers in Integrative Neuroscience, 3(May), 1–11.
Kosslyn, S. M. (1973). Scanning Visual Images: Some Structural Implications. Perception & Psychophysics, 14(1), 90–94.
Kosslyn, S. M. (1994). Image and Brain: The Resolution of the Imagery Debate. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.
Kosslyn, S. M., Ball, T. M., and Reiser, B. J. (1978). Visual Images Preserve Metric Spatial Information: Evidence from Studies of Image Scanning. Journal of Experimental Psychology. Human Perception and Performance, 4(1), 47–60. www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pubmed/627850.
Kosslyn, S. M., Ganis, G., and Thompson, W. L. (2001). Neural Foundations of Imagery. Nature Reviews. Neuroscience, 2(9), 635–642.
Lewald, J. (2002). Rapid Adaptation to Auditory-Visual Spatial Disparity. Learning & Memory, 9(5), 268–278.
Macmillan, N. A., and Kaplan, H. L. (1985). Detection Theory Analysis of Group Data: Estimating Sensitivity from Average Hit and False-Alarm Rates. Psychological Bulletin, 98(1), 185–199.
Magnotti, J. F., Basu Mallick, D., Feng, G., et al. (2015). Similar Frequency of the McGurk Effect in Large Samples of Native Mandarin Chinese and American English Speakers. Experimental Brain Research, 233(9), 2581–2586.
Marchant, J. L., Ruff, C. C., and Driver, J. (2012). Audiovisual Synchrony Enhances BOLD Responses in a Brain Network Including ultisensory STS While Also Enhancing Target-Detection Performance for Both Modalities. Human Brain Mapping, 33(5), 1212–1224.
Mast, F. W., Berthoz, A., and Kosslyn, S. M. (2001). Mental Imagery of Visual Motion Modifies the Perception of Roll-Vection Stimulation. Perception, 30(8), 945–957.
McGurk, H., and MacDonald, J. (1976). Hearing Lips and Seeing Voices. Nature, 264(23), 746–748. www.nature.com/nature/journal/v264/n5588/abs/264746a0.html.
Nath, A. R., and Beauchamp, M. S. (2011). Dynamic Changes in Superior Temporal Sulcus Connectivity during Perception of Noisy Audiovisual Speech. Journal of Neuroscience, 31(5), 1704–1714.
Noesselt, T., Rieger, J. W., Schoenfeld, M. A., et al. (2007). Audiovisual Temporal Correspondence Modulates Human Multisensory Superior Temporal Sulcus plus Primary Sensory Cortices. Journal of Neuroscience, 27(42), 11431–11441.
Noppeney, U., Josephs, O., Hocking, J., Price, C. J., and Friston, K. J. (2008). The Effect of Prior Visual Information on Recognition of Speech and Sounds. Cerebral Cortex, 18(3), 598–609.
O’Craven, K. M., and Kanwisher, N. (2000). Mental Imagery of Faces and Places Activates Corresponding Stimulus-Specific Brain Regions. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 12(6), 1013–1023. www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pubmed/11177421.
Pearson, J., Clifford, C. W. G., and Tong, F. (2008). The Functional Impact of Mental Imagery on Conscious Perception. Current Biology, 18(13), 982–986.
Perky, C. W. (1910). An Experimental Study of Imagination. American Journal of Psychology, 21(3), 422–452.
Perrodin, C., Kayser, C., Logothetis, N. K., and Petkov, C. I. (2014). Auditory and Visual Modulation of Temporal Lobe Neurons in Voice-Sensitive and Association Cortices. Journal of Neuroscience, 34(7), 2524–2537.
Plaze, M., Paillère-Martinot, M.-L., Penttilä, J., et al. (2011). “Where do Auditory Hallucinations Come From?”A Brain Morphometry Study of Schizophrenia Patients with Inner or Outer Space Hallucinations. Schizophrenia Bulletin, 37(1), 212–221.
Pylyshyn, Z. W. (1973). What the Mind’s Eye Tells the Mind’s Brain: A Critique of Mental Imagery. Psychological Bulletin, 80(1), 1–12.
Pylyshyn, Z. W. (2002). Mental Imagery: In Search of a Theory. The Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 25, 157–182.
Recanzone, G. H. (1998). Rapidly Induced Auditory Plasticity: The Ventriloquism Aftereffect. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America, 95(February), 869–875.
Schlack, A., and Albright, T. D. (2007). Remembering Visual Motion: Neural Correlates of Associative Plasticity and Motion Recall in Cortical Area MT. Neuron, 53(6), 881–890.
Segal, S. J., and Fusella, V. (1970). Influence of Imaged Pictures and Sounds on Detection of Visual and Auditory Signals. Journal of Experimental Psychology, 83(3), 458–464. www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pubmed/5480913.
Sekuler, R., Sekuler, A. B., and Lau, R. (1997). Sound Alters Visual Motion Perception. Nature, 385(6614), 308. www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pubmed/9002513.
Seltzer, B., and Pandya, D. N. (1994). Parietal, Temporal, and Occipital Projections to Cortex of the Superior Temporal Sulcus in the Rhesus Monkey: A Retrograde Tracer Study. The Journal of Comparative Neurology, 343(3), 445–463.
Shams, L., Kamitani, Y., and Shimojo, S. (2000). What You See Is What You Hear. Nature, 408(6814), 788. doi:10.1038/35048669.
Shimojo, S., and Shams, L. (2001). Sensory Modalities Are Not Separate Modalities: Plasticity and Interactions. Current Opinion in Neurobiology, 11(4), 505–509.
Spence, C., and Deroy, O. (2012). Hearing Mouth Shapes: Sound Symbolism and the Reverse McGurk Effect. I-Perception, 3(8), 550–552.
Stein, B. E., and Stanford, T. R. (2008). Multisensory Integration: Current Issues from the Perspective of the Single Neuron. Nature Reviews. Neuroscience, 9(4), 255–266.
Stevenson, R. A., and James, T. W. (2009). Audiovisual Integration in Human Superior Temporal Sulcus: Inverse Effectiveness and the Neural Processing of Speech and Object Recognition. NeuroImage, 44(3), 1210–1223.
Sweeny, T. D., Guzman-Martinez, E., Ortega, L., Grabowecky, M., and Suzuki, S. (2012). Sounds Exaggerate Visual Shape. Cognition, 124(2), 194–200.
Szycik, G. R., Stadler, J., Tempelmann, C., and Münte, T. F. (2012). Examining the McGurk Illusion Using High-Field 7 Tesla Functional MRI. Frontiers in Human Neuroscience, 6(April), 95.
van Wassenhove, V., Grant, K. W., and Poeppel, D. (2007). Temporal Window of Integration in Auditory-Visual Speech Perception. Neuropsychologia, 45(3), 598–607.
Wallace, M. T., Roberson, G. E., Hairston, W. D., et al. (2004). Unifying Multisensory Signals Across Time and Space. Experimental Brain Research. Experimentelle Hirnforschung. Expérimentation Cérébrale, 158(2), 252–258.
Weber, R. J., and Castleman, J. (1970). The Time It Takes to Imagine. Perception & Psychophysics, 8(3), 165–168.
Wegner, D. M. (1994). Ironic Processes of Mental Control. Psychological Review, 101(1), 34–52.
Werner, S., and Noppeney, U. (2010a). Distinct Functional Contributions of Primary Sensory and Association Areas to Audiovisual Integration in Object Categorization. Journal of Neuroscience, 30(7), 2662–2675.
Werner, S., and Noppeney, U. (2010b). Superadditive Responses in Superior Temporal Sulcus Predict Audiovisual Benefits in Object Categorization. Cerebral Cortex, 20(8), 1829–1842.
Winawer, J., Huk, A. C., and Boroditsky, L. (2010). A Motion Aftereffect from Visual Imagery of Motion. Cognition, 114(2), 276–284.
Woods, T. M., and Recanzone, G. H. (2004). Visually Induced Plasticity of Auditory Spatial Perception in Macaques. Current Biology, 14, 1559–1564.
Wozny, D. R., and Shams, L. (2011). Recalibration of Auditory Space Following Milliseconds of Cross-Modal Discrepancy. Journal of Neuroscience, 31(12), 4607–4612.
References
Abramson, M., and Goldinger, S. D. (1997). What the Reader’s Eye Tells the Mind’s Ear: Silent Reading Activates Inner Speech. Perception and Psychophysics, 59, 1059–1068.
Banissy, M. J., and Ward, J. (2007). Mirror-Touch Synesthesia Is Linked with Empathy. Nature Neuroscience, 10, 815–816.
Bensafi, M., and Rouby, C. (2007). Individual Differences in Odor Imaging Ability Reflected Differences in Olfactory and Emotional Perception. Chemical Senses, 32, 237–244.
Bernstein, L. E., Jiang, J., Pantazis, D., Lu, Z. L., and Joshi, A. (2011). Visual Phonetic Processing Localized Using Speech and Nonspeech Face Gestures in Video and Point-Light Displays. Human Brain Mapping, 32, 1660–1676.
Bernstein, L. E., and Liebenthal, E. (2014). Neural Pathways for Visual Speech Perception. Frontiers in Neuroscience, 8, 386.
Block, N. (2011). Perceptual Consciousness Overflows Cognitive Access. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 15, 567–575.
Budd, M. (2012). The Musical Expression of Emotion: Metaphorical-as versus Imaginative-as Perception. Estetika: The Central European Journal of Aesthetics, 49(2), 131–148.
Calvert, G. A., Bullmore, E. T., Brammer, M. J., et al. (1997). Activation of Auditory Cortex during Silent Lipreading. Science, 276(5312), 593–596.
Cappe, C., Rouiller, E. M., and Barone, P. (2009). Multisensory Anatomical Pathways. Hearing Research, 258(1–2), 28–36.
Caron-Desrochers, L., Schönwiesner, M., Focke, K., and Lehmann, A. (2018). Assessing Visual Modulation along the Human Subcortical Auditory Pathway. Neuroscience Letters, 685, 12–17.
Deroy, O., Fernandez-Prieto, I., Navarra, J., and Spence, C. (2018). Unravelling the Paradox of Spatial Pitch. In Hubbard, T. L. (ed.), Spatial Biases in Perception and Cognition. Cambridge, UK: Cambridge University Press.
Deroy, O., and Spence, C. (2016). Crossmodal Correspondences: Four Challenges. Multisensory Research, 29(1–3), 29–48.
De Vignemont, F. (2016). Mirror-Touch Synaesthesia. In Deroy, O (ed.), Sensory Blending: On Synaesthesia and Related Phenomena. Oxford, UK: Oxford University Press, 275–292.
Fox, K. C., and Christoff, K. (eds.), (2018). The Oxford Handbook of Spontaneous Thought: Mind Wandering, Creativity, and Dreaming. Oxford, UK: Oxford University Press.
Halpern, A. R. (1992). Musical Aspects of Auditory Imagery. In Reisberg, D (ed.), Auditory Imagery. Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.
Hertrich, I., Dietrich, S., and Ackermann, H. (2011). Cross-Modal Interactions during Perception of Audiovisual Speech and Nonspeech Signals: An fMRI Study. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 23(1), 221–237.
Hickok, G. (2014). The Architecture of Speech Production and the Role of the Phoneme in Speech Processing. Language, Cognition and Neuroscience, 29(1), 2–20.
Hubbard, T. L. (2010). Auditory Imagery: Empirical Findings. Psychological Bulletin, 136(2), 302.
Keeley, B. L. (2002). Making Sense of the Senses: Individuating Modalities in Humans and Other Animals. The Journal of Philosophy, 99(1), 5–28.
Lakoff, G., and Johnson, M. (1980). Metaphors We Live By. Chicago, IL: University of Chicago Press.
MacDougal, R. (1898). Music Imagery: A Confession of Experience. Psychological Review, 5(5), 463.
Nanay, B. (2017). Multimodal Mental Imagery. Cortex, 105, 125–134.
Parise, C. V. (2016). Crossmodal Correspondences: Standing Issues and Experimental Guidelines. Multisensory Research, 29(1–3), 7–28.
Peacocke, C. (2009). Experiencing Metaphorically-as in Music Perception: Clarifications and Commitments. The British Journal of Aesthetics, 49(3), 299–306.
Pekkola, J., Ojanen, V., Autti, T., et al. (2005). Primary Auditory Cortex Activation by Visual Speech: An fMRI Study at 3 T. Neuroreport, 16(2), 125–128.
Riedel, P., Ragert, P., Schelinski, S., Kiebel, S. J., and von Kriegstein, K. (2015). Visual Face-Movement Sensitive Cortex Is Relevant for Auditory-only Speech Recognition. Cortex, 68, 86–99.
Royet, J. P., Koenig, O., Gregoire, M. C., et al. (1999). Functional Anatomy of Perceptual and Semantic Processing for Odors. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 11(1), 94–109. PubMed PMID: 9950717.
Sadaghiani, S., Maier, J. X., and Noppeney, U. (2009). Natural, Metaphoric, and Linguistic Auditory Direction Signals Have Distinct Influences on Visual Motion Processing. Journal of Neuroscience, 29(20), 6490–6499.
Schwitzgebel, E. (2016). Phenomenal Consciousness, Defined and Defended as Innocently as I Can Manage. Journal of Consciousness Studies, 23(11–12), 224–235.
Serino, A., Pizzoferrato, F., and Ladavas, E. (2008). Viewing a Face (Especially One’s Own Face) Being Touched Enhances Tactile Perception on the Face. Psychological Science, 19(5), 434–438.
Shepard, R. N. (1978). The Mental Image. American Psychologist, 33(2), 125–137.
Shepard, R. N., and Cooper, L. A. (1982). Mental Images and their Transformations. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.
Shepard, R. N., and Metzler, J. (1971). Mental Rotation of Three-Dimensional Objects. Science, 171, 701–703.
Spence, C. (2011). Crossmodal Correspondences: A Tutorial Review. Attention, Perception, and Psychophysics, 73(4), 971–995.
Spence, C., and Deroy, O. (2013a). Crossmodal Mental Imagery. In Lacey, S and Lawson, R (eds.), Multisensory Imagery. New York, NY: Springer, 157–183.
Spence, C., and Deroy, O. (2013b). How Automatic are Crossmodal Correspondences? Consciousness and Cognition, 22(1), 245–260.
Tyll, S., Budinger, E., and Noesselt, T. (2011). Thalamic Influences on Multisensory Integration. Communicative & Integrative Biology, 4, 378–381.
van den Brink, R. L., Cohen, M. X., van der Burg, E., et al. (2013). Subcortical, Modality-Specific Pathways Contribute to Multisensory Processing in Humans. Cerebral Cortex, 24, 2169–2177.