Hostname: page-component-78c5997874-94fs2 Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-11-18T21:47:13.726Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

Coronavirus conspiracy suspicions, general vaccine attitudes, trust and coronavirus information source as predictors of vaccine hesitancy among UK residents during the COVID-19 pandemic

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  12 April 2021

Daniel Allington*
Affiliation:
Digital Humanities, King's College London, London WC2R 2LS, UK
Siobhan McAndrew
Affiliation:
School of Sociology, Politics, and International Studies, University of Bristol, Bristol BS8 1TU
Vivienne Moxham-Hall
Affiliation:
Policy Institute, King's College London, London WC2R 2LS
Bobby Duffy
Affiliation:
Policy Institute, King's College London, London WC2R 2LS
*
Author for correspondence: Daniel Allington, E-mail: daniel.allington@kcl.ac.uk
Rights & Permissions [Opens in a new window]

Abstract

Background

Vaccine hesitancy presents an obstacle to the campaign to control COVID-19. It has previously been found to be associated with youth, female gender, low income, low education, low medical trust, minority ethnic group membership, low perceived risk from COVID-19, use of certain social media platforms and conspiracy beliefs. However, it is unclear which of these predictors might explain variance associated with others.

Methods

An online survey was conducted with a representative sample of 4343 UK residents, aged 18–75, between 21 November and 21 December 2020. Predictors of vaccine hesitancy were assessed using linear rank-order models.

Results

Coronavirus vaccine hesitancy is associated with youth, female gender, low income, low education, high informational reliance on social media, low informational reliance on print and broadcast media, membership of other than white ethnic groups, low perceived risk from COVID-19 and low trust in scientists and medics, as well as (to a much lesser extent) low trust in government. Coronavirus conspiracy suspicions and general vaccine attitudes appear uniquely predictive, jointly explaining 35% of variance. Following controls for these variables, effects associated with trust, ethnicity and social media reliance largely or completely disappear, whereas the effect associated with education is reversed.

Conclusions

Strengthening positive attitudes to vaccination and reducing conspiracy suspicions with regards to the coronavirus may have a positive effect on vaccine uptake, especially among ethnic groups with heightened vaccine hesitancy. However, vaccine hesitancy associated with age and gender does not appear to be explained by other predictor variables tested here.

Type
Original Article
Creative Commons
Creative Common License - CCCreative Common License - BY
This is an Open Access article, distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution licence (http://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/4.0), which permits unrestricted re- use, distribution and reproduction, provided the original article is properly cited.
Copyright
Copyright © The Author(s), 2021. Published by Cambridge University Press

Introduction

Vaccine hesitancy, or ‘delay in acceptance or refusal of vaccination despite availability of vaccination services’ (MacDonald & Sage Working Group on Vaccine Hesitancy, Reference MacDonald2015, p. 4163), has been identified as one of the greatest threats to public health at a global level (WHO, 2019). A refusal rate of more than 10% is estimated to be sufficient to undermine the population benefits of vaccination against SARS-CoV-2, the novel coronavirus which causes COVID-19 (DeRoo, Pudalov, & Fu, Reference DeRoo, Pudalov and Fu2020).

Vaccine hesitancy appears to be a particular problem for the developed world. For example, negative views of vaccines are far higher in both Western and Eastern Europe than they are in South Asia and Eastern Africa (Wellcome, 2019, pp. 110–111). Some argue that vaccine hesitancy in the developed world is currently driven by the circulation of misinformation on social media (Rochel de Camargo, Reference Rochel de Camargo2020, p. 3; see Suarez-Lledo & Alvarez-Galvez, Reference Suarez-Lledo and Alvarez-Galvez2021 for a systematic review of studies of medical misinformation on social media), which could explain why under-vaccination and delayed vaccination have been rising for some time in the USA, and reached a level that has been described as a ‘cultural epidemic’ in Europe (McIntosh, Janda, Ehrich, Pettoello-Mantovani, & Somekh, Reference McIntosh, Janda, Ehrich, Pettoello-Mantovani and Somekh2016; Salmon, Dudley, Glanz, & Omer, Reference Salmon, Dudley, Glanz and Omer2015). Many studies have found a link between vaccine hesitancy and exposure to online anti-vaccination materials, as well as to the conspiracy theories that they so often promote (Ahmed, Quinn, Hancock, Freimuth, & Jamison, Reference Ahmed, Quinn, Hancock, Freimuth and Jamison2018; Dunn, Leask, Zhou, Mandl, & Coiera, Reference Dunn, Leask, Zhou, Mandl and Coiera2015; Reference Dunn, Surian, Leask, Dey, Mandl and Coiera2017; Lyons, Merola, & Reifler, Reference Lyons, Merola and Reifler2019; Wilson & Wiysonge, Reference Wilson and Wiysonge2021), and associations between coronavirus vaccine hesitancy, coronavirus conspiracy beliefs and/or use of social media or non-mainstream media as an information source have been reported (Allington, Duffy, Wessely, Dhavan, & Rubin, Reference Allington, Duffy, Wessely, Dhavan and Rubin2020; Reference Allington, McAndrew, Moxham-Hall and Duffy2021; Bertin, Nera, & Delouvée, Reference Bertin, Nera and Delouvée2020; Freeman et al., Reference Freeman, Loe, Chadwick, Vaccari, Waite, Rosebrock and Jenner2020; Jennings et al., Reference Jennings, Stoker, Willis, Valgardsson, Gaskell, Devine and Mills2021; McAndrew & Allington, Reference McAndrew and Allington2020; Murphy, Vallières, Bentall, Shevlin, & McBride, Reference Murphy, Vallières, Bentall, Shevlin and McBride2020; Romer & Jamieson, Reference Romer and Jamieson2020), with one recent study finding among people whose news diet is dominated by social media an association between conspiracy mentality and the intention to discourage coronavirus vaccination (Chadwick et al., Reference Chadwick, Kaiser, Vaccari, Freeman, Lambe, Loe and Vanderslott2021). There is also experimental support for a causal relationship between exposure to anti-vaccination materials (including conspiracy theories) and reduced intention to vaccinate (Betsch, Renkewitz, Betsch, & Ulshöfer, Reference Betsch, Renkewitz, Betsch and Ulshöfer2010; Chen, Zhang, Young, Wu, & Zhu, Reference Chen, Zhang, Young, Wu and Zhu2020; Jolley & Douglas, Reference Jolley and Douglas2014). The largest experimental study found exposure to coronavirus- and vaccine-related misinformation to reduce the proportion of participants who say that they would ‘definitely’ be vaccinated against the coronavirus by 6.2 percentage points in the UK and 6.4 percentage points in the USA (Loomba, de Figueiredo, Piatek, de Graaf, & Larson, Reference Loomba, de Figueiredo, Piatek, de Graaf and Larson2021).

However, there are many other variables that might explain vaccine hesitancy. General vaccine attitudes have been found to predict coronavirus vaccine refusal in the USA, especially among women and African Americans (Callaghan et al., Reference Callaghan, Moghtaderi, Lueck, Hotez, Strych, Dor and Motta2021), and many studies have found a relationship between trust and vaccine hesitancy (Dubé, Gagnon, Nickels, Jeram, & Schuster, Reference Dubé, Gagnon, Nickels, Jeram and Schuster2014; Hornsey, Lobera, & Díaz-Catalan, Reference Hornsey, Lobera and Díaz-Catalan2020; Mills, Jadad, Ross, & Wilson, Reference Mills, Jadad, Ross and Wilson2005; Quinn, Jamison, An, Hancock, & Freimuth, Reference Quinn, Jamison, An, Hancock and Freimuth2019; Wilder-Smith & Qureshi, Reference Wilder-Smith and Qureshi2020). It is also plausible that scientific, medical and political mistrust might drive both vaccine hesitancy and conspiracy beliefs. For example, both the heightened rates of HIV conspiracy belief and the lower rates of HIV-protective behaviours within the African American community have been attributed to mistrust triggered by the experience of racism (Ball, Lawson, & Alim, Reference Ball, Lawson and Alim2013, p. 4). Some anti-vaccine campaigners have adopted a strategy of ‘framing COVID-19 vaccination in terms of past medical abuses against minority groups’ (Callaghan et al., Reference Callaghan, Moghtaderi, Lueck, Hotez, Strych, Dor and Motta2021, p. 2), and there is qualitative evidence to suggest that coronavirus vaccine hesitancy among African Americans may be driven by mistrust in the medical establishment and by perceptions of racism in the political system (Momplaisir et al., Reference Momplaisir, Haynes, Nkwihoreze, Nelson, Werner and Jemmott2021). Similarly, some experts have attributed heightened levels of coronavirus misinformation acceptance and coronavirus vaccine hesitancy within some minority ethnic communities in the UK to mistrust driven by experiences of racism, discrimination and exclusion (Razai, Osama, McKechnie, & Majeed, Reference Razai, Osama, McKechnie and Majeed2021, p. 1).

The current study accordingly tests for the possible impact on coronavirus vaccine hesitancy in the UK of conspiracy beliefs with regards to the coronavirus, general vaccine attitudes, risk perceptions, informational reliance on social media and trust in scientists, healthcare professionals and the government. Other factors which recent British studies have found to be associated with coronavirus vaccine hesitancy include youth, female gender, minority ethnic group membership, low education and low income, with the perception of the coronavirus as a personal threat being associated with lower levels of hesitancy (Allington, McAndrew, Moxham-Hall, & Duffy, Reference Allington, McAndrew, Moxham-Hall and Duffy2021; Freeman et al., Reference Freeman, Loe, Chadwick, Vaccari, Waite, Rosebrock and Jenner2020; Jennings et al., Reference Jennings, Stoker, Willis, Valgardsson, Gaskell, Devine and Mills2021); all of these are therefore also tested for. By using a large representative sample and constructing a variety of multivariate models, this study contributes to knowledge at once by attempting to replicate the findings of previous studies and by attempting to explain and establish the relative importance of the predictors through controls.

Hypotheses

The study was designed to test the hypothesis that coronavirus vaccine hesitancy will have:

  1. (1) A negative relationship with age

  2. (2) A positive relationship with female gender

  3. (3) A positive relationship with membership of other than white ethnic groups

  4. (4) A negative relationship with level of education

  5. (5) A negative relationship with household income

  6. (6) A negative relationship with reliance on legacy media as a source of information about coronavirus

  7. (7) A positive relationship with reliance on social media as a source of information about coronavirus

  8. (8) A negative relationship with perceived risk from the coronavirus, with regards to:

    1. (a) The self

    2. (b) People in the UK

    3. (c) People elsewhere in the world

  9. (9) A negative relationship with trust in:

    1. (a) The national government

    2. (b) Scientists working in the academic sector

    3. (c) Scientists working in the private sector

    4. (d) Medical professionals

  10. (10) A negative relationship with general attitudes to vaccination

  11. (11) A positive relationship with conspiracy suspicions with regards to the coronavirus

Hypotheses 6–11 were tested both before and after controlling for the demographic variables mentioned in hypotheses 1–5. For analytic purposes, all ethnic groups were aggregated into two categories: white and other than white. Education was operationalised as highest completed level of education, which was measured on an ordinal scale ranging from primary school to postgraduate qualifications or their equivalent. In view of Quinn et al.'s (Reference Quinn, Jamison, An, Hancock and Freimuth2019) argument that it is important to distinguish general attitudes from those relating to a specific vaccine, it is emphasised that hypothesis 10 relates to attitudes to vaccination in general (and not to coronavirus vaccination in particular), and hypothesis 11 relates to conspiracy suspicions with regards to the coronavirus itself (rather than coronavirus vaccination), while the dependent variable in all cases is hesitancy with regards to coronavirus vaccination.

Methodology

Questionnaire

Key questionnaire items are presented in Table 1. Each respondent received one of two versions of the vaccine hesitancy question, at random. The first version asked ‘If a vaccine for coronavirus becomes available, how likely or unlikely would you personally be to get the vaccine?’ The second asked ‘If a vaccine for coronavirus becomes available, how likely or unlikely would you personally be to get the vaccine if you were offered it?’ This was done because of concerns raised that the question might be conflating likelihood of accepting the vaccine with likelihood of being offered it (see Full Fact, 2021).

Table 1. Key questionnaire items

Questionnaire items used to assess vaccine attitudes were drawn directly from the Wellcome Global Monitor survey (Wellcome, 2019, Q24, Q25 and Q26), whereas items used to assess conspiracy suspicions were adapted from the Flexible Inventory of Conspiracy Suspicions (Wood, Reference Wood2016). Items used to assess informational reliance on various media sources were drawn from an earlier study of conspiracy beliefs (Allington et al., Reference Allington, Duffy, Wessely, Dhavan and Rubin2020). Items used to assess trust were adapted from the Wellcome Global Monitor Survey (Wellcome, 2019, Q11B, Q11E, Q14A and Q15a).

Ethical statement

Data collection followed ethical and data protection procedures at the University of Bristol, King's College London and Ipsos MORI. Prior to commencing the study, participants were informed that they were taking part in coronavirus-related research led by the University of Bristol, and were provided with the principal investigator's name and contact details.

Data collection

Data collection was carried out online by Ipsos MORI, and ran from 21 November to 21 December 2020. The data collection formed part of a longitudinal study in which each wave involved collection from a stratified random sample of members of a recruited panel of UK adults. As the current sample included recontactees from two previous waves, and as there was additional recruitment from other panels where responses within certain strata were inadequate to produce representativeness, it is not a probability sample. The panel was stratified for representativeness of the UK population on age, gender, region, and working status. Demographic weights were calculated post-collection by Ipsos MORI on the basis of education and geographical region and of gender interlocked with age, NRS social grade and working status. The sample was designed and weighted for demographic representativeness and is treated as equivalent to a random sample below. In total, 4343 individuals gave consent to take part and completed the questionnaire.

Descriptive statistics

For descriptive statistics, see Table 2.Footnote Footnote 1 The 12 respondents who stated that they preferred to identify their gender in another way than as male or female are included neither in the ‘female’ nor in the ‘male’ column in Table 2; for analytic purposes, they were grouped together with males in a single non-female category. The 55 respondents who did not provide information on their ethnicity are included neither in the ‘white’ nor in the ‘other’ column; for analytic purposes, they were treated as missing data for all calculations involving ethnicity.

Table 2. Descriptive statistics

The sample appears broadly representative of the UK population on gender, age and minority ethnic group membership, with over-representation of highly-educated individuals. Just over half the respondents stated that they were ‘certain’ or ‘very likely’ to be vaccinated.

Key measures

In total, 2174 respondents were asked the shorter version of the vaccine hesitancy question, and 2169 were asked the longer version of the question. Welch's unequal variances t test on ranks confirms that there was no significant difference in answers, t (3962.18) = 0.46, p = 0.648, and the items were accordingly combined to create a vaccine hesitancy indicator for our analyses, with lower expressed likelihood interpreted as greater hesitancy.

Rank-order correlations and Guttman's lambda 6 were used to assess internal reliability of scales and implied scales. Answers to items used to assess conspiracy suspicions had a mean correlation of r s = 0.67, λ 6 = 0.90. Answers to questions used to assess attitudes to vaccines in general had a mean correlation of r s = 0.69, λ 6 = 0.83. Answers to questions used to assess informational reliance on social media had a mean correlation of r s = 0.49, λ 6 = 0.80. Answers to the two questions used to assess informational reliance on the legacy media were positively correlated, r s = 0.22. Answers to the three questions used to assess risk perceptions with regards to the coronavirus had a mean correlation of r s = 0.56, which would have equated to λ 6 = 0.77. However, these were not aggregated. Answers to the four questions used to assess trust had a mean correlation of r s = 0.38, which would have equated to λ 6 = 0.69. Again, these were not aggregated.

For all analytic purposes, ‘don't know’ and ‘prefer not to answer’ responses were treated as missing data. Altogether, 378 respondents answered ‘don't know’ to the question used to assess vaccine hesitancy.

Analytic methodology

Analysis was carried out primarily through the construction and examination of a series of linear rank-order models. Models of this type, often described as rank regression models, involve applying the rank transformation to response and predictor variables and then fitting a linear model to the ranks using the ordinary least squares method. Models of this kind were initially proposed for modelling monotonic but non-linear relationships between continuous variables (Iman & Conover, Reference Iman and Conover1979), and are widely recommended for dealing with outliers (Chen, Tang, Lu, & Tu, Reference Chen, Tang, Lu and Tu2014). As the nonparametric equivalent of linear modelling, they have also been argued to be appropriate for ordinal data while requiring fewer assumptions than ordered logit or probit models (Fu, Wang, & Liu, Reference Fu, Wang and Liu2012). Because they can accommodate ordinal predictors, linear rank-order models enable the preservation of more information than would be the case for logit or probit models. Moreover, unlike such models, they permit the calculation of r 2, which provides a meaningful and intuitively interpretable metric of comparison between competing models. Finally, they provide an intuitive measure of effect size: a coefficient of 1.0 for an independent variable corresponds to an average increase of one rank in the dependent variable for each rank by which the independent variable increases.

Some scholars have argued that linear rank-order models may produce higher type I error rates (Headrick & Rotou, Reference Headrick and Rotou2001). For this reason, a parallel set of logit models was created using the same variables, albeit with ordinal variables dichotomised and with continuous variables standardised. Risk, trust and level of education were dichotomised by coding the top two levels (equating to ‘A fairly high risk’ and ‘A very high risk’ for risk, ‘A fair amount’ and ‘A great deal’ for trust, and undergraduate and postgraduate degrees or their equivalent for education) as 1 and the others as 0. Vaccine hesitancy was dichotomised by coding ‘Certain’ or ‘Very likely’ as 0 and anything else as 1. Household income was dichotomised by coding the median category of £25 000–£34 999 and anything above it as 1 and everything below it as 0. Demographic weights were applied in both linear rank-order models and logit models.

A matrix of unweighted rank-order correlation coefficients is additionally provided, both in the interest of transparency and as a form of exploratory analysis.

Findings

Correlations

Rank-order correlations between key variables are presented in Table 3. Rank-order correlations between coronavirus vaccine hesitancy and all other variables are visualised in Fig. 1. Information source variables are unaggregated, as there is clear interest in comparing correlations both for print and broadcast media and for individual social media platforms. Informational reliance on broadcast media has a stronger negative correlation with vaccine hesitancy than does informational reliance on print media (r s = −0.19 as opposed to r s = −0.07), and whereas informational reliance on all social media platforms is positively correlated with vaccine hesitancy, this correlation is strongest with regards to Facebook and YouTube (r s = 0.15 and r s = 0.18, respectively). However, the strongest positive correlate of vaccine hesitancy is the aggregate measure of conspiracy suspicions with regards to the coronavirus (r s = 0.45), and the strongest negative correlates are positive attitude towards vaccines in general (r s = −0.54) and perceived risk posed by the coronavirus (especially personal risk, r s = −0.27). After these, the strongest associations were the negative correlation with trust in scientists working in universities (r s = −0.37) and for companies (r s = −0.33) and with trust in medical professionals (r s = −0.34); the negative correlation with trust in government was much weaker (r s = −0.17).

Fig. 1. Rank-order correlations with vaccine hesitancy.

Table 3. Rank-order correlations

Among demographic variables, age was most strongly correlated with vaccine hesitancy, with younger people being more vaccine hesitant (r s = −0.30). Unsurprisingly, age was also negatively correlated with informational use of social media (especially Instagram, r s = −0.35, Twitter, r s = −0.32 and YouTube, r s = −0.30), which was in turn positively correlated with conspiracy suspicions (especially with regards to Facebook, r s = 0.25, YouTube, r s = 0.24 and Instagram, r s = 0.21).

Membership of an other than white ethnic group was positively correlated with vaccine hesitancy (r s = 0.14), as was female gender (r s = 0.08). Level of education was positively correlated with attitudes towards vaccines in general (r s = 0.16) and negatively correlated with coronavirus conspiracy suspicions (r s = −0.15), and so it is perhaps surprising that it should be uncorrelated with coronavirus vaccine hesitancy (r s = −0.01); on the other hand, it is negatively correlated with age (r s = −0.23), which could act to mask a relationship. Household income was found to have a very weak negative correlation with vaccine hesitancy (r s = −0.05), but also to have a slightly stronger negative correlation with all forms of risk perception (r s ⊂ [−0.07, −0.08]).

Given the high fatality rate for COVID-19 among members of certain minority ethnic groups in the UK (ONS, 2020), it is noteworthy that ethnicity was uncorrelated with perceived risk to the self (r s = 0.00). Ethnic minority status was, however, positively correlated both with conspiracy suspicions (r s = 0.11) and with use of social media for information about coronavirus (especially YouTube, r s = 0.19, and WhatsApp, r s = 0.20), and negatively correlated with both vaccine attitudes (r s = −0.09) and age (r s = −0.20). It is thus already apparent from the correlation matrix that there are a number of variables which might account for the association between ethnicity and vaccine hesitancy. Given suggestions that higher vaccine hesitancy among minority ethnic groups might be accounted for by mistrust, it is interesting that there was no correlation between ethnicity and trust in government (r s = 0.00), a very weak correlation between membership of an other than white ethnic group and trust in scientists, whether working in the academic or the private sector (r s = −0.05), and an only slightly stronger negative correlation with trust in medical professionals (r s = −0.07).

Statistical models and hypothesis tests

Tables 4 and 5 present a series of eight linear rank-order models. Coefficients are quoted both as estimates with p values and as 95% confidence intervals, and visualised in Fig. 2 (which excludes models that do not feature demographic predictors).

Fig. 2. Predictors of vaccine hesitancy, with 95% confidence intervals.

Table 4. Linear rank-order models I, IIa, IIb, IIIa and IIIb

Table 5. Linear rank-order models IVa, IVb, Va, Vb and VI

Model I tests demographic predictors. Importantly, level of education emerges as a predictor of lower vaccine hesitancy once other demographic variables are controlled for. H1 (negative association with age), H2 (positive association with female gender), H3 (positive association with other than white ethnic group) and H5 (negative association with household income) are all supported, p < 0.001, as is H4 (negative association with level of education), p < 0.001. Relatively, little variation in vaccine hesitancy is explained by demographic variables alone (r 2 =  0.12), although, as we shall see, these explain more than some non-demographic variables. Moreover, the coefficients for age and female gender remain almost unchanged throughout the remaining models.

Model IIa tests information source predictors, with model IIb adding demographic controls. These show that the association of legacy media with lower vaccine hesitancy and the association of social media with higher vaccine hesitancy are independent and only partially explained by age. H6 (negative association with legacy media) and H7 (positive association with social media) are supported, p < 0.001. However, without demographic controls, these variables explain less variance in vaccine hesitancy than demographic variables alone (r 2 = 0.07 for model IIa).

Model IIIa tests risk perception predictors, with model IIIb adding demographic controls. These show that perception of personal risk is the strongest predictor, although its association is partially explained by demographic variables (note the positive correlation between age and perceived personal risk, r s = 0.22). H8a (positive association with perceived personal risk) is supported both before and after controls, p < 0.001. H8b (positive association with perceived risk to other people in the UK) is unsupported before controls, p = 0.093, but supported after controls, p = 0.006. H8c (positive association with perceived risk to people elsewhere in the world) is supported both before controls, p < 0.001, and after controls, p < 0.001. However, without demographic controls, these variables still explain less variance in vaccine hesitancy than demographic variables alone (r 2 =  0.08 for model IIIa).

Model IVa tests trust predictors, with model IVb adding demographic controls. All tested forms of trust are significant in model IV, p < 0.001, but trust in the government had by far the smallest effect, whereas trust in scientists had the largest. Significance and effect sizes were virtually unchanged by controls. H9a–d are therefore all supported, both before and after demographic controls. It is notable that the association of other than white ethnic group with vaccine hesitancy (e.g. in model I) is partially reduced by introduction of trust variables as predictors in model IVb.

Model Va tests vaccine attitude and conspiracy suspicion predictors, with model Vb adding demographic controls. H10 (negative association with vaccine attitudes) and H11 (positive association with conspiracy suspicions) are supported both before and after controls, p ≤ 0.001. Model Va explains substantially more of the variance in observed vaccine hesitancy than any previous model (r 2 = 0.35 as opposed to r 2 =  0.19 for model IIIb and r 2 = 0.26 for model IVb), and this rises further still with demographic controls in model Vb (r 2 = 0.43). It is notable that the association between vaccine hesitancy and ethnic minority membership is statistically insignificant in model Vb but not in model IVb, which suggests that more of the variance associated with ethnicity is explained by vaccine attitudes and conspiracy suspicions than by trust. Moreover, the negative association between vaccine hesitancy and education in models I, IIb, IIIb and IVc is replaced by a positive association in model Vb.

Model VI tests all predictors together. It can be observed that model VI represents only a very marginal improvement over model Vb in terms of variation explained (r 2 = 0.46 as opposed to r 2 = 0.43). In other words, if we already know a person's age, gender, income, education, attitudes to vaccines in general and suspicions with regards to the possibility of a coronavirus-related conspiracy, then we will be able to predict his or her level of coronavirus vaccine hesitancy quite well, and additionally knowing his or her ethnic group, perception of coronavirus risks, level of trust in scientists, medics, and the government and sources of coronavirus information will tell us little more. It is also noteworthy that the coefficients both for conspiracy suspicions and for vaccine attitudes change very little between models Va, Vb and VI, while all forms of risk perception but perceived personal risk disappear from model VI, as do the effects of social media and most of the effects of trust. The positive association with education from model Vb remains, but loses statistical significance.

As discussed above, logit models were constructed because of theoretical concerns regarding type I errors. These concerns do not appear to have been borne out. Looking across all coefficients in all models, 44 were found to be significant at p < 0.001 in the linear rank-order models, whereas 45 were found to be significant to the same level in the logit models. This means that there is no evidence of type I error inflation as a result of the use of linear rank-order models.

Conclusions and recommendations

As expected given findings of earlier studies, vaccine hesitancy was found to be negatively associated with age, household income, level of education, level of coronavirus risk perception (possibly excluding perceived risk to people living in other countries), trust in government, scientific and medical authorities and informational reliance on legacy media, and positively associated with female gender, other than white ethnic group, and informational reliance on social media. The effect associated with age was consistent across models, as was the weaker (but still very highly statistically significant) effect associated with gender, indicating that these effects are not explicable in terms of mutual correlations with other variables measured here. However, the most powerful predictors of vaccine hesitancy were attitudes to vaccines in general and conspiracy suspicions with regards to coronavirus, which appear to explain much more of the variation in coronavirus vaccine hesitancy than any of the aforementioned variables. Indeed, much or all of the variation associated with ethnic group, trust and informational reliance on social media appears to be explained by these two variables, whereas the association with education is reversed once they are controlled for.

It is notable that the association of other than white ethnic group with vaccine hesitancy (e.g. in model I) is partially reduced by introduction of trust variables as predictors in model IVb. This suggests that some of the heightened vaccine hesitancy which has been observed in some minority ethnic groups is attributable to lower scientific or medical trust. However, these variables were only weakly correlated with ethnicity, and government trust was not correlated at all. Ethnicity loses much more of its predictive power after controls for conspiracy suspicions and vaccine attitudes than it does after controls for trust, which suggests that these suspicions and attitudes may account for more of the vaccine hesitancy that is associated with minority ethnic group membership. However, this does not mean that a general intervention designed to address vaccine attitudes and conspiracy suspicions in the population as a whole would remove the association between vaccine hesitancy and ethnicity: as Chou and Budenz caution, health communicators should eschew generic messaging (Reference Chou and Budenz2020). Useful guidelines exist for developing vaccination campaigns targeting specific ethnic or religious groups (Butler, MacDonald & Sage Working Group on Vaccine Hesitancy Reference Butler and MacDonald2015).

The findings of this study suggest that vaccine hesitancy is strongly tied to both conspiracy suspicions and attitudes to vaccines in general. This presents a unique challenge for public health organisations and government vaccination programmes, which need to combat misinformation to ensure uptake of COVID-19 vaccination. Although the findings presented here cannot support the idea of a causal link with media usage, they are consistent with the assumption of such a link, which is also consistent with or supported by a large volume of research literature. Lower levels of vaccine hesitancy among respondents who rely on legacy media for information about coronavirus are consistent with the assumption that certain forms of communication may already be having a positive effect, even if they are not necessarily taking place through channels used by all members of the population. By contrast, the continued association of social media use with vaccine hesitancy (even after demographic controls) is consistent with the assumption that platform attempts to promote the dissemination of valid information and limit the dissemination of misinformation (e.g. UK Government, 2020) have not achieved an impact – or had not by the time of data collection. Moreover, the finding that the predictive power of social media use disappears with controls for coronavirus conspiracy suspicions and general vaccine attitudes is consistent with the assumption that the association of social media with vaccine hesitancy may be attributable to misinformation, which would provide grounds for optimism that interventions designed to limit the circulation of misinformation on those platforms may ultimately bear fruit.

Limitations and scope for further research

The research here relies entirely on self-report measures for dependent and independent variables. Moreover, data collection was carried out as the rollout of the UK's vaccination programme began, and before several vaccines had been approved. As noted above, this was only shortly after several major platforms adopted enhanced policies to limit the circulation of coronavirus disinformation. Finally, although the sample was designed to be representative of the UK population (and appears representative on key demographic variables), it is not a true random sample.

Although this study has largely confirmed expectations arising from earlier studies and clarified the relative importance of the hypothesised predictors, an approach such as mediation analysis may be needed in order to further explore the inter-relations between the variables in question. Moreover, further data collection is urgently required in order to understand the reasons for different vaccine attitudes and levels of conspiracy suspicions within minority ethnic communities, particularly given that this does not appear to be accounted for by lower medical and political trust. The finding that the effects of age were not reducible to risk perceptions, conspiracy suspicions, vaccine attitudes or media use also needs further investigation, as does the finding that education is positively associated with vaccine hesitancy after controls for vaccine attitudes and coronavirus conspiracy suspicions. Finally, more research is clearly needed in order to understand the basis of vaccine hesitancy among women, as the association of vaccine hesitancy with female gender is not explained by any of the other variables investigated here. The effect size detected here appears relatively small, but existing research suggests that female caregivers may have an especially important role in vaccination decision-making within many communities (Allen et al., Reference Allen, de Jesus, Mars, Tom, Cloutier and Shelton2012, p. 3). If this holds true for coronavirus vaccination, then women will be a particularly important audience to reach, as vaccine hesitance on the part of one individual might potentially result in vaccine non-uptake throughout an entire family unit.

Technical appendix

All calculations were carried out in R 3.6.3 (R Core Team, 2020), with psych 2.0.12 (Revelle, Reference Revelle2020) for calculation of Guttman's lambda 6. Visualisations were created using ggplot2 3.3.3 (see Wickham, Reference Wickham2016).

Financial support

This research was supported by the Economic and Social Research Council, ES/V 015494/1.

Footnotes

The notes appear after the main text.

1 Due to rounding, percentages are not guaranteed to sum to 100.

References

Ahmed, N., Quinn, S. C., Hancock, G. R., Freimuth, V. S., & Jamison, A. (2018). Social media use and influenza vaccine uptake among white and African American adults. Vaccine, 36(49), 75567561.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Allen, J. D., de Jesus, M., Mars, D., Tom, L., Cloutier, L., & Shelton, R. C. (2012). Decision-making about the HPV vaccine among ethnically diverse parents: Implications for health communications. Journal of Oncology, 2012(401949), 15.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Allington, D., Duffy, B., Wessely, S., Dhavan, N., & Rubin, J. (2020). Health-protective behaviour, social media usage and conspiracy belief during the COVID-19 public health emergency. Psychological Medicine, 17.Google ScholarPubMed
Allington, D., McAndrew, S., Moxham-Hall, V., & Duffy, B. (2021). Media usage predicts intention to be vaccinated against SARS-CoV-2 in the US and the UK. Vaccine, 39(18), 25952603.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Ball, K., Lawson, W., & Alim, T. (2013). Medical mistrust, conspiracy beliefs, and HIV-related behaviour among AFRICAN Americans. Journal of Psychology and Behavioural Science, 1(1), 17.Google Scholar
Bertin, P., Nera, K., & Delouvée, S. (2020). Conspiracy beliefs, rejection of vaccination, and support for hydroxychloroquine: A conceptual replication-extension in the COVID-19 pandemic context. Frontiers in Psychology, 11(565128), 19.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Betsch, C., Renkewitz, F., Betsch, T., & Ulshöfer, C. (2010). The influence of vaccine-critical websites on perceiving vaccination risks. Journal of Health Psychology, 15(3), 446455.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Butler, R., MacDonald, N. E., & Sage Working Group on Vaccine Hesitancy. (2015). Diagnosing the determinants of vaccine hesitancy in specific subgroups: The guide to tailoring immunization programmes (TIP). Vaccine, 33, 41764179.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Callaghan, T., Moghtaderi, A., Lueck, J. A., Hotez, P., Strych, U., Dor, A., … Motta, M. (2021). Correlates and disparities of COVID-19 vaccine hesitancy. Social Science & Medicine, 272(113638).CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Chadwick, A., Kaiser, J., Vaccari, C., Freeman, D., Lambe, S., Loe, B. S.Vanderslott, S., et al. (2021). Online social endorsement and COVID-19 vaccine hesitancy in the UK. Social Media and Society, 7(2), 117.Google Scholar
Chen, T., Tang, W., Lu, Y., & Tu, X. (2014). Rank regression: An alternative regression approach for data with outliers. Shanghai Archives of Psychiatry, 26(5), 310315.Google ScholarPubMed
Chen, L., Zhang, Y., Young, R., Wu, X., & Zhu, G. (2020). Effects of vaccine-related conspiracy theories on Chinese young adults’ perceptions of the HPV vaccine: An experimental study. Health Communication, 111.Google ScholarPubMed
Chou, W.-Y. S., & Budenz, A. (2020). Considering emotion in COVID-19 vaccine communication: Addressing vaccine hesitancy and fostering vaccine confidence. Health Communication, 35(14), 17181722.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
DeRoo, S. S., Pudalov, N. J., & Fu, L. Y. (2020). Planning for a COVID-19 vaccination program. JAMA, 323(24), 24582459.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Dubé, E., Gagnon, D., Nickels, E., Jeram, S., & Schuster, M. (2014). Mapping vaccine hesitancy – country-specific characteristics of a global phenomenon. Vaccine, 32, 66496654.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Dunn, A. G., Leask, J., Zhou, X., Mandl, K. D., & Coiera, E. (2015). Associations between exposure to and expression of negative opinions about human papillomavirus vaccines on social media: An observational study. Journal of Medical Internet Research, 17(6), e144.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Dunn, A. G., Surian, D., Leask, J., Dey, A., Mandl, K. D., & Coiera, E. (2017). Mapping information exposure on social media to explain differences in HPV vaccine coverage in the United States. Vaccine, 35(23), 30333040.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Freeman, D., Loe, B. S., Chadwick, A., Vaccari, C., Waite, F., Rosebrock, L.Jenner, L., et al. (2020). COVID-19 vaccine hesitancy in the UK: The Oxford Coronavirus Explanations, Attitudes, and Narratives Survey (OCEANS) II. Psychological Medicine, 115.Google ScholarPubMed
Fu, L. Y., Wang, Y. G., & Liu, C. J. (2012). Rank regression for analyzing ordinal qualitative data for treatment comparison. Phytopathology, 102(11), 10641070.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Full Fact. (2021). One in Six May Refuse the COVID-19 Vaccine. https://fullfact.org/health/kings-vaccine-survey/Google Scholar
Headrick, T., & Rotou, O. (2001). An investigation of the rank transformation in multiple regression. Computational Statistics & Data Analysis, 38(2), 203215.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Hornsey, M. J., Lobera, J., & Díaz-Catalan, C. (2020). Vaccine hesitancy is strongly associated with distrust of conventional medicine, and only weakly associated with trust in alternative medicine. Social Science & Medicine, 255(113019), 16.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Iman, R. L., & Conover, W. J. (1979). The use of the rank transform in regression. Technometrics, 21(4), 499509.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Jennings, W., Stoker, G., Willis, H., Valgardsson, V., Gaskell, J., Devine, D., … Mills, M. C. (2021). Lack of trust and social Media Echo chambers predict COVID-19 vaccine hesitancy. medRxiv Preprint, 18.Google Scholar
Jolley, D., & Douglas, K. M. (2014). The effects of anti-vaccine conspiracy theories on vaccination intentions. PLoS ONE, 9(2), 19.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Loomba, S., de Figueiredo, A., Piatek, S. J., de Graaf, K., & Larson, H. J. (2021). Measuring the impact of COVID-19 vaccine misinformation on vaccination intent in the UK and USA. Nature Human Behaviour, 5), 337348.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Lyons, B., Merola, V., & Reifler, J. (2019). Not just asking questions: Effects of implicit and explicit conspiracy information about vaccines and genetic modification. Health Communication, 34(14), 17411750.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
MacDonald, N. E., & Sage Working Group on Vaccine Hesitancy. (2015). Vaccine hesitancy: Definition, scope and determinants. Vaccine, 33, 41614164.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
McAndrew, S., & Allington, D. (2020). Mode and frequency of COVID-19 information updates, political values, and future COVID-19 vaccine attitudes. PsyArXiv Preprints, 127. https://psyarxiv.com/j7srx/.Google Scholar
McIntosh, E. D. G., Janda, J., Ehrich, J. H. H., Pettoello-Mantovani, M., & Somekh, E. (2016). Vaccine hesitancy and refusal. European Paediatric Association Pages, 175, P248P249, E1.Google ScholarPubMed
Mills, E., Jadad, A. R., Ross, C., & Wilson, K. (2005). Systematic review of qualitative studies exploring parental beliefs and attitudes toward childhood vaccination identifies common barriers to vaccination. Journal of Clinical Epidemiology, 58(11), 10811088.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Momplaisir, F., Haynes, N., Nkwihoreze, H., Nelson, M., Werner, R. M., & Jemmott, J. (2021). Understanding drivers of coronavirus disease 2019 vaccine hesitancy among blacks. Clinical Infectious Diseases, 16.Google ScholarPubMed
Murphy, J., Vallières, F., Bentall, R. P., Shevlin, & M., & McBride, , (2020). Preparing for a COVID-19 vaccine: Identifying and psychologically profiling those who are vaccine hesitant or resistant in two general population samples. PsyArXiv Preprints, 142.Google Scholar
ONS. (2020). Updating Ethnic Contrasts in Deaths Involving the Coronavirus (COVID-19), England and Wales: Deaths Occurring 2 March to 28 July 2020. Office for National Statistics. https://www.ons.gov.uk/peoplepopulationandcommunity/birthsdeathsandmarriages/deaths/articles/updatingethniccontrastsindeathsinvolvingthecoronaviruscovid19englandandwales/deathsoccurring2marchto28july2020Google Scholar
Quinn, S. C., Jamison, A. M., An, J., Hancock, G. R., & Freimuth, V. S. (2019). Measuring vaccine hesitancy, confidence, trust and flu vaccine uptake: Results of a national survey of white and African American adults. Vaccine, 37, 11681173.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Razai, M. S., Osama, R., McKechnie, D. G. J., & Majeed, A. (2021). COVID-19 vaccine hesitancy among ethnic minority groups. British Medical Journal, 372(n513), 12.Google ScholarPubMed
R Core Team. (2020). R: A language and environment for statistical computing (version 6.3.6). Vienna, Austria: R Foundation for Statistical Computing. https://www.R-project.org/Google Scholar
Revelle, W. (2020). Psych: Procedures for psychological, psychometric, and personality research. Evanston, Illinois: Northwestern University. https://CRAN.R-project.org/package=psych.Google Scholar
Rochel de Camargo, K. Jr. (2020). Here we go again: The reemergence of anti-vaccine activism on the internet. Cadernos de Saúde Pública, 36, e00037620.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Romer, D., & Jamieson, K. H. (2020). Conspiracy theories as barriers to controlling the spread of COVID-19 in the U.S. Social Science & Medicine, 263, 113356.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Salmon, D. A., Dudley, M. Z., Glanz, J. M., & Omer, S. B. (2015). Vaccine hesitancy: Causes, consequences, and a call to action. Vaccine, 33, D66D71.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Suarez-Lledo, V., & Alvarez-Galvez, J. (2021). Journal of Medical Internet Research, 23(1), e17187.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
UK Government. (2020). Social Media Giants Agree Package of Measures with UK Government to Tackle Vaccine Disinformation: Platforms and UK Government Agree a Package of Measures to Reduce Vaccine Disinformation. https://www.gov.uk/government/news/social-media-giants-agree-package-of-measures-with-uk-government-to-tackle-vaccine-disinformationGoogle Scholar
Wellcome (2019). Wellcome global monitor: How does the world feel about science and health? London: Wellcome Trust.Google Scholar
WHO. (2019). Ten threats to global health in 2019. Geneva: World Health Organisation. https://www.who.int/news-room/spotlight/ten-threats-to-global-health-in-2019.Google Scholar
Wickham, H. (2016). Ggplot2: Elegant graphics for data analysis. New York: Springer-Verlag.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Wilder-Smith, A. B., & Qureshi, K. (2020). Resurgence of measles in Europe: A systematic review on parental attitudes and beliefs of measles vaccine. Journal of Epidemiology and Global Health, 10(1), 4658.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Wilson, S. L., & Wiysonge, C. (2021). Social Media and vaccine hesitancy. BMJ Global Health, 5, e004206.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Wood, M. J. (2016). Conspiracy suspicions as a proxy for beliefs in conspiracy theories: Implications for theory and measurement. British Journal of Psychology, 108(3), 507527.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Figure 0

Table 1. Key questionnaire items

Figure 1

Table 2. Descriptive statistics

Figure 2

Fig. 1. Rank-order correlations with vaccine hesitancy.

Figure 3

Table 3. Rank-order correlations

Figure 4

Fig. 2. Predictors of vaccine hesitancy, with 95% confidence intervals.

Figure 5

Table 4. Linear rank-order models I, IIa, IIb, IIIa and IIIb

Figure 6

Table 5. Linear rank-order models IVa, IVb, Va, Vb and VI