Hostname: page-component-8448b6f56d-mp689 Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-04-24T05:33:55.026Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

Trust Nobody: How Voters React to Conspiracy Theories

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  16 June 2022

Giovanna M. Invernizzi*
Affiliation:
Collegio Carlo Alberto, Turin, Italy
Ahmed Ezzeldin Mohamed
Affiliation:
Department of Political Science, Columbia University, New York, USA
*
*Corresponding author. Email: giovanna.invernizzi@carloalberto.org

Abstract

How does exposure to conspiracy theories affects voters’ political attitudes? Using an online experiment among US subjects, we show that exposure to conspiracy theories decreases voters’ trust in the domestic informational environment. Subjects were exposed to conspiracy theories that are entirely unrelated to American domestic politics, which further underscores such narratives’ danger. However, we fail to reject the null hypothesis that voters do not weigh unrelated conspiracies in their evaluation of politicians’ performance and domestic political institutions. Overall, our findings illustrate that an informational environment permeated by conspiracy theories could impede the functioning of democracy by eroding trust in information providers and undermining the credibility of political information.

Type
Research Article
Copyright
© The Author(s), 2022. Published by Cambridge University Press on behalf of The Experimental Research Section of the American Political Science Association

Access options

Get access to the full version of this content by using one of the access options below. (Log in options will check for institutional or personal access. Content may require purchase if you do not have access.)

Footnotes

This article has earned badges for transparent research practices: Open Data and Open Materials. For details see the Data Availability Statement.

References

Bräuninger, Thomas and Marinov, Nikolay. 2022. Political Elites and the “War on Truth”. Journal of Public Economics 206: 104585.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Butler, Lisa D., Koopman, Cheryl and Zimbardo, Philip G.. 1995. The Psychological Impact of Viewing the Film “JFK”: Emotions, Beliefs, and Political Behavioral Intentions. Political Psychology 16(2): 237–57.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Einstein, Katherine Levine and Glick, David M. 2015. Do I Think BLS Data Are BS? The Consequences of Conspiracy Theories. Political Behavior 37(3): 679701.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Goertzel, Ted. 1994. Belief in Conspiracy Theories. Political Psychology 15(4): 731–42.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Invernizzi, Giovanna Maria and Mohamed, Ahmed Ezzeldin. 2022. Replication Data for: Trust Nobody: How Voters React to Conspiracy Theories. Harvard Dataverse, V1. doi: 10.7910/DVN/OSEXBV.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Kim, Minchul and Cao, Xiaoxia. 2016. The Impact of Exposure to Media Messages Promoting Government Conspiracy Theories on Distrust in the Government: Evidence from a Two-Stage Randomized Experiment. International Journal of Communication 10: 20.Google Scholar
Schuirmann, Donald J. 1987. A Comparison of the Two One-Sided Tests Procedure and the Power Approach for Assessing the Equivalence of Average Bioavailability. Journal of Pharmacokinetics and Biopharmaceutics 15(6): 657–80.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Uscinski, Joseph E. and Parent, Joseph M.. 2014. American Conspiracy Theories. Oxford: Oxford University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Supplementary material: Link

Invernizzi and Mohamed Dataset

Link
Supplementary material: PDF

Invernizzi and Mohamed supplementary material

Invernizzi and Mohamed supplementary material

Download Invernizzi and Mohamed supplementary material(PDF)
PDF 332.3 KB