We use cookies to distinguish you from other users and to provide you with a better experience on our websites. Close this message to accept cookies or find out how to manage your cookie settings.
To save content items to your account,
please confirm that you agree to abide by our usage policies.
If this is the first time you use this feature, you will be asked to authorise Cambridge Core to connect with your account.
Find out more about saving content to .
To save content items to your Kindle, first ensure coreplatform@cambridge.org
is added to your Approved Personal Document E-mail List under your Personal Document Settings
on the Manage Your Content and Devices page of your Amazon account. Then enter the ‘name’ part
of your Kindle email address below.
Find out more about saving to your Kindle.
Note you can select to save to either the @free.kindle.com or @kindle.com variations.
‘@free.kindle.com’ emails are free but can only be saved to your device when it is connected to wi-fi.
‘@kindle.com’ emails can be delivered even when you are not connected to wi-fi, but note that service fees apply.
It is more pleasant to present great ideas than to engage in painstaking collection of data.
Tjeerd van Andel
Introduction
There is something captivating about large groups of animals moving swiftly and synchronously. A flock of pigeons wheeling over a city park or a silvery cloud of anchovies at the Monterey Bay Aquarium is fascinating and beautiful to people far removed from those of us who study and think critically about animal aggregations and schools. Although scientists have been interested in animal group dynamics for many years, these phenomena have proved difficult to manage quantitatively. Until recently, much that was written about school structure and function was of necessity based on the analysis of very small groups or upon rather subjective or rational arguments.
A pertinent example of the latter is Hamilton's 1971 model for animal aggregation, the “Selfish Herd.” This results from a progression of logic that predicts that individuals will seek the interior of a group because they are safer there than they would be at the edge. Given his assumptions, the frogs in Hamilton's thought experiment hop into groups and thereby improve their odds. At least one of those frogs will lose due to its lack of nearby neighbors. The “selfish herd” has rightly been an important part of thinking about predators and schooling prey for two decades.
But models must be tested against nature and the naturalists among us know that there's more to it than jockeying for the inside track. Among the city pigeons, recognizable individuals change relative positions frequently and move throughout the flock.
Recommend this
Email your librarian or administrator to recommend adding this to your organisation's collection.