Skip to main content Accessibility help
×
Home
Hostname: page-component-99c86f546-4hcbs Total loading time: 1.231 Render date: 2021-12-06T20:36:30.543Z Has data issue: true Feature Flags: { "shouldUseShareProductTool": true, "shouldUseHypothesis": true, "isUnsiloEnabled": true, "metricsAbstractViews": false, "figures": true, "newCiteModal": false, "newCitedByModal": true, "newEcommerce": true, "newUsageEvents": true }

Part Four - Semantics, pragmatics and discourse

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  05 November 2015

Edith L. Bavin
Affiliation:
La Trobe University, Victoria
Letitia R. Naigles
Affiliation:
University of Connecticut
Get access
Type
Chapter
Information
Publisher: Cambridge University Press
Print publication year: 2015

Access options

Get access to the full version of this content by using one of the access options below. (Log in options will check for institutional or personal access. Content may require purchase if you do not have access.)

References

Bloom, P. (2000). How Children Learn the Meanings of Words. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.Google Scholar
Bowerman, M., & Levinson, S. C. (eds.) (2001). Language Acquisition and Conceptual Development. Cambridge University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Clark, E. V. (1993). The Lexicon in Acquisition. Cambridge University Press. (2009). First Language Acquisition, 2nd edn. Cambridge University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Gentner, D., & Goldin-Meadow, S. (eds.) (2003). Language in Mind. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.Google Scholar
Akhtar, N. (2005). The robustness of learning through overhearing. Developmental Science, 8, 199209.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Berman, R. A. (2014). Cross-linguistic comparisons in child language research. Journal of Child Language, 41, 2637.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Berman, J. M., Graham, S. A., Callaway, D., & Chambers, C. G. (2013). Preschoolers use emotion in speech to learn new words. Child Development, 84, 17911805.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Bion, R., Borovsky, A., & Fernald, A. (2013). Fast mapping, slow learning: Disambiguation of novel word-object mappings in relation to vocabulary learning at 18, 24, and 30 months. Cognition, 126, 3953.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Waxman, S., Fu, X., Arunachalam, S., Leddon, E., Geraghty, K., & Song, H. (2013). Are nouns learned before verbs? Infants provide insight into a long-standing debate. Child Development Perspectives, 7, 155–9.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Chierchia, G., & McConnell-Ginet, S. (2000). Meaning and Grammar: An Introduction to Semantics. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.Google Scholar
Crain, S., & Thornton, R. (1998). Investigations in Universal Grammar: A Guide to Experiments in the Acquisition of Syntax and Semantics. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.Google Scholar
Guasti, M. T. (2002). Language Acquisition: The Growth of Grammar. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.Google Scholar
Huang, Y. T., & Snedeker, J. (2011). Cascading activation across levels of representation in children’s lexical processing. Journal of Child Language, 38(3), 644–61.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Snedeker, J., & Yuan, S. (2008). The role of prosodic and lexical constraints in parsing in young children (and adults). Journal of Memory and Language, 58, 574608.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Trueswell, J. C., & Gleitman, L. R. (2007). Learning to parse and its implications for language acquisition. In Gaskell, G. (ed.), The Oxford Handbook of Psycholing. Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Van Gompel, R. P. (ed.) (2013). Sentence Processing. Hove: Psychology Press.Google ScholarPubMed
Blum-Kulka, S., & Snow, C. (eds.). (2002). Talking to Adults: The Contribution of Multi-party Discourse to Language Acquisition. Mahwah, NJ: Erlbaum.Google Scholar
Ninio, A., & Snow, C. (1996). Pragmatic Development. Boulder, CO: Westview Press.Google Scholar
Piaget, J. (1926/1974). The Language and Thought of the Child, trans. Gabains, M.. New York: New American Library.Google Scholar
Subrahmanyam, K., & Šmahel, D. (2011). Digital Youth: The Role of Media in Development. New York: Springer.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Berman, R. A. (2008). The psycholinguistics of developing text construction. Journal of Child Language, 35, 735–71.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Berman, R. A., & Slobin, D. I. (1994). Relating Events in Narrative: A Crosslinguistic Developmental Study. Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.Google Scholar
Hickmann, M. (2003). Children’s Discourse: Person, Time, and Space across Languages. Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
McCabe, A., & Peterson, C. (eds.). (1991). Developing Narrative Structure. Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.Google Scholar
Strömqvist, S., & Verhoeven, L. (eds.). (2004). Relating Events in Narrative: Typological and Contextual Perspectives. Mahwah, NJ: Erlbaum.Google Scholar

Send book to Kindle

To send this book to your Kindle, first ensure no-reply@cambridge.org is added to your Approved Personal Document E-mail List under your Personal Document Settings on the Manage Your Content and Devices page of your Amazon account. Then enter the ‘name’ part of your Kindle email address below. Find out more about sending to your Kindle.

Note you can select to send to either the @free.kindle.com or @kindle.com variations. ‘@free.kindle.com’ emails are free but can only be sent to your device when it is connected to wi-fi. ‘@kindle.com’ emails can be delivered even when you are not connected to wi-fi, but note that service fees apply.

Find out more about the Kindle Personal Document Service.

Available formats
×

Send book to Dropbox

To send content items to your account, please confirm that you agree to abide by our usage policies. If this is the first time you use this feature, you will be asked to authorise Cambridge Core to connect with your account. Find out more about sending content to Dropbox.

Available formats
×

Send book to Google Drive

To send content items to your account, please confirm that you agree to abide by our usage policies. If this is the first time you use this feature, you will be asked to authorise Cambridge Core to connect with your account. Find out more about sending content to Google Drive.

Available formats
×