Hostname: page-component-8448b6f56d-cfpbc Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-04-23T14:39:34.486Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

Prenatal substance exposure and maternal hostility from pregnancy to toddlerhood: Associations with temperament profiles at 16 months of age

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  15 October 2021

Brendan D. Ostlund
Affiliation:
Department of Psychology, The Pennsylvania State University, University Park, USA
Koraly E. Pérez-Edgar
Affiliation:
Department of Psychology, The Pennsylvania State University, University Park, USA
Shannon Shisler
Affiliation:
Research Institute on Addictions, University at Buffalo, State University of New York, Buffalo, USA
Sarah Terrell
Affiliation:
Department of Human Development and Family Studies, The Pennsylvania State University, University Park, USA
Stephanie Godleski
Affiliation:
Department of Psychology, Rochester Institute of Technology, Rochester, USA
Pamela Schuetze
Affiliation:
Department of Psychology, State University of New York, Buffalo, USA
Rina D. Eiden*
Affiliation:
Department of Psychology, The Pennsylvania State University, University Park, USA
*
Author for Correspondence: Rina D. Eiden, The Pennsylvania State University, 256 Moore Building, University Park, PA 16802; E-mail: rde5106@psu.edu
Rights & Permissions [Opens in a new window]

Abstract

We investigated whether infant temperament was predicted by level of and change in maternal hostility, a putative transdiagnostic vulnerability for psychopathology, substance use, and insensitive parenting. A sample of women (N = 247) who were primarily young, low-income, and had varying levels of substance use prenatally (69 nonsmokers, 81 tobacco-only smokers, and 97 tobacco and marijuana smokers) reported their hostility in the third trimester of pregnancy and at 2, 9, and 16 months postpartum, and their toddler's temperament and behavior problems at 16 months. Maternal hostility decreased from late pregnancy to 16 months postpartum. Relative to pregnant women who did not use substances, women who used both marijuana and tobacco prenatally reported higher levels of hostility while pregnant and exhibited less change in hostility over time. Toddlers who were exposed to higher levels of prenatal maternal hostility were more likely to be classified in temperament profiles that resemble either irritability or inhibition, identified via latent profile analysis. These two profiles were each associated with more behavior problems concurrently, though differed in their association with competence. Our results underscore the utility of transdiagnostic vulnerabilities in understanding the intergenerational transmission of psychopathology risk and are discussed in regards to the Research Domain Criteria (RDoC) framework.

Type
Special Issue Article
Creative Commons
Creative Common License - CCCreative Common License - BY
This is an Open Access article, distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution licence (http://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/4.0/), which permits unrestricted re-use, distribution, and reproduction in any medium, provided the original work is properly cited.
Copyright
Copyright © The Author(s), 2021. Published by Cambridge University Press

A mother's mood during the transition from pregnancy to parenthood may be characterized by periods of both stability and change. Recent work in perinatology and developmental psychopathology converge on the suggestion that a mother's emotional experience while pregnant and in the first years of life influences her child's long-term development, including elevated risk for neurodevelopmental and behavioral problems from infancy to adolescence (see Van den Bergh et al., Reference Van den Bergh, van den Heuvel, Lahti, Braeken, de Rooij, Entringer and Schwab2017 for review). For example, pre- and early postnatal maternal emotion marked by anxiety has been linked to infant negative affect and childhood internalizing symptoms (e.g., Lawrence, Creswell, Cooper, & Murray, Reference Lawrence, Creswell, Cooper and Murray2020; Spry et al., Reference Spry, Aarsman, Youssef, Patton, Macdonald, Sanson and Olsson2020). These effects likely vary by the type, timing, and intensity of a pregnant woman's pre- or postnatal emotional experience, which itself may be exacerbated by other stressors, such as substance use and poverty (e.g., Eiden et al., Reference Eiden, Leonard, Colder, Homish, Schuetze and Huestis2011). Building on these findings, we argue three significant gaps in our literature must be addressed to aid parents, practitioners, and policymakers in determining how best to support pregnant women and their infants. Specifically, studies to date have often limited the assessment of a mother's emotional experience to monolithic clinical diagnoses, relied on static measures of maternal mental health, and have largely failed to disentangle the influence of pre- and postnatal maternal mood on infant neurobehavior.

The purpose of this study was to examine whether individual differences in infant temperament could be predicted by a mother's level of hostility, a putative transdiagnostic vulnerability for psychopathology, substance use, and insensitive parenting (Eiden et al., Reference Eiden, Leonard, Colder, Homish, Schuetze and Huestis2011; Schuetze, Eiden, & Dombkowski, Reference Schuetze, Eiden and Dombkowski2006). We assessed hostility during pregnancy and across the first 16 postnatal months in a sample of women who were primarily young, low-income, and had varying levels of substance use prenatally. Using a prospective longitudinal design, we sought to address some of the methodological limitations in the extant literature. Namely, we examined both level and change in hostility to characterize the role of pre- and postnatal maternal emotion on infant dysregulation within the context of high sociodemographic risk. In order to further knowledge on the intergenerational transmission of psychopathology risk, we integrated this approach into the National Institute of Mental Health (NIMH)'s Research Domain Criteria (RDoC) – an initiative aimed at describing patterns of both dysfunction and adaptive functioning across levels of analysis to clarify how psychopathologies manifest across development (Franklin, Jamieson, Glenn, & Nock, Reference Franklin, Jamieson, Glenn and Nock2015; Insel et al., Reference Insel, Cuthbert, Garvey, Heinssen, Pine, Quinn and Wang2010; Kozak & Cuthbert, Reference Kozak and Cuthbert2016).

Hostility: a transdiagnostic vulnerability evident in the perinatal period

Perinatal psychiatric disorders are common obstetric complications linked to adverse child outcomes from birth onward (Gluckman, Hanson, Cooper, & Thornburg, Reference Gluckman, Hanson, Cooper and Thornburg2008; Howard & Khalifeh, Reference Howard and Khalifeh2020; Van den Bergh et al., Reference Van den Bergh, van den Heuvel, Lahti, Braeken, de Rooij, Entringer and Schwab2017). Included in these diagnostic categories, however, are heterogeneous subgroups of individuals with symptom clusters that often do not fit neatly into traditional psychiatric taxonomy (e.g., Putnam et al., Reference Putnam, Wilcox, Robertson-Blackmore, Sharkey, Bergink and Munk-Olsen2017; Walsh et al., Reference Walsh, Mccormack, Webster, Pinto, Lee, Feng and Monk2019), a common problem in clinical research. Consequently, the NIMH introduced RDoC to aid researchers in identifying intermediate phenotypes – defined by functioning along a continuum of neural, cognitive, emotional, and behavioral activity – that may explain heterogeneity within, as well as comorbidities between, psychiatric disorders (Casey, Oliveri, & Insel, Reference Casey, Oliveri and Insel2014; Cuthbert & Insel, Reference Cuthbert and Insel2010; Insel et al., Reference Insel, Cuthbert, Garvey, Heinssen, Pine, Quinn and Wang2010). Burgeoning evidence suggests that this approach may be particularly useful when considering risk subgroups identified based on vulnerabilities that cut across diagnostic categories (Blau, Orloff, & Hormes, Reference Blau, Orloff and Hormes2020; Lin et al., Reference Lin, Kaliush, Conradt, Terrell, Neff, Allen and Crowell2019; Obeysekare et al., Reference Obeysekare, Cohen, Coles, Pearlstein, Monzon and Sharkey2020; Ostlund et al., Reference Ostlund, Vlisides-Henry, Crowell, Raby, Terrell, Brown and Conradt2019). A focus on symptoms manifested in the pre- and early postpartum period may also help identify patterns of functioning and mental health outcomes across two generations.

Transdiagnostic vulnerabilities are relatively stable, trait-like patterns of behavior that range from normal to abnormal. Exceptionally high (or low) levels of these vulnerabilities contribute to a range of psychiatric disorders, demonstrating their clinical utility (Beauchaine, Zisner, & Sauder, Reference Beauchaine, Zisner and Sauder2017; Dalgleish, Black, Johnston, & Bevan, Reference Dalgleish, Black, Johnston and Bevan2020; Harvey, Watkins, Mansell, & Shafran, Reference Harvey, Watkins, Mansell and Shafran2004; Krueger & Eaton, Reference Krueger and Eaton2015; Nolan-Hoeksema & Watkins, Reference Nolan-Hoeksema and Watkins2011). However, there is a dearth of evidence describing transdiagnostic vulnerabilities among pregnant women. To address this shortcoming, Lin et al. (Reference Lin, Kaliush, Conradt, Terrell, Neff, Allen and Crowell2019) examined whether a pregnant woman's level of emotion dysregulation, a known contributor to psychopathology risk across the lifespan (Beauchaine, Reference Beauchaine2015; Cole, Hall, & Hajal, Reference Cole, Hall, Hajal, Beauchaine and Hinshaw2013), was associated with measures of mental health and physiological responding to stress. The authors found that a mother's level of emotion dysregulation while pregnant was related to more chronic and episodic stress, higher trait and pregnancy-specific anxiety, more self-injurious thoughts and behaviors, and more self- and interviewer-reported depressive and borderline symptoms. They also found that pregnant women with higher levels of emotion dysregulation exhibited a blunted parasympathetic response to an ecologically valid infant cry stressor, reflecting a diminished physiological reactivity to infant cues that may thwart responsive caregiving efforts (Ablow, Marks, Feldman, & Huffman, Reference Ablow, Marks, Feldman and Huffman2013; Del Vecchio, Walter, & O'Leary, Reference Del Vecchio, Walter and O'Leary2009; Schuetze & Zeskind, Reference Schuetze and Zeskind2001). Utilizing the same sample, Ostlund et al. (Reference Ostlund, Vlisides-Henry, Crowell, Raby, Terrell, Brown and Conradt2019) found that newborns whose mother reported higher levels of emotion dysregulation while pregnant exhibited blunted arousal and attention soon after birth.

Despite elevated psychopathology risk, few studies that have examined prenatal transdiagnostic vulnerabilities investigate these risk processes among pregnant women who are high risk by virtue of sociodemographic factors such as age, race/ethnicity, or socioeconomic status (e.g., Ostlund et al., Reference Ostlund, Vlisides-Henry, Crowell, Raby, Terrell, Brown and Conradt2019; see Walsh et al., Reference Walsh, Mccormack, Webster, Pinto, Lee, Feng and Monk2019 for exception). Whether these transdiagnostic vulnerabilities adequately capture psychopathology risk among women who are predominately young, low-income, and used illicit substances while pregnant remains to be seen. This clustering of demographic, economic, and structural stressors increases risk for maladaptive outcomes for both a mother and her developing child, making this a particularly vulnerable population.

Here, the focus is on maternal hostility while pregnant and in the first two years postpartum, a putative transdiagnostic vulnerability related to substance use, interpersonal discord, and psychopathology. Hostility is defined by a dysfunctional pattern of cognition – one that is prone to negative, cynical, and denigrating thoughts and feelings, along with antagonistic attitudes toward and in evaluation of others (e.g., viewing others as distrustful and purposefully hurtful; Berkowitz, Reference Berkowitz1993; Buss, Reference Buss1961; Miller, Smith, Turner, Guijarro, & Hallet, Reference Miller, Smith, Turner, Guijarro and Hallet1996). This oppositional cognitive bias abets intense and persistent anger and resentment, and may incite aggressive behavior (e.g., Eckhardt, Norlander, & Deffenbacher, Reference Eckhardt, Norlander and Deffenbacher2004; Houston & Vavak, Reference Houston and Vavak1991; Ramirez & Andreu, Reference Ramirez and Andreu2006). Among women, high levels of hostility have been linked to increased rates of depression, stress, and antisocial behavior (Eiden et al., Reference Eiden, Leonard, Colder, Homish, Schuetze and Huestis2011; Sellers et al., Reference Sellers, Harold, Elam, Rhoades, Potter, Mars and Collishaw2014), as well as substance use disorders (Robinson, Brower, & Gomberg, Reference Robinson, Brower and Gomberg2001). Further, mothers who use illicit substances and are high in hostility are more likely to engage in harsh and insensitive parenting practices compared to their peers (Eiden, Peterson, & Coleman, Reference Eiden, Peterson and Coleman1999; Schuetze et al., Reference Schuetze, Eiden and Dombkowski2006). This pattern of parenting among mothers prone to hostility has, in turn, been linked to more behavior problems in their children (Rubin, Burgess, Dwyer, & Hastings, Reference Rubin, Burgess, Dwyer and Hastings2003; Schuetze, Lopez, Granger, & Eiden, Reference Schuetze, Lopez, Granger and Eiden2008).

Maternal use of marijuana and tobacco while pregnant

Smokers are more likely to report negative attitudes toward other people and frequent, intense bouts of anger and aggression compared to nonsmokers (Miller et al., Reference Miller, Smith, Turner, Guijarro and Hallet1996). Trait hostility is also a consistent predictor of smoking among both men and women (e.g., Whiteman, Fowkes, Deary, & Lee, Reference Whiteman, Fowkes, Deary and Lee1997) and associated with lower rates of abstinence after treatment for smoking cessation (Kahler, Strong, Niaura, & Brown, Reference Kahler, Strong, Niaura and Brown2004). In contrast to the consistent associations between hostility and smoking, the literature on the association between hostility and marijuana use is more mixed. Studies collecting daily data on marijuana use and mood symptoms report that marijuana use is often preceded by increases in negative affect (see Wycoff, Metrik, & Trull, Reference Wycoff, Metrik and Trull2018 for review). There are also reports of acute effects of marijuana use on hostility, with some studies reporting increases in hostility following marijuana use while others have reported general decreases in negative affect, including hostility (Wycoff et al., Reference Wycoff, Metrik and Trull2018). Little is known about the associations between co-use of tobacco and marijuana and changes in hostility over time.

In one of the few studies examining hostility among pregnant smokers, authors reported that maternal hostility prospectively predicted persistent smoking during pregnancy, even in the context of other mood disorder symptoms, such as depression (Eiden et al., Reference Eiden, Leonard, Colder, Homish, Schuetze and Huestis2011). This is particularly important since tobacco is one of the most commonly used substances in pregnancy, with rates as high as 23%, especially among young, low-income women, as noted in the Surgeon General's Report (U.S. Department of Health and Human Services (USDHHS), 2014). Tobacco in the form of cigarettes delivers significant amounts of chemical toxins to the fetus via the maternal bloodstream (USDHHS, 2014) and increases norepinephrine, dopamine, acetylcholine, and serotonin concentrations in the developing brain (Lichtensteiger, Ribary, Schlumpf, Odermatt, & Widmer, Reference Lichtensteiger, Ribary, Schlumpf, Odermatt and Widmer1988; Slotkin et al., Reference Slotkin, Skavicus, Card, Stadler, Levin and Seidler2015). There are robust causal linkages between prenatal tobacco exposure and infant morbidity and mortality, including risk for prematurity, low birthweight, and sudden infant death syndrome (USDHHS, 2014). Behaviorally, prenatal cigarette exposure increases risk for irritability and higher arousal in the early neonatal period (Stroud et al., Reference Stroud, Paster, Papandonatos, Niaura, Salisbury, Battle and Lester2009), problems with self-soothing and attention/orienting in the later neonatal period (Espy et al., Reference Espy, Fang, Johnson, Stopp, Wiebe and Respass2011; Stroud et al., Reference Stroud, Papandonatos, Salisbury, Phipps, Huestis, Niaura and Lester2016), disruptive behavior, including aggression, conduct disorder, and attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder (ADHD) diagnosis in later childhood (Clark, Espy, & Wakschlag, Reference Clark, Espy and Wakschlag2016; Estabrook et al., Reference Estabrook, Massey, Clark, Burns, Mustanski, Cook and Wakschlag2016; USDHHS, 2014), and substance use in adolescence (Scott-Goodwin, Puerto, & Moreno, Reference Scott-Goodwin, Puerto and Moreno2016; Slotkin, Reference Slotkin2008).

Although the literature on prenatal cigarette exposure is large, there are two major open issues worth noting. First, many studies have clear methodological shortcomings that limit interpretation. These include a large number of retrospective studies that have operationalized maternal smoking in pregnancy via single item and/or self-report measures, as well as studies that lack a chemically verified, demographically similar comparison group that has abstained from smoking (Molnar et al., Reference Molnar, Rancourt, Schlauch, Wen, Maiorana and Eiden2017). Perceived social stigma as well as possible legal ramifications may prevent a woman from disclosing her substance use while pregnant, making it difficult to obtain accurate information. While no approach is perfect, measuring a woman's substance use during pregnancy via multiple methods (e.g., self-report, saliva sampling) may increase the likelihood that we are characterizing her behavior accurately and reliably. Accurately characterizing prenatal substance use is particularly important when working with a high-risk sample of pregnant women (i.e., young, single, low-income) since the prevalence of smoking in pregnancy, as well as the developmental salience of these sociodemographic risks for child outcomes, tend to be higher.

Second, many studies to date have not considered prenatal use of other substances that may have additive or synergistic effects, such as cannabis. In a special issue devoted to co-use of tobacco and cannabis in pregnancy, De Genna, Stroud, and Eiden (Reference De Genna, Stroud and Eiden2019) noted that the literature on developmental outcomes is quite small despite rates of co-use ranging from 45% to approximately 85% among substance users, which are higher than the use of marijuana or tobacco alone (Chabarria et al., Reference Chabarria, Racusin, Antony, Kahr, Suter and Aagaard2016; Coleman-Cowger, Schauer, & Peters, Reference Coleman-Cowger, Schauer and Peters2017; El Marroun et al., Reference El Marroun, Hudziak, Tiemeier, Creemers, Steegers, Jaddoe and Huizink2011; Ko, Farr, Tong, Creanga, & Callaghan, Reference Ko, Farr, Tong, Creanga and Callaghan2015). Marijuana use during pregnancy is concerning given significant increases in the potency of the main psychoactive component of marijuana (i.e., delta-9-tetrahydrocannabinol or THC) since the 1990s (Mehmedic et al., Reference Mehmedic, Chandra, Slade, Denham, Foster, Patel and ElSohly2010) and the increasing perception among pregnant women that marijuana use in pregnancy is safe (Bayrampour, Zahradnik, Lisonkova, & Janssen, Reference Bayrampour, Zahradnik, Lisonkova and Janssen2019). Both tobacco and cannabis interfere with neurotransmitter levels, brain biochemistry, and brain morphology for a developing child (Downer, Gowran, & Campbell, Reference Downer, Gowran and Campbell2007; El Marroun et al., Reference El Marroun, Tiemeier, Franken, Jaddoe, van der Lugt, Verhulst and White2016; Scott-Goodwin et al., Reference Scott-Goodwin, Puerto and Moreno2016). Results from both preclinical and human studies indicate that nicotine and tetrahydrocannabinol – the psychoactive compounds in tobacco and cannabis – interfere with dopaminergic, serotonergic, and GABAergic systems (DiNieri et al., Reference DiNieri, Wang, Szutorisz, Spano, Kaur, Casaccia and Hurd2011; England et al., Reference England, Aagaard, Bloch, Conway, Cosgrove, Grana and Wakschlag2017; Morena, Patel, Bains, & Hill, Reference Morena, Patel, Bains and Hill2016), and alter neuronal development through their impact on nicotinic and cannabinoid receptors (see Bara, Ferland, Rompala, Szutorisz, & Hurd, Reference Bara, Ferland, Rompala, Szutorisz and Hurd2021 and England et al., Reference England, Aagaard, Bloch, Conway, Cosgrove, Grana and Wakschlag2017; for detailed reviews). Both substances cross the placenta and enter the fetal bloodstream (Little & VanBeveren, Reference Little and VanBeveren1996) and may have stronger additive or synergistic effects than either substance alone.

In a recent study of neonatal neurobehavior with repeated assessments across the first month of life, co-use was associated with nearly double the need for external soothing (as opposed to self-soothing) of newborns. Co-use also demonstrated 42%–75% stronger associations with lower neonatal attention and lethargy compared to tobacco exposure alone (Stroud et al., Reference Stroud, Papandonatos, McCallum, Kehoe, Salisbury and Huestis2018). Using data from the present sample, Eiden, Schuetze, Shisler, and Huestis (Reference Eiden, Schuetze, Shisler and Huestis2018a) found that co-use of tobacco and cannabis was also associated with higher infant autonomic dysregulation compared to tobacco alone. Finally, using data from the present sample, co-use has been associated with higher internalizing behaviors and more sleep problems among girls at preschool age (Eiden et al., Reference Eiden, Zhao, Casey, Schuetze, Shisler and Colder2018b) and blunted stress-reactivity patterns at school age (Eiden et al., Reference Eiden, Shisler, Granger, Schuetze, Colangelo and Huestis2020) compared to tobacco alone or non-exposed children. No studies, to our knowledge, have examined whether co-use of tobacco and marijuana impacts individual differences in temperamental reactivity, despite the fact that temperament is an early indicator of childhood psychopathology risk.

Temperament and RDoC

Among studies that have examined pre- and early postnatal predictors of temperament, a vast majority have focused on a single dimension of infant behavior, chiefly domains of negative affect (Van den Bergh et al., Reference Van den Bergh, van den Heuvel, Lahti, Braeken, de Rooij, Entringer and Schwab2017). Although examination of individual traits has its relative merits, this approach tends to undervalue the phenotypic heterogeneity inherent to aberrant and normative behavior that may be better captured by a constellation of traits. To this end, RDoC promotes the use of quantitative approaches that reduce complex data from multiple dimensions into homogenous phenotypes, which reflect a profile of traits related to psychological health and dysfunction (Cuthbert, Reference Cuthbert2014). This approach has rarely been applied to pediatric samples (see Karalunas et al., Reference Karalunas, Fair, Musser, Aykes, Iyer and Nigg2014 for an exception), limiting our understanding of the developmental trajectories of RDoC constructs and, by extension, the etiology of early childhood psychopathology.

In outlining the developmental aspect of RDoC, the NIMH recognizes that, “many areas of the child psychopathology literature (e.g., reward sensitivity, cognitive and emotional dysregulation, behavioral inhibition) serve as a more compatible model for a dimensionally based approach compared to the highly specified categories of adult psychopathology” (NIMH, 2018). Building on this definition, a young child's behavior may be characterized as an emergent property of multiple RDoC constructs, including, but not limited to, social communication (social processes system), acute threat and frustrative nonreward (negative valence systems), attention and cognitive control (cognitive system), and motor action (sensorimotor system). These RDoC constructs map onto core dimensions of temperament that together comprise a unique constellation of proclivities that define a young child's behavior – affect, regulation, attention, activity, and arousal (Shiner et al., Reference Shiner, Buss, McClowry, Putnam, Saudino and Zentner2012). We argue that temperament is well suited to serve as a link between extant developmental evidence and the RDoC framework, as temperament is a dimensionally based approach focused on the mechanisms that underlie the manifestation and change of numerous traits over time (see Ostlund, Myruski, Buss, & Pérez-Edgar, Reference Ostlund, Myruski, Buss and Pérez-Edgar2021 for further discussion).

While the constituent dimensions of temperament are largely agreed upon (Shiner et al., Reference Shiner, Buss, McClowry, Putnam, Saudino and Zentner2012), researchers still disagree on how individual differences in the constellation of a young child's traits ought to be characterized. Recent findings point to person-centered approaches, using analytical methods such as latent profile analysis (LPA), as one promising data-driven method for identify subgroups of infants who are phenotypically similar. Underlying this approach is the assumption that the complex interplay among multiple dimensionally based temperament-relevant behaviors gives rise to finite patterns of trait expression that are shared among subgroups of children. Classifying distinct, homogeneous groups of young children provides researchers (as well as parents) with a readily interpretable, holistic representation of multiple traits to better understand a specific child and their potential trajectory (e.g., Clauss, Avery, & Blackford, Reference Clauss, Avery and Blackford2015; Liu et al., Reference Liu, Bann, Lester, Tronick, Das, Lagasse and Bada2010).

Although the number of profiles tends to vary depending on sample characteristics (e.g., child's age, assessment via parental report versus behavioral observation), most researchers identify 3–5 temperament profiles in early childhood (Beekman et al., Reference Beekman, Neiderhiser, Buss, Loken, Moore, Leve and Reiss2015; Caspi & Silva, Reference Caspi and Silva1995; Gartstein et al., Reference Gartstein, Prokasky, Bell, Calkins, Bridgett, Braungart-Rieker and Seamon2017; Janson & Mathiesen, Reference Janson and Mathiesen2008; Komsi et al., Reference Komsi, Räikkönen, Pesonen, Heinonen, Keskivaara and Strandberg2006; Lin, Ostlund, Conradt, Lagasse, & Lester, Reference Lin, Ostlund, Conradt, Lagasse and Lester2018; Planalp & Goldsmith, Reference Planalp and Goldsmith2020; Putnam & Stifter, Reference Putnam and Stifter2005; Sanson et al., Reference Sanson, Letcher, Smart, Prior, Toumbourou and Oberklaid2009; Scott et al., Reference Scott, Lemery-Chalfant, Clifford, Tein, Stoll and Goldsmith2016). One commonly identified profile is characterized by above-average levels of activity and negative affect. This profile is highly heritable (Planalp & Goldsmith, Reference Planalp and Goldsmith2020; Scott et al., Reference Scott, Lemery-Chalfant, Clifford, Tein, Stoll and Goldsmith2016), stable across early childhood (Beekman et al., Reference Beekman, Neiderhiser, Buss, Loken, Moore, Leve and Reiss2015; Komsi et al., Reference Komsi, Räikkönen, Pesonen, Heinonen, Keskivaara and Strandberg2006; Van Den Akker, Deković, Prinzie, & Asscher, Reference Van Den Akker, Deković, Prinzie and Asscher2010), and predicts externalizing and, to a lesser extent, internalizing problems (Janson & Mathiesen, Reference Janson and Mathiesen2008; Sanson et al., Reference Sanson, Letcher, Smart, Prior, Toumbourou and Oberklaid2009; Van Den Akker et al., Reference Van Den Akker, Deković, Prinzie and Asscher2010). Due to inconsistencies in naming conventions, this profile has been referred to by various names including undercontrolled (Janson & Mathiesen, Reference Janson and Mathiesen2008; Komsi et al., Reference Komsi, Räikkönen, Pesonen, Heinonen, Keskivaara and Strandberg2006), high reactive (Prokasky et al., Reference Prokasky, Rudasill, Molfese, Putnam, Gartstein and Rothbart2017), reactive/inhibited (Sanson et al., Reference Sanson, Letcher, Smart, Prior, Toumbourou and Oberklaid2009), and active reactive (Beekman et al., Reference Beekman, Neiderhiser, Buss, Loken, Moore, Leve and Reiss2015).

A second often-identified profile is likewise characterized by high negative affect, but also includes below average levels of positive affect, activity, and regulatory capabilities (e.g., negative reactive, Beekman et al., Reference Beekman, Neiderhiser, Buss, Loken, Moore, Leve and Reiss2015; negative reactive, dysregulated, Lin et al., Reference Lin, Ostlund, Conradt, Lagasse and Lester2018; unregulated, Prokasky et al., Reference Prokasky, Rudasill, Molfese, Putnam, Gartstein and Rothbart2017; dysregulated, negative reactive, Scott et al., Reference Scott, Lemery-Chalfant, Clifford, Tein, Stoll and Goldsmith2016). A similar profile characterized specifically by high fear relative to other dimensions of negative affect has also been identified (Beekman et al., Reference Beekman, Neiderhiser, Buss, Loken, Moore, Leve and Reiss2015; Janson & Mathiesen, Reference Janson and Mathiesen2008; Putnam & Stifter, Reference Putnam and Stifter2005; Van Den Akker et al., Reference Van Den Akker, Deković, Prinzie and Asscher2010). These profiles predict childhood behavior problems as well (Lin et al., Reference Lin, Ostlund, Conradt, Lagasse and Lester2018; Stifter, Putnam, & Jahromi, Reference Stifter, Putnam and Jahromi2008), and are reminiscent of the behavioral inhibition typology identified by Kagan and colleagues (Garcia-Coll, Kagan, & Reznick, Reference Garcia-Coll, Kagan and Reznick1984; Kagan & Snidman, Reference Kagan and Snidman1991).

Two additional profiles are also commonly identified. One profile includes infants who exhibit high levels of positive affect and self-regulation (e.g., positive reactive, Beekman et al., Reference Beekman, Neiderhiser, Buss, Loken, Moore, Leve and Reiss2015; high positive affect, well regulated, Lin et al., Reference Lin, Ostlund, Conradt, Lagasse and Lester2018; well regulated, positive reactive, Scott et al., Reference Scott, Lemery-Chalfant, Clifford, Tein, Stoll and Goldsmith2016), and another includes infants who are average to slightly below average levels on all temperament dimensions (e.g., unremarkable, Janson & Mathiesen, Reference Janson and Mathiesen2008; moderately low reactive, moderately dysregulated, Lin et al., Reference Lin, Ostlund, Conradt, Lagasse and Lester2018; typical, Planalp & Goldsmith, Reference Planalp and Goldsmith2020; low/low, Putnam & Stifter, Reference Putnam and Stifter2005; average, Prokasky et al., Reference Prokasky, Rudasill, Molfese, Putnam, Gartstein and Rothbart2017). Together, these profiles offer a robust and reliable representation of infant behavior, and may serve to link a mother's transdiagnostic vulnerabilities to psychopathology risk in her offspring.

Present study

Our goals for the present study were threefold. First, we sought to characterize individual differences in the trajectory of maternal hostility from pregnancy to 16 months postpartum. Few studies have examined the trajectories of maternal emotionality during the transition to parenthood, with the majority of the published findings focused on clinical symptoms. Putnam and colleagues (Putnam et al., Reference Putnam, Wilcox, Robertson-Blackmore, Sharkey, Bergink and Munk-Olsen2017), for example, found that trajectories of depressive symptoms for 95% of women in their sample (N = 615) were best characterized as stable or decreasing from pregnancy to 5 years postpartum. In line with these longitudinal studies of clinical symptoms, we hypothesized that, on average, hostility would decrease linearly across time, although trajectories would differ between mothers. However, to our knowledge, no study to date has examined trajectories of maternal hostility from pregnancy through the first two years postnatal. Given the dearth of research on the topic, we consider this an exploratory hypothesis that may inform future research.

Second, we examined whether level (prenatal) and change (pregnancy to 16 months postnatal) in maternal hostility were predicted by prenatal substance use. We hypothesized that pregnant women who used both marijuana and tobacco would report higher levels of hostility concurrently and would maintain higher levels over time relative to pregnant women who used only tobacco prenatally, as well as women who did not use either substance. We considered this hypothesis to be exploratory given the dearth of research on trajectories of co-use in pregnancy and maternal emotionality.

Third, we tested the hypothesis that 16-month-old infants of mothers who reported higher hostility while pregnant and maintained higher levels over time would exhibit a dysregulated temperament phenotype. We predicted that four temperament profiles would be identified, consistent with prior research on similarly aged infants from high-risk backgrounds (Beekman et al., Reference Beekman, Neiderhiser, Buss, Loken, Moore, Leve and Reiss2015; Lin et al., Reference Lin, Ostlund, Conradt, Lagasse and Lester2018). We hypothesized that higher levels of prenatal hostility and larger increases in hostility across time would be associated with an infant's membership in a temperament profile characterized by above average levels of negative affect and below average levels of attention and inhibitory control (e.g., negative reactive, Beekman et al., Reference Beekman, Neiderhiser, Buss, Loken, Moore, Leve and Reiss2015; negative reactive, dysregulated, Lin et al., Reference Lin, Ostlund, Conradt, Lagasse and Lester2018). Consistent with prior research (Lin et al., Reference Lin, Ostlund, Conradt, Lagasse and Lester2018), we did not expect a mother's substance use while pregnant to be directly related to her infant's membership in a specific temperament profile. Given established links between prenatal substance use and maternal hostility, as well as between maternal hostility and child behavior, we explored whether maternal hostility mediated the association between a mother's substance use while pregnant and her infant's membership to a temperament profile.

Method

Procedure

All procedures for the current study were approved by the university's Institutional Review Board (see Eiden et al., Reference Eiden, Shisler, Granger, Schuetze, Colangelo and Huestis2020 for detail discussion of sample selection). Women came in to the laboratory during each trimester of pregnancy, with informed written consent collected at the first trimester visit. Mother–infant dyads returned for additional laboratory assessments at 2, 9, and 16 months postpartum.

Participants

Women in their first trimester of pregnancy were recruited from a local hospital at their first prenatal appointment. Women were eligible if they were < 20 weeks’ gestation, having a singleton birth, aged 18 years or older, using no illicit drugs other than cannabis, no heavy alcohol use (women drank < 4 drinks per occasion and did not average > 1 drink a day), and were able to complete the self-report screening form in English. Use of cigarettes, alcohol, and illicit substances (e.g., cannabis, methamphetamine, cocaine, opioids) were assessed via the Time Follow-Back Interview (TLFB; Sobell & Sobell, Reference Sobell and Sobell1992) and salivary assays at the end of each trimester. Newborn meconium was also tested to determine fetal exposure to cigarettes and a range of illicit substances. Given the initial goal of the study to examine effects of maternal prenatal tobacco use on developmental mechanisms and outcomes of their offspring, tobacco users were oversampled based on this initial screener so that the closest eligible nonsmoking woman (matched on maternal age and highest educational attainment) was recruited for every two smoking women. This allowed for a full range of light to heavy smokers, and accounted for the likely scenario of greater attrition among tobacco smokers.

A total of 258 mother–infant dyads participated in the 2-month appointment, at which time they were considered officially enrolled. One mother–infant dyad was excluded from analyses because infant meconium was positive for methamphetamine, two infants were excluded because they had hydrocephaly, two were excluded because of a later diagnosis of autism, one was excluded because of maternal binge drinking during pregnancy, and one additional participant was excluded due to low maternal cognitive functioning. Finally, four participants were excluded because they were assigned to the tobacco control group, but were smoking moderate amounts of marijuana during pregnancy, resulting in a final sample size of 247 (69 nonsmokers, 81 tobacco-only smokers, and 97 tobacco and marijuana smokers).

Demographic information was collected at the first trimester appointment. Maternal age ranged from 18 to 39 (M = 24.09, SD = 5.00), and mothers were 51% African-American, 31% Caucasian, 19% Hispanic, and 8% other or mixed race with several mothers reporting more than one race. Approximately 46% percent of women were married or living with their partner at the first prenatal appointment, 33% were in a relationship but not living with their partner, 20% were single, and 1% were divorced. Approximately 29% of women had less than a high-school education, 29% completed high school, 29% completed some college courses but did not earn a degree, 9% had a vocational degree or technical training degree, and 4% received a Bachelor's degree. Thus, the sample consisted of primarily young, unmarried, women of color with lower levels of educational attainment. Demographic information for the sample is presented in Table 1.

Table 1. Demographic information.

a Sociodemographic risk composite of maternal race, education, occupation, and partner status.

b Buss–Perry Aggression Scale (Buss & Perry, Reference Buss and Perry1992).

Measures

Maternal prenatal substance use

Maternal prenatal substance use was measured through multiple methods including self-reports and biological assays. We administered the TLFB (Sobell & Sobell, Reference Sobell and Sobell1992) toward the end of each trimester, which yielded daily data regarding maternal substance use. Mothers were provided with a calendar on which they identified approximate conception date as well as important events (e.g., holidays, birthdays, parties, sports events, anniversaries, funerals, vacations) as anchor points to aid recall. The TLFB is a reliable and valid method of obtaining daily data on patterns of substance use, including tobacco and cannabis (Robinson, Sobell, Sobell, & Leo, Reference Robinson, Sobell, Sobell and Leo2014), has good test–retest reliability, and is highly correlated with other intensive self-report substance use measures (Brown et al., Reference Brown, Burgess, Sales, Whiteley, Evans and Miller1998). The TLFB yielded data on the average number of cigarettes and joints smoked per day across the entire pregnancy, as well as the average number of standard alcoholic drinks per day across pregnancy. All mothers smoked combustible cigarettes and none were e-cigarette users.

In addition to maternal self-reports, maternal oral fluid samples were collected at each trimester and analyzed by the US Drug Testing Laboratory (Des Plaines, IL) for cotinine, the primary nicotine biomarker, and for THC, the primary psychoactive component of cannabis. Cotinine assays were conducted with enzyme-linked immunosorbent assay (ELISA) or liquid chromatography-tandem mass spectrometry (LC-MS/MS) at 10 ng/mL cutoff, and ranged from 0 to 569 ng/mL. Assays for THC were conducted with immunoassay screening (4.0 μg/L cutoff) and GC–MS confirmation (4.0 μg/L cutoff). Infant meconium samples were collected across several days after delivery until the appearance of milk stool, and were assayed with a validated LC–MSMS method (Gray et al., Reference Gray, Eiden, Leonard, Connors, Shisler and Huestis2010a) at 2.5 ng/g nicotine, 1 ng/g cotinine, and 5 ng/g OHCOT, and with a validated two-dimensional GC–MS analytical method for THC, 11-hydroxy-THC; 8,11-dihydroxy-THC; 11-nor-9-carboxy-THC (THCCOOH), and cannabinol (Gray et al., Reference Gray, Eiden, Leonard, Connors, Shisler and Huestis2010b). Limits of quantification for cannabinoid meconium assays were 10 ng/g for all analytes, except 11-hydroxy-THC at 15 ng/g.

Mothers were assigned to the tobacco smoking group if they self-reported smoking during pregnancy on the screener or the TLFB, if oral fluid samples were cotinine positive, or if infant meconium was positive for cotinine, nicotine, or trans-3′ hydroxycotinine (OHCOT). Mothers were assigned to the co-exposed group (tobacco and marijuana smokers) if, in addition to meeting the criteria for the tobacco group, they also self-reported cannabis use during pregnancy, or if infant meconium was positive for cannabinoids, or if maternal oral fluid was positive for Δ9-THC in any of the three trimesters. Mothers were assigned to the control group if all of the above criteria were negative each trimester during pregnancy.

Sociodemographic risk

Sociodemographic risk was calculated as a composite of maternal race, education, occupation, and partner status. For all items, a higher score was indicative of greater risk. The maternal race variable acted as a proxy for structural barriers and bias faced by individuals from marginalized groups. Risk was coded as positive (1) if mothers indicated that they were nonwhite (69% met this criterion). For maternal education, risk was positive (1) if the participant had not received a high school diploma or equivalent (29.1% met this criterion). Maternal occupation was coded using the Hollingshead scale (M = 2.06, SD = 1.60, Range = 1–8). The score was then divided by the maximum value of 9 in order to create a proportion, and was then recoded so that higher numbers indicated greater risk (lower occupational status). For partner status, risk was positive (1) if the participant was not married or living with a partner (54.7% met this criterion). The final sociodemographic cumulative risk variable was created by averaging the four items described above, with a possible maximum score of 1 (M = 0.49, SD = 0.25, range = 0.04–0.89).

Maternal pre-/postnatal hostility

Maternal hostility was self-reported in the third trimester (M = 2.71, SD = 0.72), and at child age of 2 (M = 2.48, SD = 0.71), 9 (M = 2.36, SD = 0.65), and 16 months (M = 2.41, SD = 0.70) using the Buss-Perry Aggression Questionnaire (Buss & Perry, Reference Buss and Perry1992), which contains 29 items measuring anger and hostility (e.g., ‘I have become so mad that I have broken things’). Items are measured on a 5-point scale ranging from 1 (extremely uncharacteristic of me) to 5 (extremely characteristic of me), thus higher scores reflect more hostility. Internal consistency was excellent across all time points, with Cronbach's α ranging from .91 to .92.

Infant temperament

Infant temperament was assessed via maternal report of the Toddler Behavior Assessment Questionnaire (TBAQ; Goldsmith, Reference Goldsmith1996) at the 16-month appointment. The TBAQ consists of 120 items measuring 11 different domains of temperament. Each item is rated on a Likert-type scale ranging from 1 (never) to 7 (always). Subscales include activity level, anger, interest, object fear, pleasure, sadness, social fear, and soothability (10 items each), as well as perceptual sensitivity (11 items), inhibitory control (13 items), and appropriate attentional allocation (16 items). Internal consistency was good for most subscales (αs ranging from .71 to .79). Reliability was lower for interest (α = .69), sadness (α = .65), social fear (α = .58), activity level (α = .58), inhibitory control (α = .54), and attention (α = .53) subscales. Descriptive information and correlations among temperament dimensions are reported in Table 2.

Table 2. Descriptive information and correlations among temperament dimensions.

Note: * p < .05, ** p < .01, *** p < .001. “Struggle” = behavioral reactivity based on observational assessment of an arm restraint task (Goldsmith & Rothbart, Reference Goldsmith and Rothbart1999). Scores on all temperament dimensions excluding “Struggle” were calculated based on maternal report of infant behavior at 16-months of age via the Toddler Behavior Assessment Questionnaire (TBAQ; Goldsmith, Reference Goldsmith1996). Scores for TBAQ subscales were not calculated if subscale was missing 50% of their respective items.

Infant behavioral reactivity was assessed via a standardized paradigm intended to elicit anger and frustration (Goldsmith & Rothbart, Reference Goldsmith and Rothbart1999). In this paradigm, infants were allowed to play with an attractive toy for 30 s while seated in a high chair. The caregiver was seated next to the child, but was asked not to engage with the child during the assessment. Once the child was engaged in the toy, a research assistant standing behind the high chair placed her hands on the child's forearms, moved them to the child's sides, and held them there for 30 s, while maintaining a neutral expression. After the first trial, the research assistant engaged the child with the toy for another 30 s followed by a second 30-s trial of arm restraint. The session was stopped (N = 5) if the child reached a maximum distress code, defined as the child reaching the highest intensity of negative affect of a full cry, or at the caregiver's request. Following the Laboratory Temperament Assessment Battery (LabTAB) manual (Goldsmith & Rothbart, Reference Goldsmith and Rothbart1999), these children were assigned the maximum distress code for the remainder of the session. The child was allowed to play with the toy at the end of the two trials.

Both trials were coded in six 5-s epochs. Intensity of anger and intensity of struggle were the primary measures used in the current analyses. For each 5-s epoch, the peak intensity of anger was scored using Affex (Izard, Dougherty, & Hembree, Reference Izard, Dougherty and Hembree1983), adapted from Izard's Maximally Discriminative Facial Movement Coding System (1979). Both peak intensity of anger and struggle were coded on a scale ranging from 0 to 3, with higher scores indicating higher intensity of anger and struggle. The affect expression scores for each 5-s epoch were averaged for each trial in order to create two composite scores for each trial, reflecting average intensity of anger and sadness for Trial 1 and Trial 2. Higher scores indicated higher arousal or reactivity. Two coders blind to all information about the families coded behavioral reactivity. Interrater reliability was calculated for 10% of the tapes with intra-class correlation coefficients ranging from .91 to .96.

Infant behavior problems and competences

Infant behavior problems and competence were assessed at the 16-month appointment via maternal report of the Brief Infant-Toddler Social and Emotional Assessment (BITSEA; Carter & Briggs-Gowan, Reference Carter and Briggs-Gowan2005). The BITSEA is a 42-item scale with answers ranging from 0 (not true/rarely) to 2 (very true/ often). For the current study, the problem subscale (31 items, e.g., “Cries or has tantrums until he or she is exhausted.”) and competence subscale (11 items, e.g., “Tries to help when someone is hurt (e.g., gives a toy).”) were used. The problem subscale reflects maladaptive patterns of child behavior (e.g., age-inappropriate impulsivity, defiance, or aggressiveness), whereas the competence subscale is viewed as reflecting age-appropriate social–emotional skills, such as compliance, attention regulation, emotional awareness, and prosocial peer behavior (Carter, Briggs-Gowan, Jones, & Little, Reference Carter, Briggs-Gowan, Jones and Little2003). Scores on the problem scale of ≥ 15 for boys and ≥ 13 for girls indicate children in the possible problem range, while competence scores of ≤ 14 for boys and girls suggests the possible deficit/delay range. Internal consistency of the scale was good for the problem scale (α = .83) and lower for the competence scale (α = .67).

Analytic strategy

Data analysis was conducted in R v4.0.2 (R Core Team, 2020). We first estimated latent profiles of infant temperament using standardized scores for the arm restraint and TBAQ using the tidyLPA package (Rosenberg, Beymer, Anderson, van Lissa, & Schmidt, Reference Rosenberg, Beymer, Anderson, van Lissa and Schmidt2018). Missing data were handled using missForest (Stekhoven & Bühlmann, Reference Stekhoven and Bühlmann2012), an iterative machine learning imputation method (Breiman, Reference Breiman2001; Tang & Ishwaran, Reference Tang and Ishwaran2017). The best fitting model was determined based on the following criteria: a small Bayesian information criteria (BIC), indicating better relative model fit (Schwarz, Reference Schwarz1978), a large entropy, indicating higher confidence in classification (Celeux & Soromenho, Reference Celeux and Soromenho1996), a minimum of 25 infants as members of the smallest profile (Lubke & Neale, Reference Lubke and Neale2006), and interpretable profile solutions. Infant membership to a temperamental profile from the best fitting class solution was dichotomized (yes/no) in relation to a reference group and included in subsequent analyses. We examined whether infant temperament profiles were associated with key health and demographic variables, including maternal age at recruitment, maternal relationship status, maternal education, infant sex, gestational age, birth weight, and whether the infant was born small for gestational age (i.e., age-adjusted birth weight below tenth percentile). Variables that were associated with temperament profiles were included as covariates in the path analysis (Maxwell, Delaney, & Kelley, Reference Maxwell, Delaney and Kelley2018).

Next, we fit an unconditional latent growth curve model to examine level and change in maternal hostility from pregnancy to 16 months postnatal. Analyses were conducted using maximum likelihood estimation with robust standard errors (MLR) and full information maximum likelihood (FIML) in the lavaan package (Rosseel, Reference Rosseel2012). Paths to observed variables were set to 1 for the latent intercept variable. For the latent slope variable, paths to observed variables were set to 0, 1, 2.75, and 4.50, to reflect the number of months from the prenatal assessment divided by a constant (4).

The unconditional latent growth curve model was then used to assess whether a mother's substance use prenatally, as well as her hostility while pregnant and from pregnancy to 16 months postnatal, predicted individual differences in her infant's temperament (i.e., conditional latent growth curve model). A mother's prenatal substance use was effects coded, yielding two comparisons: (a) pregnant women who used both marijuana and tobacco or tobacco only were compared to pregnant women who did not use substances prenatally, and (b) pregnant women who used tobacco but not marijuana were compared to women who used both marijuana and tobacco. Comparison 1 examined the effect of prenatal substance use (vs. no use) on infant outcomes, while Comparison 2 assessed differences between co-use and use of tobacco alone. Model fit was considered good if the following criteria were met: a comparative fit index (CFI) > 0.95, a root mean square error of approximation (RMSEA) < 0.06, a standardized root mean square residual (SRMR) < 0.08, and a nonsignificant χ 2 test (Hu & Bentler, Reference Hu and Bentler1999). We tested the significance of the latent growth parameters as mediators of the proposed link between prenatal maternal substance use and toddler temperament profiles within the same path model based on 10,000 bootstrapped samples with 95% bias-corrected confidence intervals (CIs). Finally, we examined post hoc comparisons between temperament profiles and the problem behavior and competences scales of the BITSEA to aid interpretation of these person-centered variables.

Results

Temperament profiles

Descriptive data and bivariate correlations among infant temperament dimensions assessed at 16 months of age are presented in Table 2. Model fit indices, entropy, and sample size for a solution's smallest profile are presented in Table 3. The BIC was lowest for the four-class solutions. The four- and five-class solutions had the largest entropy values. Given that it had the lowest BIC, a large entropy, and interpretable and adequately sized profiles, the four-class solution was deemed the most parsimonious. Temperament profiles from the four-class solution were used in all subsequent analyses and are presented in Figure 1 (see Supplementary Information for density plots of each temperament dimension based on profile membership).

Figure 1. Temperament profiles at 16 months of age. Mean standardized scores on temperament dimension from the Toddler Behavior Assessment Questionnaire (Goldsmith, Reference Goldsmith1996) and an arm restraint task (Goldsmith & Rothbart, Reference Goldsmith and Rothbart1999) are presented. “Struggle” = behavioral reactivity to an arm restraint task at 16 months of age. “Obj. fear” = object fear. “Inh. control” = inhibitory control. “Perc. sensitivity” = perceptual sensitivity.

Table 3. Summary of fit statistics for latent class analysis of infant temperament.

Note: N = 207.

BIC = Bayesian information criteria

High reactive

The first profile consisted of 28% of the sample (n = 57; 27 females, 30 males). This profile was labeled high reactive because it comprised infants with higher levels of anger, sadness, and object fear, in combination with low levels of soothability and inhibitory control. In addition, these infants were characterized by above average perceptual sensitivity.

Low reactive

The second profile described the plurality of infants and included 40% of the sample (n = 83; 38 females, 45 males). This group was named low reactive because these infants struggled the least in response to the arm restraint task and had above average inhibitory control. Further, these infants had moderately below average scores across the majority of domains, including interest, pleasure, anger, sadness, and perceptual sensitivity.

Well regulated

The third profile included approximately 16% of the sample (n = 32; 18 females, 14 males). This group was labeled well regulated because these infants were characterized by low levels of negative affect – including low anger, sadness, and object and social fear – as well as high levels of inhibitory control and soothability. These infants also had lower activity levels and above average attention, interest, and pleasure. This profile was included as the reference condition in all models.

Dysregulated

Lastly, the fourth profile comprised 17% of the sample (n = 35; 18 females, 17 males). This group was named dysregulated due to having above average levels of anger and sadness in combination with below average levels of inhibitory control. These infants were additionally characterized by high activity levels and below average attention and interest.

Maternal hostility

Self-reported hostility for each mother from the third trimester of pregnancy to 16 months postnatal is presented in Figure 2.

Figure 2. Trajectories of self-reported maternal hostility from the third trimester of pregnancy to 16 months postnatal. Data from each mother across time are represented by a unique color (a); the solid red line indicates linear fit. Data from a random 10% of mothers who had data at each time point are presented in black to exemplify maternal trajectories (b); the remainder of the sample is presented in gray.

Repeated measures analysis of variance (ANOVA)

Results from a one-way repeated measures ANOVA showed that maternal hostility significantly differed across time points, although the effect size was small, F(2.88, 486.38) = 19.65, p < .001, η 2g = 0.03. Pairwise comparisons (Benjamini–Hochberg adjusted; Benjamini & Hochberg, Reference Benjamini and Hochberg1995) revealed that maternal hostility while pregnant was significantly higher than hostility at each time point (ps < .001). Maternal hostility at 2 months postnatal was significantly higher than hostility at 9 months postnatal (p = .03), and marginally higher than hostility at 16 months postnatal (p = .07). Maternal hostility at 9 and 16 months postnatal did not significantly differ (p = .40).

Latent growth curve models

The latent growth curve model adequately fit the data; χ2(5) = 32.47, p < .001, CFI = 0.94, RMSEA = 0.15, SRMR = 0.07. The mean level of hostility in the third trimester of pregnancy (intercept) was 2.59. The variance of the latent intercept mean was significant (z = 8.61, p < .001), indicating that women differed in their level of hostility while pregnant. The mean slope for hostility was statistically significant and negative (b = −0.06, p < .001), indicating that, on average, maternal hostility decreased from the third trimester of pregnancy to 16 months postnatal. Trajectories of hostility did not significantly differ between mothers (z = 0.53; p = .59). The latent intercept and slope variables were not correlated (β = −0.05, p = .88).

Path models

Prenatal substance use and prenatal sociodemographic risk were included as predictors of maternal and infant variables (Figure 3). The model fit the data adequately; χ2(20) = 61.95, p < .001, CFI = 0.94, RMSEA = 0.09, SRMR = 0.05. We chose the well-regulated profile as the reference group and excluded it from subsequent analyses based on prior findings among infants with prenatal substance exposure (Lin et al., Reference Lin, Ostlund, Conradt, Lagasse and Lester2018), and the fact that this profile was characterized by low negative affect and high attention, soothability, and inhibitory control (Figure 1). Associations with temperament profiles were therefore interpreted relative to the well-regulated profile.

Figure 3. Final path model with standardized path coefficients. Solid lines reflect significant associations. A mothers’ prenatal substance use was effects coded, yielding two comparisons: (a) pregnant women who used both marijuana and tobacco or tobacco only (“THC + Tob. & Tob. only”) were compared to pregnant women who did not use either substance (“none”); and (b) pregnant women who used tobacco but not marijuana (“tobacco only”) were compared to women who used both marijuana and tobacco (“THC + Tob.”). Time coding for the latent growth variables reflects the number of months from the prenatal assessment divided by a constant (4). Temperament profiles were dummy coded; the well-regulated profile served as the reference group for all comparisons.

Results indicate that, relative to pregnant women who did not use substances, women who used both marijuana and tobacco reported higher levels of hostility while pregnant (p < .001) and exhibited less change in hostility over time (p = .03). Infants of women who reported higher levels of hostility while pregnant were more likely to be classified in the high reactive (p = .03) and dysregulated (p = .01) profiles. No other paths were significant (ps > .11). The model that included infant birth weight – the only covariate significantly associated with a temperament profile (low reactive) in multinomial regression analyses (all other ps > .19 relative to well-regulated reference group) – also fit the data adequately; χ2(22) = 54.85, p = .001, CFI = 0.95, RMSEA = 0.08, SRMR = 0.04. Results of this model indicate that, relative to pregnant women who did not use substances, women who used both marijuana and tobacco reported higher levels of hostility while pregnant (p < .001) and exhibited less change in hostility over time (p = .05). Moreover, infants of women who reported higher levels of hostility while pregnant were more likely to be classified in the dysregulated profile (p = .04), relative to the well-regulated profile. No other paths were significant (ps > .16). We tested whether maternal hostility while pregnant mediated the link between prenatal substance use and infant membership in either the high reactive or dysregulated temperament profiles. Neither the path to the high reactive profile (95% CI [−0.004, 0.026]) nor the dysregulated profile (95% CI [−0.005, 0.034]) was significant.

Post hoc analyses

To establish criterion validity and aid interpretation, we conducted post hoc comparisons among the observed temperament profiles and the problem behavior (N = 200, M = 12.50, SD = 6.91) and competence (N = 201, M = 16.31, SD = 3.11) scales of the BITSEA (Carter & Briggs-Gowan, Reference Carter and Briggs-Gowan2005). Problem behavior and competence scores were not correlated, r(200) = −.06, p = .42. One competence score from an infant in the high reactive profile was identified as an outlier; results remained the same when this infant was excluded from analyses (results available upon request). Problem behavior and competence scores were still not correlated, r(199) = −.05, p = .50.

Using a one-way ANOVA, we found that infant's problem behavior significantly differed based on temperament profile membership, F(3,196) = 15.49, p < 001 (Figure 4). Tukey post hoc comparisons showed that problem behavior was higher for infants in the high reactive profile (M = 16.52, SD = 7.49) relative to both the low reactive (M = 10.87, SD = 5.78) and well-regulated (M = 7.98, SD = 5.23) profiles (ps < .001). Problem behavior was also higher for infants in the dysregulated profile (M = 13.76, SD = 5.96) relative to the well-regulated profile (p = .001), but not relative to the low reactive profile (p = .11). Problem behavior did not differ between infants in the low reactive and well-regulated profiles (p = .13), or between infants in the dysregulated and high reactive profiles (p = .17).

Figure 4. Post hoc comparisons among infant temperament profiles and the (A) problem behavior and (B) competence scales of the Brief Infant-Toddler Social and Emotional Assessment (BITSEA) (Carter & Briggs-Gowan, Reference Carter and Briggs-Gowan2005).

Similarly, we found that infant's competences significantly differed based on temperament profile membership, F(3,197) = 9.70, p < .001 (Figure 4). Tukey post hoc comparisons showed that competence was higher for infants in the well-regulated profile (M = 18.16, SD = 2.68) relative to the dysregulated (M = 15.51, SD = 2.91) and low reactive (M = 15.31, SD = 2.81) profiles (ps < .003). Competence was also higher for infants in the high reactive profile (M = 17.14, SD = 3.20) relative to the low reactive profile (p = .002) as well as the dysregulated profile (p = .05). There was no significant difference between the dysregulated and low reactive profiles (p = .99), nor between the well-regulated and high reactive profiles (p = .40).

Lastly, we examined whether a mother's level of hostility was correlated with her 16-month-old infant's scores on the BITSEA. Infant problem behavior was positively associated with maternal hostility during pregnancy (r = .16, p = .03) and at 2- (r = .23, p = .001), 9- (r = .27, p < .001), and 16- (r = .27, p < .001) months postnatally. Infant competence was marginally associated with maternal hostility at 9 months postnatal (r = −0.13, p = .07); otherwise, these variables were not significantly correlated (p > .21).

Discussion

Using a prospective longitudinal design, we examined the pre- and postnatal influence of a transdiagnostic maternal vulnerability on infant temperament, specifically within the context of varying levels of substance use exposure in utero. Our study adds to extant evidence by showing that maternal hostility (a) decreases from the third trimester of pregnancy to 16 months postpartum, (b) positively associates with co-occurring marijuana and tobacco use while pregnant, and (c) predicts membership to two behaviorally dysregulated temperament profiles in their infant. This effect was specific to the prenatal period: maternal hostility while pregnant predicted specific infant temperament profiles above and beyond the influence of postnatal hostility. These temperament profiles (high reactive and dysregulated) are potential intermediate risk phenotypes on the path to childhood psychopathology. Our results underscore the utility of transdiagnostic vulnerabilities, particularly while pregnant, in understanding the intergenerational transmission of psychopathology risk.

RDoC highlights intermediate phenotypes of psychological health and dysfunction, encouraging the use of quantitative approaches that reduce data from multiple dimensions to identify individuals who are phenotypically alike. Consistent with this recommendation, we used a person-centered approach and observed four distinct, homogenous groups of infants based on twelve dimensions of temperament. These profiles mirror groups identified by multiple other researchers who have used similar analytic approaches (Beekman et al., Reference Beekman, Neiderhiser, Buss, Loken, Moore, Leve and Reiss2015; Gartstein et al., Reference Gartstein, Prokasky, Bell, Calkins, Bridgett, Braungart-Rieker and Seamon2017; Janson & Mathiesen, Reference Janson and Mathiesen2008; Komsi et al., Reference Komsi, Räikkönen, Pesonen, Heinonen, Keskivaara and Strandberg2006; Planalp & Goldsmith, Reference Planalp and Goldsmith2020; Prokasky et al., Reference Prokasky, Rudasill, Molfese, Putnam, Gartstein and Rothbart2017; Putnam & Stifter, Reference Putnam and Stifter2005; Sanson et al., Reference Sanson, Letcher, Smart, Prior, Toumbourou and Oberklaid2009; Scott et al., Reference Scott, Lemery-Chalfant, Clifford, Tein, Stoll and Goldsmith2016; Van Den Akker et al., Reference Van Den Akker, Deković, Prinzie and Asscher2010), although not all studies identified the same four groups in their analyses (see Ostlund et al., Reference Ostlund, Myruski, Buss and Pérez-Edgar2021 for discussion).

Most notably, three of the observed temperament profiles – high reactive, low reactive, and well regulated – resemble groups identified in two different samples reported on by Lin et al. (Reference Lin, Ostlund, Conradt, Lagasse and Lester2018), the only other study to our knowledge that has examined temperament profiles in infants who were prenatally exposed to substances. Pregnant women in each sample examined by Lin and colleagues had varying levels of polysubstance use, including smoking (53%–54%) and marijuana use (18%–23%), despite being recruited for either cocaine (Maternal Lifestyle Study; Lester et al., Reference Lester, ElSohly, Wright, Smeriglio, Verter, Bauer and Maza2001) or methamphetamine (Infant Development, Environment, and Lifestyle study; Smith et al., Reference Smith, LaGasse, Derauf, Grant, Shah, Arria and Lester2006) use. The replicability of these profiles is particularly impressive given that prior studies have utilized a variety of temperament measures (e.g., Lin et al., Reference Lin, Ostlund, Conradt, Lagasse and Lester2018; Planalp & Goldsmith, Reference Planalp and Goldsmith2020) with children of various ages (e.g., Beekman et al., Reference Beekman, Neiderhiser, Buss, Loken, Moore, Leve and Reiss2015; Scott et al., Reference Scott, Lemery-Chalfant, Clifford, Tein, Stoll and Goldsmith2016), lending support for the utility of these temperament profiles in describing phenotypically similar children beginning in the first year of life. Incorporating evidence from temperament research may serve to expand RDoC by providing an established theoretical perspective on developmental change, and should be taken into consideration going forward (Ostlund et al., Reference Ostlund, Myruski, Buss and Pérez-Edgar2021).

The observed profiles characterize variation on dimensions of temperament that reflect functioning along continua of multiple RDoC constructs. In our sample, activity (sensorimotor systems), anger and sadness (negative valance systems), attention, perceptual sensitivity, and inhibitory control (cognitive systems) were influential in determining an infant's membership to a specific profile. However, when identifying phenotypes that portend childhood psychological health and dysfunction, the whole may be greater than the sum of its parts. That is, it may be the confluence of multiple RDoC constructs (characterized by temperament traits) that gives rise to individual differences in infant behavior.

For instance, given low levels of negative affect (fear, anger, sadness), high self- and co-regulatory capacities (soothability, inhibitory control, attention), high level of engagement and enjoyment in activities such as solitary play (interest, pleasure), we would expect infants in the well-regulated profile to show the most adaptive social and emotional outcomes. Post hoc analyses support this prediction, showing that infants in this profile are reported to have the highest levels of competence concurrently. Inhibitory control and attentional capacities modulate negative affect, become increasingly employed as a young child develops, and are used together in service of goal-directed behavior (Braungart-Rieker, Hill-Soderlund, & Karrass, Reference Braungart-Rieker, Hill-Soderlund and Karrass2010; Morales, Fu, & Pérez-Edgar, Reference Morales, Fu and Pérez-Edgar2016). In this way, an infant's behavior may be considered as an emergent property of multiple RDoC constructs that becomes increasingly intertwined across early development and may be reliably captured via person-centered temperament profiles. By measuring multiple RDoC constructs, person-centered approaches allow us to pinpoint strengths and weaknesses among homogeneous groups of infants.

It is also worth considering the caregiving environment in which an infant's temperamental proclivities may be displayed in early childhood. Indeed, infants were more likely to be classified in the well-regulated profile, relative to the high reactive or dysregulated profiles, if their mothers reported lower levels of hostility while pregnant. Given the negative association between maternal levels of hostility and sensitive parenting (Eiden et al., Reference Eiden, Peterson and Coleman1999; Schuetze et al., Reference Schuetze, Eiden and Dombkowski2006), it follows that these well-regulated infants may experience a more supportive, nurturing environments relative to their peers. While genetic influences between maternal hostility and infant temperament cannot be ruled out, it is worth noting that these infants may ultimately have the most adaptive outcomes by virtue of a good “match” between their dispositional traits and their parents’ proclivity toward sensitive parenting (Thomas & Chess, Reference Thomas and Chess1977).

Relative to the well-regulated profile, infants were more likely to be classified in the high reactive or dysregulated profiles if their mothers reported higher levels of hostility while pregnant. Both of these temperament profiles were related to higher levels of problem behavior concurrently. A core feature of each of these profiles is anger, suggesting homotypic continuity in the affective repertoire of a highly hostile pregnant women and her behaviorally dysregulated infant. Perra, Paine, and Hay (Reference Perra, Paine and Hay2020) identified subgroups of infants at differential risk for externalizing psychopathology at age 7 years based on early levels of anger-aggressiveness (see also Liu et al., Reference Liu, Moore, Beekman, Pérez-Edgar, Leve, Shaw and Neiderhiser2018). While infants in the high anger/high aggressive group tended to maintain risk over time, the authors found that warm parenting was a protective factor that protected against the escalation in aggression over early development. On the other hand, socioeconomic adversity predicted membership in the high anger/high aggressive group, while a mother's own history of antisocial behavior predicted escalation in aggression over early development (Perra et al., Reference Perra, Paine and Hay2020).

Unfortunately, the caregiving environment of mothers who are high in hostility and use marijuana and tobacco prenatally tends to be characterized by insensitivity (Eiden et al., Reference Eiden, Peterson and Coleman1999; Schuetze et al., Reference Schuetze, Eiden and Dombkowski2006). To this end, coercion theory (Patterson, Debaryshe, & Ramsey, Reference Patterson, Debaryshe and Ramsey1989) posits that negative reinforcement patterns in the caregiver–child relationship contribute to the development of psychopathology when paired with insensitive and inconsistent caregiving. Over time, an infant's temperament may transact with their early caregiving environment to increase (or decrease) psychopathology risk (e.g., NICHD Early Child Care Research Network, 2004; Shaw, Lacourse, & Nagin, Reference Shaw, Lacourse and Nagin2005). These temperamental differences may therefore affect the dynamic interplay between the young child and their early environment, which may lay the foundation for subsequent psychopathology risk throughout development (Crowell, Puzia, & Yaptangco, Reference Crowell, Puzia and Yaptangco2015).

The two behaviorally dysregulated temperament profiles diverge, however, when fearfulness, perceptual sensitivity, and regulatory capacities are considered. Specifically, the constellation of features that define infants in the dysregulated and high reactive profile correspond to early behavioral indices of trait irritability (Beauchaine et al., Reference Beauchaine, Zisner and Sauder2017; Leibenluft & Stoddard, Reference Leibenluft and Stoddard2015) and behavioral inhibition (Garcia-Coll et al., Reference Garcia-Coll, Kagan and Reznick1984; Kagan & Snidman, Reference Kagan and Snidman1991), respectively. Trait impulsivity is thought to be heritable, possibly via shared sensitivity in mesolimbic dopaminergic functioning (Beauchaine et al., Reference Beauchaine, Zisner and Sauder2017; Gatzke-Kopp, Reference Gatzke-Kopp2011). Indeed, Field and colleagues (Field et al., Reference Field, Diego, Hernandez-Reif, Salman, Schanberg, Kuhn and Bendell2002, Reference Field, Diego, Hernandez-Reif, Figueiredo, Deeds, Ascencio and Kuhn2008) noted that newborn level of peripheral dopamine is associated with maternal dopamine levels, and that dopamine levels are lower among newborns of mothers who reported high levels of anger. Elevated hostility while pregnant may further potentiate the sensitivity of mesolimbic dopaminergic functioning.

For an infant in the high reactive profile, the proclivity toward high perceptual sensitivity and negative reactivity may require greater co-regulatory support from their caregiver during distress recovery. The dyadic attunement needed for co-regulation is likely negatively impacted by both maternal hostility and maternal substance use, each of which are independently associated with lower levels of sensitivity (Eiden et al., Reference Eiden, Peterson and Coleman1999; Schuetze et al., Reference Schuetze, Eiden and Dombkowski2006). Based on the prior literature (Beauchaine et al., Reference Beauchaine, Zisner and Sauder2017; Clauss et al., Reference Clauss, Avery and Blackford2015), the divergence between the two profiles would suggest that infants in the dysregulated profile are at increased risk for irritability and externalizing problems, while high reactive infants are more likely to demonstrate social withdrawal and internalizing difficulties.

It is worth noting that the temperamental characteristics that define infants in the high reactive profile – perceptual sensitivity, attention, and negative affect – may also increase these infant's sensitivity to both positive and negative aspects of the early caregiving environment (Ellis, Boyce, Belsky, Bakermans-Kranenburg, & van Ijzendoorn, Reference Ellis, Boyce, Belsky, Bakermans-Kranenburg and van Ijzendoorn2011; Slagt, Semon, Deković, & van Aken, Reference Slagt, Semon, Deković and van Aken2016). Specifically, adaptive calibration models suggest that temperamentally reactive infants show an enhanced biological sensitivity to the environment and parental behavior, which in turn is a core conduit through which the environment “gets under the skin” to shape psychobiological development (Del Giudice, Ellis, & Shirtcliff, Reference Del Giudice, Ellis and Shirtcliff2011). Follow-up work will need to more closely capture the temporal dynamics of maternal hostility, maternal behavior, and infant profiles (e.g., latent transition analysis) in order to better disentangle the mechanisms influencing individual trajectories.

With this same motivation in mind, we worked to separate pre- and postnatal influences of maternal hostility on infant behavioral dysregulation by simultaneously considering level (prenatal) and change (pregnancy to 16 months postpartum). Maternal hostility during each of these periods likely operates via distinct mechanisms to affect the neural, physiological, and behavioral parameters of the young child. Prenatally, instances of acute hostility may, for example, be associated with a distinct physiological profile marked by altered sympathetic activity, which in turn may restrict uteroplacental blood flow to the fetus via catecholaminergic pathways (see Rakers et al., Reference Rakers, Bischoff, Schiffner, Haase, Rupprecht, Kiehntopf and Schwab2015 for animal model). Postnatally, any lasting biological shifts triggered by in utero exposure could be potentiated by the insensitive and noncontingent parenting behaviors previously associated with elevated hostility.

From pregnancy to 16 months postnatal, we observed a linear decrease in maternal hostility, a pattern that may reflect a regression to a homeostatic set point following normative biological and behavioral changes from pregnancy to postpartum (e.g., Hendrick, Altshuler, & Suri, Reference Hendrick, Altshuler and Suri1998). This shift may also reflect a mother's growing ease and confidence with the daily demands of child rearing. Thus, infants whose mothers did not display the typical decreasing trajectory may be particularly vulnerable to poor adaptive outcomes.

Of course, maternal hostility is not acting in a vacuum. Indeed, in the current sample, women who used both marijuana and tobacco reported higher levels of hostility while pregnant and exhibited less change in hostility over time. It is difficult to determine directionality when considering the association between maternal hostility and use of marijuana and/or tobacco prenatally, due to their high co-occurrence (Eiden et al., Reference Eiden, Leonard, Colder, Homish, Schuetze and Huestis2011). In addition, mothers in this sample were disproportionately exposed to a number of environmental and structural stressors that likely increased levels of hostility, the incidence of substance use, and when present, the differential impact of these factors on infants relative to mothers with greater structural supports (e.g., education, income, and partner status).

These broader stressors likely also impacted other known risk factors, such as sleep disturbances (e.g., Obeysekare et al., Reference Obeysekare, Cohen, Coles, Pearlstein, Monzon and Sharkey2020; Román-Gálvez et al., Reference Román-Gálvez, Amezcua-Prieto, Salcedo-Bellido, Martínez-Galiano, Kahn and Bueno-Cavanillas2018), relatively poor nutrition (e.g., DeCapo, Thompson, Dunn, & Sullivan, Reference DeCapo, Thompson, Dunn and Sullivan2019; Hurley, Caulfield, Sacco, Costigan, & DiPietro, Reference Hurley, Caulfield, Sacco, Costigan and DiPietro2005), and elevated levels of stress and distress (e.g., Walsh et al., Reference Walsh, Mccormack, Webster, Pinto, Lee, Feng and Monk2019). More systematic studies identifying how constellations of transdiagnostic vulnerabilities influence the development (or maintenance) of intermediate risk phenotypes in offspring may allow for more precise characterization of core dysfunctions for a mother and her infant that can be targeted by prevention efforts.

To note some additional limitations in the current study, we had multiple subscales of the TBAQ show relatively low Cronbach's alpha values. This may be attributable, in part, to the fact that this measure was not designed with a high-risk population in mind, and may have operated differently in the current sample. We discuss this limitation further in the Supplementary Information. Nevertheless, the observed temperament profiles mirror prior work with community samples (e.g., Gartstein et al., Reference Gartstein, Prokasky, Bell, Calkins, Bridgett, Braungart-Rieker and Seamon2017; Planalp & Goldsmith, Reference Planalp and Goldsmith2020), lending support to their replicability across risk status in early childhood. An additional limitation is that mothers reported on their own and their infant's behavior at each time point, which may inflate common-method variance. Acknowledging this potential shortcoming, we did include a behavioral observation variable (arm restraint) in the LPA. It is worth noting that behavioral reactivity to the arm restraint task had a modest influence on group differentiation (see Supplementary Figure 2). Nevertheless, recent findings suggest that a mother's mental health may not bias her report of infant temperament (Olino, Guerra-Guzman, Hayden, & Klein, Reference Olino, Guerra-Guzman, Hayden and Klein2020), as is commonly assumed. Future research may benefit from a broader assessment of behavioral data related to temperament as a complement to parent-reported data. Furthermore, the number of infants in each of the observed temperament profiles was relatively small, even though our findings correspond to profiles observed in two other samples of infants prenatally exposed to substances (Lin et al., Reference Lin, Ostlund, Conradt, Lagasse and Lester2018).

It is also worth noting that the current sample of pregnant women were recruited primarily for tobacco use and a subsample also used marijuana via smoking, which may limit the generalizability of our findings among pregnant women who use marijuana only and use marijuana without smoking (e.g., edible marijuana). Given that the way in which marijuana is used may vary based on geographic location and socioeconomic status in the United States (e.g., Borodovsky, Crosier, Lee, Sargent, & Budney, Reference Borodovsky, Crosier, Lee, Sargent and Budney2016), future research that includes a broader definition of marijuana use and marijuana use without co-use of tobacco is warranted. Moreover, in order to include ascertainment of substance use based on multiple indices, our analyses were limited to comparing groups of women who were categorized based on prenatal cigarette and marijuana use compared to non-use. Future research might consider a recruitment strategy that is not based on a case-control design and perhaps use other methods such as propensity score matching on demographics and use the dose–response measures of substance use in analyses. This approach would complement the current findings and provide insight into whether (and how) prenatal substance use relates to early childhood temperament in a dose-dependent fashion.

A few strengths of our study are worth highlighting. We prospectively assessed a sample of primarily young, low-income women and their infants from pregnancy to 16 months postpartum. We adopted a rigorous methodology for collecting longitudinal data, which included a detailed assessment of a woman's substance use while pregnant and the use of both parent-report and behavioral observation data to characterize infant temperament profiles. We also incorporated multiple statistical methods – latent growth curve modeling and LPA – to examine how prenatal hostility, as well as change in maternal hostility, are associated with an infant's temperament profile membership. It is worth noting that we only examined linear change in maternal hostility over time. Yet, an infant's development over the first 16 months of life is dramatic: a young child's emotional, cognitive, and motor repertoire quickly evolve from rudimentary and atomized to sophisticated and contingent, introducing an ever-changing set of challenges a parent must adapt to. Future research might consider examining dynamic change (e.g., quadratic, piecewise) beginning in early pregnancy and proceeding through the first 2 years of life.

Finally, our sample of families was racially diverse (51% African-American) as well as demographically and economically high-risk (predominately young, low income, and single) relative to many studies that examine pre- and early postnatal transdiagnostic vulnerabilities. Given the underrepresentation of diverse populations in psychological research (Gatzke-Kopp, Reference Gatzke-Kopp2016), and the disparate rates of perinatal morbidity and mortality among African-American women (Moaddab et al., Reference Moaddab, Dildy, Brown, Bateni, Belfort and Clark2018; Saluja & Bryant, Reference Saluja and Bryant2020; Somer, Sinkey, & Bryant, Reference Somer, Sinkey and Bryant2017), future research should build on our preliminary findings to further understand and disrupt transdiagnostic vulnerabilities for mothers and infants from historically underrepresented groups during the pre- and postnatal period.

Conclusion

Our findings advance our understanding of the intergenerational transmission of psychopathology risk in two important ways. First, we measured trajectories of a transdiagnostic vulnerability from pregnancy to 16 months postpartum, incorporating change into our conceptualization of pre- and early postnatal maternal risk. We showed that a mother's level of hostility while pregnant and across her infant's early development are related to use of marijuana and tobacco prenatally and, in the case of prenatal hostility, associates with her infant's temperament. Second, we replicated temperament profiles that have been identified by other researchers in a sample of infants with varying levels of prenatal substance exposure and psychosocial stressors, demonstrating the utility of person-centered approaches for identifying RDoC-informed phenotypes in the first two years of life. Integrating a transdiagnostic perspective into our approach to perinatal mental health and early child development may ultimately provide specific targets for preventative services aimed at reducing childhood psychopathology, via early dissemination of parenting interventions that challenge dysfunctional cognitive biases, ameliorate familial risk, and support an infant's emotional development.

Supplementary Material

The supplementary material for this article can be found at https://doi.org/10.1017/S0954579421001000

Acknowledgments

We would like to thank the families who participated in our study. Special thanks to Dr. Amol Lele for collaboration on data collection at Women and Children's Hospital of Buffalo.

Author Contributions

S. Shisler, S. Godleski, P. Schuetze, and R.D. Eiden contributed to the study concept and data collection. B.D. Ostlund and S. Terrell performed the data analysis and interpretation under the supervision of K. Pérez-Edgar and R.D. Eiden. All authors contributed to drafting the manuscript. All authors approved the final version of the manuscript.

Funding Statement

This manuscript was supported by the National Institute on Drug Abuse under award number R01DA01963201. The content is solely the responsibility of the authors and does not necessarily represent the official views of the National Institute on Drug Abuse or the National Institutes of Health.

Conflicts of interest

None

References

Ablow, J. C., Marks, A. K., Feldman, S. S., & Huffman, L. C. (2013). Associations between first-time expectant women's representations of attachment and their physiological reactivity to infant cry. Child Development, 84, 13731391.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Bara, A., Ferland, J.-M. N., Rompala, G., Szutorisz, H., & Hurd, Y. L. (2021). Cannabis and synaptic reprogramming of the developing brain. Nature Reviews Neuroscience, 55, doi:10.1038/s41583-021-00465-5Google Scholar
Bayrampour, H., Zahradnik, M., Lisonkova, S., & Janssen, P. (2019). Women's perspective about cannabis use during pregnancy and the postpartum period: An integrative review. Preventive Medicine, 119, 1723. doi:10.1016/j.ypmed.2018.12.002CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Beauchaine, T. P. (2015). Future directions in emotion dysregulation and youth psychopathology. Journal of Clinical Child and Adolescent Psychology, 44, 875896.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Beauchaine, T. P., Zisner, A. R., & Sauder, C. L. (2017). Trait impulsivity and the externalizing spectrum. Annual Review of Clinical Psychology, 13, 343368. doi:10.1146/annurev-clinpsy-021815-093253CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Beekman, C., Neiderhiser, J. M., Buss, K. A., Loken, E., Moore, G. A., Leve, L. D., … Reiss, D. (2015). The development of early profiles of temperament: Characterization, continuity, and etiology. Child Development, 86, 17941811. doi:10.1111/cdev.12417CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Benjamini, Y., & Hochberg, Y. (1995). Controlling the false discovery rate: A practical and powerful approach to multiple testing. Journal of the Royal Statistical Society Series B, 57, 289300. doi:10.1111/j.2517-6161.1995.tb02031.xGoogle Scholar
Berkowitz, L. (1993). Aggression: Its causes, consequences, and control. McGraw-Hill Book Company.Google Scholar
Blau, L. E., Orloff, N., & Hormes, J. M. (2020). The transdiagnostic nature of cravings: Smoking cessation and food craving in pregnancy. Midwifery, 87, 102730. doi:10.1016/j.midw.2020.102730CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Borodovsky, J. T., Crosier, B. S., Lee, D. C., Sargent, J. D., & Budney, A. J. (2016). Smoking, vaping, eating: Is legalization impacting the way people use cannabis? International Journal of Drug Policy, 36, 141147. doi:10.1016/j.drugpo.2016.02.022CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Braungart-Rieker, J. M., Hill-Soderlund, A. L., & Karrass, J. (2010). Fear and anger reactivity trajectories from 4 to 16 months: The roles of temperament, regulation, and maternal sensitivity. Developmental Psychology, 46, 791804. doi:10.1037/a0019673CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Breiman, L. (2001). Random forests. Machine Learning, 45, 532.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Brown, R. A., Burgess, E. S., Sales, S. D., Whiteley, J. A., Evans, D. M., & Miller, I. W. (1998). Reliability and validity of a smoking timeline follow-back interview. Psychology of Addictive Behavior, 12, 101112.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Buss, A. H. (1961). The psychology of aggression. Wiley.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Buss, A. H., & Perry, M. (1992). The aggression questionnaire. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 63, 452459.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Carter, A. S., & Briggs-Gowan, M. (2005). ITSEA BITSEA: The infant-toddler and brief infant toddler social emotional assessment. San Antonio, TX: PsychCorp.Google Scholar
Carter, A. S., Briggs-Gowan, M. J., Jones, S. M., & Little, T. D. (2003). The infant-toddler social and emotional assessment (ITSEA): Factor structure, reliability, and validity. Journal of Abnormal Child Psychology, 31, 495514.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Casey, B. J., Oliveri, M. E., & Insel, T. (2014). A neurodevelopmental perspective on the research domain criteria (RDoC) framework. Biological Psychiatry, 76, 350353. doi:10.1016/j.biopsych.2014.01.006CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Caspi, A., & Silva, P. A. (1995). Temperamental qualities at age three predict personality traits in young adulthood: Longitudinal evidence from a birth cohort. Child Development, 66, 486498.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Celeux, G., & Soromenho, G. (1996). An entropy criterion for assessing the number of clusters in a mixture model. Journal of Classification, 13, 195212. doi:10.1007/bf01246098CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Chabarria, K. C., Racusin, D. A., Antony, K. M., Kahr, M., Suter, M. A., & Aagaard, K. M. (2016). Marijuana use and its effect on pregnancy. American Journal of Obstetrics and Gynecology, 215, e1e7. doi:10.1016/j.ajog.2016.05.044CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Clark, C. A. C., Espy, K. A., & Wakschlag, L. (2016). Developmental pathways from prenatal tobacco and stress exposure to behavioral disinhibition. Neurotoxicology and Teratology, 53, 6474.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Clauss, J. A., Avery, S. N., & Blackford, J. U. (2015). The nature of individual differences in inhibited temperament and risk for psychiatric disease: A review and meta-analysis. Progress in Neurobiology, 127–128, 2345. doi:10.1016/j.pneurobio.2015.03.001CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Cole, P. M., Hall, S. E., & Hajal, N. J. (2013). Emotion dysregulation as a risk factor for psychopathology. In Beauchaine, T. P. & Hinshaw, S. P. (Eds.), Child and adolescent psychopathology (2nd ed., pp. 341373). Hoboken, NJ: Wiley.Google Scholar
Coleman-Cowger, V. H., Schauer, G. L., & Peters, E. N. (2017). Marijuana and tobacco co-use among a nationally representative sample of US pregnant and non-pregnant women: 2005–2014 national survey on drug Use and health findings. Drug and Alcohol Dependence, 177, 130135. doi:10.1016/j.drugalcdep.2017.03.025CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Crowell, S. E., Puzia, M. E., & Yaptangco, M. (2015). The ontogeny of chronic distress: Emotion dysregulation across the life span and its implications for psychological and physical health. Current Opinion in Psychology, 3, 9199.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Cuthbert, B. N. (2014). Translating intermediate phenotypes to psychopathology: The NIMH research domain criteria. Psychophysiology, 51, 12051206. doi:10.1111/psyp.12342CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Cuthbert, B. N., & Insel, T. R. (2010). Toward new approaches to psychotic disorders: The NIMH research domain criteria project. Schizophrenia Bulletin, 36, 10611062. doi:10.1093/schbul/sbq108CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Dalgleish, T., Black, M., Johnston, D., & Bevan, A. (2020). Transdiagnostic approaches to mental health problems: Current status and future directions. Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology, 88, 179195. doi:10.1037/ccp0000482CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
DeCapo, M., Thompson, J. R., Dunn, G., & Sullivan, E. L. (2019). Perinatal nutrition and programmed risk for neuropsychiatric disorders: A focus on animal models. Biological Psychiatry, 85, 122134. doi:10.1016/j.biopsych.2018.08.006CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
De Genna, N. M., Stroud, L. R., & Eiden, R. D. (2019). Co-use of tobacco and marijuana during pregnancy: Impact on nervous system development. Neurotoxicology and Teratology, 74, 106807. https://doi.org/j.ntt.2019.04.004CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Del Giudice, M., Ellis, B. J., & Shirtcliff, E. A. (2011). The adaptive calibration model of stress responsivity. Neuroscience and Biobehavioral Reviews, 35, 15621592. doi:10.1016/j.neubiorev.2010.11.007CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Del Vecchio, T., Walter, A., & O'Leary, S. G. (2009). Affective and physiological factors predicting maternal response to infant crying. Infant Behavior and Development, 32, 117122.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
DiNieri, J. A., Wang, X., Szutorisz, H., Spano, S. M., Kaur, J., Casaccia, P., … Hurd, Y. L. (2011). Maternal cannabis use alters ventral striatal dopamine D2 gene regulation in the offspring. Biological Psychiatry, 70, 763769. doi:10.1016/j.biopsych.2011.06.027CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Downer, E. J., Gowran, A., & Campbell, V. A. (2007). A comparison of the apoptotic effect of Δ 9-tetrahydrocannabinol in the neonatal and adult rat cerebral cortex. Brain Research, 1175, 3947.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Eckhardt, C., Norlander, B., & Deffenbacher, J. (2004). The assessment of anger and hostility: A critical review. Aggression and Violent Behavior, 9, 1743.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Eiden, R. D., Leonard, K. E., Colder, C. R., Homish, G. G., Schuetze, P., & Huestis, M. A. (2011). Anger, hostility, and aggression as predictors of persistent smoking during pregnancy. Journal of Studies on Alcohol and Drugs, 72, 926932. doi:10.15288/jsad.2011.72.926CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Eiden, R. D., Peterson, M., & Coleman, T. (1999). Maternal cocaine use and the caregiving environment during early childhood. Psychology of Addictive Behavior, 13, 293302. doi:10.1037/0893-164X.13.4.293CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Eiden, R. D., Schuetze, P., Shisler, S., & Huestis, M. A. (2018a). Prenatal exposure to tobacco and cannabis: Effects on autonomic and emotion regulation. Neurotoxicology and Teratology, 68, 4756. doi:10.1016/j.ntt.2018.04.007CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Eiden, R. D., Shisler, S., Granger, D. A., Schuetze, P., Colangelo, J., & Huestis, M. A. (2020). Prenatal tobacco and cannabis exposure: Associations with cortisol reactivity in early school age children. International Journal of Behavioral Medicine, 27, 343356. doi:10.1007/s12529-020-09875-8CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Eiden, R. D., Zhao, J., Casey, M., Schuetze, P., Shisler, S., & Colder, C. R. (2018b). Pre- and postnatal tobacco and cannabis exposure and child behavior problems: Bidirectional associations, joint effects, and sex differences. Drug and Alcohol Dependence, 185, 8292.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Ellis, B. J., Boyce, W. T., Belsky, J., Bakermans-Kranenburg, M. J., & van Ijzendoorn, M. H. (2011). Differential susceptibility to the environment: An evolutionary-neurodevelopmental theory. Development and Psychopathology, 23, 728. doi:10.1017/S0954579410000611CrossRefGoogle Scholar
El Marroun, H., Hudziak, J. J., Tiemeier, H., Creemers, H., Steegers, E. A., Jaddoe, V. W., … Huizink, A. C. (2011). Intrauterine cannabis exposure leads to more aggressive behavior and attention problems in 18-month-old girls. Drug and Alcohol Dependence, 118, 470474. doi:10.1016/ j.drugalcdep.2011.03.004CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
El Marroun, H., Tiemeier, H., Franken, I. H., Jaddoe, V. W. V., van der Lugt, A., Verhulst, F. C., … White, T. (2016). Prenatal cannabis and tobacco exposure in relation to brain morphology: A prospective neuroimaging study in young children. Biological Psychiatry, 79, 971979.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
England, L. J., Aagaard, K., Bloch, M., Conway, K., Cosgrove, K., Grana, R., … Wakschlag, L. (2017). Developmental toxicity of nicotine: A transdisciplinary synthesis and implications for emerging tobacco products. Neuroscience and Biobehavioral Review, 72, 176189. doi:10.1016/j.neubiorev.2016.11.013CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Espy, K. A., Fang, H., Johnson, C., Stopp, C., Wiebe, S. A., & Respass, J. (2011). Prenatal tobacco exposure: Developmental outcomes in the neonatal period. Developmental Psychology, 47, 153169. doi:10.1037/a0020724CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Estabrook, R., Massey, S. H., Clark, C. A. C., Burns, J. L., Mustanski, B. S., Cook, E. H., … Wakschlag, L. S. (2016). Separating family-level and direct exposure effects of smoking during pregnancy on offspring externalizing symptoms: Bridging the behavior genetic and behavior teratologic divide. Behavior Genetics, 46, 389402.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Field, T., Diego, M., Hernandez-Reif, M., Figueiredo, B., Deeds, O., Ascencio, A., … Kuhn, C. (2008). Prenatal dopamine and neonatal behavior and biochemistry. Infant Behavior and Development, 31, 590593.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Field, T., Diego, M., Hernandez-Reif, M., Salman, F., Schanberg, S., Kuhn, C., … Bendell, D. (2002). Prenatal anger effects on the fetus and neonate. Journal of Obstetrics and Gynecology, 22, 260266.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Franklin, J. C., Jamieson, J. P., Glenn, C. R., & Nock, M. K. (2015). How developmental psychopathology theory and research can inform the research domain criteria (RDoC) project. Journal of Clinical Child & Adolescent Psychology, 44, 280290. doi:10.1080/15374416.2013.873981CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Garcia-Coll, C., Kagan, J., & Reznick, J. S. (1984). Behavioral inhibition in young children. Child Development, 55, 10051019.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Gartstein, M. A., Prokasky, A., Bell, M. A., Calkins, S., Bridgett, D. J., Braungart-Rieker, J., … Seamon, E. (2017). Latent profile and cluster analysis of infant temperament: Comparisons across person-centered approaches. Developmental Psychology, 53, 18111825. doi:10.1037/dev0000382CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Gatzke-Kopp, L. M. (2011). The canary in the coalmine: The sensitivity of mesolimbic dopamine to environmental adversity during development. Neuroscience and Biobehavioral Reviews, 35, 794803. doi:10.1016/j.neubiorev.2010.09.013CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Gatzke-Kopp, L. M. (2016). Diversity and representation: Key issues for psychophysiological science. Psychophysiology, 53, 313. doi:10.1111/psyp.12566CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Gluckman, P. D., Hanson, M. A., Cooper, C., & Thornburg, K. L. (2008). Effect of in utero and early-life conditions on adult health and disease. New England Journal of Medicine, 359, 6173.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Goldsmith, H. H. (1996). Studying temperament via construction of the toddler behavior assessment questionnaire. Child Development, 67, 218235. doi:10.2307/ 1131697CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Goldsmith, H. H., & Rothbart, M. K. (1999). The laboratory temperament assessment battery (locomotor version 3.1). Madison: University of Wisconsin–Madison.Google Scholar
Gray, T. R., Eiden, R. D., Leonard, K. E., Connors, G., Shisler, S., & Huestis, M. A. (2010a). Nicotine and metabolites in meconium as evidence of maternal cigarette smoking during pregnancy and predictors of neonatal growth deficits. Nicotine and Tobacco Research, 12, 658664. doi:10.1093/ntr/ntq068CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Gray, T. R., Eiden, R. D., Leonard, K. E., Connors, G. J., Shisler, S., & Huestis, M. A. (2010b). Identifying prenatal cannabis exposure and effects of concurrent tobacco exposure on neonatal growth. Clinical Chemistry, 56, 14421450. doi:10.1373/clinchem.2010.147876CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Harvey, A. G., Watkins, E., Mansell, W., & Shafran, R. (2004). Cognitive behavioural processes across psychological disorders. Oxford, United Kingdom: Oxford University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Hendrick, V., Altshuler, L. L., & Suri, R. (1998). Hormonal changes in the postpartum and implications for postpartum depression. Psychosomatics, 39, 93101.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Houston, B. K., & Vavak, C. R. (1991). Cynical hostility: Developmental factors, psychosocial correlates, and health behaviors. Health Psychology, 10, 917. doi:10.1037//0278-6133.10.1.9CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Howard, L. M., & Khalifeh, H. (2020). Perinatal mental health: A review of progress and challenges. World Psychiatry, 19, 313327. doi:10.1002/wps.20769CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Hu, L., & Bentler, P. M. (1999). Cutoff criteria for fit indexes in covariance structure analysis: Conventional criteria versus new alternatives. Structural Equation Modeling, 6, 155. doi:10.1080/10705519909540118CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Hurley, K. M., Caulfield, L. E., Sacco, L. M., Costigan, K. A., & DiPietro, J. A. (2005). Psychosocial influences in dietary patterns during pregnancy. Journal of the American Dietetic Association, 105, 963966. doi:10.1016/j.jada.2005.03.007CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Insel, T., Cuthbert, B., Garvey, M., Heinssen, R., Pine, D. S., Quinn, K., … Wang, P. (2010). Research domain criteria (RDoC): Toward a new classification framework for research on mental disorders. American Journal of Psychiatry, 167, 748751. doi:10.1176/appi.ajp.2010.09091379CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Izard, C. E., Dougherty, L. M., & Hembree, E. A. (1983). A system for identifying affect expressions by holistic judgments (AFFEX). Newark, DE: Computer Network Services and University Media Services, University of Delaware.Google Scholar
Izard's Maximally Discriminative Facial Movement Coding System. (1979). unpublished manuscript.Google Scholar
Janson, H., & Mathiesen, K. S. (2008). Temperament profiles from infancy to middle childhood: Development and associations with behavior problems. Developmental Psychology, 44, 13141328. doi:10.1037/a0012713CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Kagan, J., & Snidman, N. (1991). Infant predictors of inhibited and uninhibited profiles. Psychological Science, 2, 4044.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Kahler, C. W., Strong, D. R., Niaura, R., & Brown, R. A. (2004). Hostility in smokers with past major depressive disorder: Relation to smoking patterns, reasons for quitting, and cessation outcomes. Nicotine & Tobacco Research, 6, 818.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Karalunas, S. L., Fair, D., Musser, E. D., Aykes, K., Iyer, S. P., & Nigg, J. T. (2014). Subtyping attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder using temperament dimensions toward biologically based nosologic criteria. JAMA Psychiatry, 97239, 10151024. doi:10.1001/jamapsychiatry.2014.763CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Ko, J. Y., Farr, S. L., Tong, V. T., Creanga, A. A., & Callaghan, W. M. (2015). Prevalence and patterns of marijuana use among pregnant and nonpregnant women of reproductive age. American Journal of Obstetrics and Gynecology, 213, e210. doi:10.1016/j.ajog.2015.03.021CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Komsi, N., Räikkönen, K., Pesonen, A.-K., Heinonen, K., Keskivaara, P., & Strandberg, T. E. (2006). Continuity of temperament from infancy to middle childhood. Infant Behavior & Development, 29, 494508. doi:10.1016/j.infbeh.2006.05.002CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Kozak, M. J., & Cuthbert, B. N. (2016). The NIMH research domain criteria initiative: Background, issues, and pragmatics. Psychophysiology, 53, 286297. doi:10.1111/psyp.12518CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Krueger, R. F., & Eaton, N. R. (2015). Transdiagnostic factors of mental disorders. World Psychiatry, 14, 2729. doi:10.1002/wps.20175CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Lawrence, P. J., Creswell, C., Cooper, P. J., & Murray, L. (2020). The role of maternal anxiety disorder subtype, parenting and infant stable temperamental inhibition in child anxiety: A prospective longitudinal study. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 61, 779788. doi:10.1111/JCPP.13187CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Leibenluft, E., & Stoddard, J. (2015). The developmental psychopathology of irritability. Developmental Psychopathology, 25, 14731487. doi:10.1017/S0954579413000722.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Lester, B. M., ElSohly, M., Wright, L. L., Smeriglio, V. L., Verter, J., Bauer, C. R., … Maza, P. L. (2001). The maternal lifestyle study: Drug use by meconium toxicology and maternal self-report. Pediatrics, 107, 309317. doi:10.1542/peds.107.2.309CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Lichtensteiger, W., Ribary, U., Schlumpf, M., Odermatt, B., & Widmer, H. R. (1988). Prenatal adverse effects of nicotine on the developing brain. Progress in Brain Research, 73, 137157.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Lin, B., Kaliush, P. R., Conradt, E., Terrell, S., Neff, D., Allen, A. K., … Crowell, S. E. (2019). Intergenerational transmission of emotion dysregulation: Part I. Psychopathology, self-injury, and parasympathetic responsivity among pregnant women. Development and Psychopathology, 31, 817831. doi:10.1017/S0954579419000336CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Lin, B., Ostlund, B. D., Conradt, E., Lagasse, L. L., & Lester, B. (2018). Testing the programming of temperament and psychopathology in two independent samples of children with prenatal substance exposure. Development and Psychopathology, 30, 10231040. doi:10.1017/S0954579418000391CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Little, B. B., & VanBeveren, T. T. (1996). Placental transfer of selected substances of abuse. Seminar in Perinatology, 20, 147153.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Liu, C., Moore, G. A., Beekman, C., Pérez-Edgar, K. E., Leve, L. D., Shaw, D. S., … Neiderhiser, J. M. (2018). Developmental patterns of anger from infancy to middle childhood predict problem behaviors at age 8. Developmental Psychology, 54, 20902100. doi:10.1037/dev0000589CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Liu, J., Bann, C., Lester, B., Tronick, E., Das, A., Lagasse, L., … Bada, H. (2010). Neonatal neurobehavior predicts medical and behavioral outcome. Pediatrics, 125, e90e98.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Lubke, G., & Neale, M. C. (2006). Distinguishing between latent classes and continuous factors: Resolution by maximum likelihood? Multivariate Behavioral Research, 41, 499532.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Maxwell, S. E., Delaney, H. D., & Kelley, K. (2018). Designing experiments and analyzing data: A model comparison perspective (3rd ed). New York: Routledge.Google Scholar
Mehmedic, Z., Chandra, S., Slade, D., Denham, H., Foster, S., Patel, A. S., … ElSohly, M. A. (2010). Potency trends of Δ9-THC and other cannabinoids in confiscated cannabis preparations from 1993 to 2008. Journal of Forensic Science, 55, 12091217.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Miller, T. Q., Smith, T. W., Turner, C. W., Guijarro, M. L., & Hallet, A. J. (1996). Meta-analytic review of research on hostility and physical health. Psychological Bulletin, 119, 322.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Moaddab, A., Dildy, G. A., Brown, H. L., Bateni, Z. H., Belfort, M. A., & Clark, S. L. (2018). Health care disparity and pregnancy-related mortality in the united states, 2005-2014. Obstetrics & Gynecology, 131, 707712. doi:10.1097/AOG.0000000000002534CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Molnar, D. S., Rancourt, D., Schlauch, R., Wen, X., Maiorana, N., & Eiden, R. D. (2017). Tobacco exposure and conditional weight-for-length-gain by 2 years of age. Journal of Pediatric Psychology, 42, 679688.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Morales, S., Fu, X., & Pérez-Edgar, K. E. (2016). A developmental neuroscience perspective on affect-biased attention. Developmental Cognitive Neuroscience, 21, 2641. doi:10.1016/j.dcn.2016.08.001CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Morena, M., Patel, S., Bains, J. S., & Hill, M. N. (2016). Neurobiological interactions between stress and the endocannabinoid system. Neuropsychopharmacology, 41, 80102. doi:10.1038/npp.2015.166CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
National Institute of Mental Health (NIMH). (2018). Research Domain Criteria (RDoC): Developmental and environmental aspects. Retrieved from https://www.nimh.nih.gov/research-priorities/rdoc/developmental-and-environmental-aspects.shtmlGoogle Scholar
NICHD Early Child Care Research Network. (2004). Trajectories of physical aggression from toddlerhood to middle childhood: Predictors, correlates, and outcomes. Monographs of the Society for Research in Child Development, 69, 1143. doi:10.1111/j.0037-976X.2004.00312.xGoogle Scholar
Nolan-Hoeksema, S., & Watkins, E. R. (2011). A heuristic for developing transdiagnostic models of psychopathology: Explaining multifinality and divergent trajectories. Perspectives on Psychological Science, 6, 589609. doi:10.1177/1745691611419672CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Obeysekare, J. L., Cohen, Z. L., Coles, M. E., Pearlstein, T. B., Monzon, C., & Sharkey, K. M. (2020). Delayed sleep timing and circadian rhythms in pregnancy and transdiagnostic symptoms associated with postpartum depression. Translational Psychiatry, 10. doi:10.1038/s41398-020-0683-3CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Olino, T. M., Guerra-Guzman, K., Hayden, E. P., & Klein, D. N. (2020). Evaluating maternal psychopathology biases in reports of child temperament: An investigation of measurement invariance. Psychological Assessment, 32, 10371046. doi:10.1037/pas0000945.suppCrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Ostlund, B., Myruski, S., Buss, K., & Pérez-Edgar, K. E. (2021). The centrality of temperament to the research domain criteria (RDoC): The earliest building blocks of psychopathology. Development and Psychopathology, 115. doi:10.1017/ S0954579421000511Google Scholar
Ostlund, B. D., Vlisides-Henry, R. D., Crowell, S. E., Raby, K. L., Terrell, S., Brown, M. A., … Conradt, E. (2019). Intergenerational transmission of emotion dysregulation: Part II. Developmental origins of newborn neurobehavior. Development and Psychopathology, 114. doi:10.1017/S0954579419000440Google ScholarPubMed
Patterson, G. R., Debaryshe, B. D., & Ramsey, E. (1989). A developmental perspective on antisocial behavior. American Psychologist, 44, 329335.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Perra, O., Paine, A. L., & Hay, D. F. (2020). Continuity and change in anger and aggressiveness from infancy to childhood: The protective effects of positive parenting. Development and Psychopathology, 120. doi:10.1017/S0954579420000243Google Scholar
Planalp, E. M., & Goldsmith, H. H. (2020). Observed profiles of infant temperament: Stability, heritability, and associations with parenting. Child Development, 91, e563e580. doi:10.1111/cdev.13277CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Prokasky, A., Rudasill, K., Molfese, V. J., Putnam, S., Gartstein, M., & Rothbart, M. (2017). Identifying child temperament types using cluster analysis in three samples. Journal of Research in Personality, 67, 190201. doi:10.1016/j.jrp.2016.10.008CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Putnam, K. T., Wilcox, M., Robertson-Blackmore, E., Sharkey, K., Bergink, V., Munk-Olsen, T., … Consortium, for the P. D. A. T. C. and T. (PACT). (2017). Clinical phenotypes of perinatal depression and time of symptom onset: Analysis of data from an international consortium. Lancet Psychiatry, 4, 477485. doi:10.1016/S2215-0366(17)30136-0CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Putnam, S. P., & Stifter, C. A. (2005). Behavioral approach-inhibition in toddlers: Prediction from infancy, positive and negative affective components, and relations with behavior problems. Child Development, 76, 212226.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Rakers, F., Bischoff, S., Schiffner, R., Haase, M, Rupprecht, S., Kiehntopf, M., … Schwab, M. (2015). Role of catecholamines in maternal-fetal stress transfer in sheep. American Journal of Obstetrics and Gynecology, 213, e1e9. doi:10.1016/j.ajog.2015.07.020CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Ramirez, J. M., & Andreu, J. M. (2006). Aggression, and some related psychological constructs (anger, hostility, and impulsivity); some comments from a research project. Neuroscience and Biobehavioral Reviews, 30, 276291. doi:10.1016/j.neubiorev.2005.04.015CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
R Core Team. (2020). R: A language and environment for statistical computing. Vienna, Austria: R Foundation for Statistical Computing. URL https://www.R-project.org/Google Scholar
Robinson, E. A., Brower, K. J., & Gomberg, E. S. (2001). Explaining unexpected gender differences in hostility among persons seeking treatment for substance use disorders. Journal of Studies on Alcohol and Drugs, 62, 667674.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Robinson, S. M., Sobell, L. C., Sobell, M. B., & Leo, G. I. (2014). Reliability of the timeline followback for cocaine, cannabis, and cigarette use. Psychology of Addictive Behavior, 28, 154162. doi:10.1037/a0030992CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Román-Gálvez, R. M., Amezcua-Prieto, C., Salcedo-Bellido, I., Martínez-Galiano, J. M., Kahn, K. S., & Bueno-Cavanillas, A. (2018). Factors associated with insomnia in pregnancy: A prospective cohort study. European Journal of Obstetrics and Gynecology and Reproductive Biology, 221, 7075. doi:10.1016/j.ejogrb.2017.12.007CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Rosenberg, J. M., Beymer, P. N., Anderson, D. J., van Lissa, C. J., & Schmidt, J. A. (2018). tidyLPA: An R package to easily carry out latent profile analysis (LPA) using open-source or commercial software. Journal of Open Source Software, 3, 978.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Rosseel, Y. (2012). Lavaan: An R package for structural equation modeling. Journal of Statistical Software, 48, 136. http://www.jstatsoft.org/v48/i02/CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Rubin, K. H., Burgess, K. B., Dwyer, K. M., & Hastings, P. D. (2003). Predicting preschoolers’ externalizing behaviors from toddler temperament, conflict, and maternal negativity. Developmental Psychology, 39, 164.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Saluja, B., & Bryant, Z. (2020). How implicit bias contributes to racial disparities in maternal morbidity and mortality in the united states. Journal of Women's Health, doi:10.1089/jwh.2020.8874Google ScholarPubMed
Sanson, A., Letcher, P., Smart, D., Prior, M., Toumbourou, J. W., & Oberklaid, F. (2009). Associations between early childhood temperament clusters and later psychosocial adjustment. Merrill-Palmer Quarterly, 55, 2654. doi:10.1353/mpq.0.0015CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Schuetze, P., Eiden, R. D., & Dombkowski, L. (2006). The association between cigarette smoking during pregnancy and maternal behavior during the neonatal period. Infancy, 10, 267288.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Schuetze, P., Lopez, F. A., Granger, D. A., & Eiden, R. D. (2008). The association between prenatal exposure to cigarettes and cortisol reactivity and regulation in 7-month-old infants. Developmental Psychobiology, 50, 819834. doi:10.1002/dev.20334CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Schuetze, P., & Zeskind, P. S. (2001). Relations between women's depressive symptoms and perceptions of infant distress signals varying in pitch. Infancy, 2, 483499.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Schwarz, G. (1978). Estimating the dimension of a model. Annals of Statistics, 6, 461464.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Scott, B. G., Lemery-Chalfant, K., Clifford, S., Tein, J.-Y., Stoll, R., & Goldsmith, H. H. (2016). A twin factor mixture modeling approach to childhood temperament: Differential heritability. Child Development, 87, 19401955. doi:10.1111/cdev.12561CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Scott-Goodwin, A., Puerto, M., & Moreno, I. (2016). Toxic effects of prenatal exposure to alcohol, tobacco and other drugs. Reproductive Toxicology, 61, 120130.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Sellers, R., Harold, G. T., Elam, K., Rhoades, K. A., Potter, R., Mars, B., … Collishaw, S. (2014). Maternal depression and co-occurring antisocial behaviour: Testing maternal hostility and warmth as mediators of risk for offspring psychopathology. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 55, 112120.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Shaw, D. S., Lacourse, E., & Nagin, D. S. (2005). Developmental trajectories of conduct problems and hyperactivity from ages 2 to 10. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 46, 931942. doi:10.1111/j.1469-7610.2004.00390.xCrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Shiner, R., Buss, K., McClowry, S., Putnam, S., Saudino, K., & Zentner, M. (2012). What is temperament now? Assessing progress in temperament research on the twenty-fifth anniversary of goldsmith et al. (1987). Child Development Perspectives, 6, 436444. doi:10.1111/j.1750– 8606.2012.00254.xGoogle Scholar
Slagt, M., Semon, J., Deković, M., & van Aken, M. A. G. (2016). Differences in sensitivity to parenting depending on child temperament: A meta-analysis. Psychological Bulletin, 142, 10681110. doi:10.1037/bul0000061CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Slotkin, T. A. (2008). If nicotine is a developmental neurotoxicant in animal studies, dare we recommend nicotine replacement therapy in pregnant women and adolescents? Neurotoxicology and Teratology, 30, 119.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Slotkin, T. A., Skavicus, S., Card, J., Stadler, A., Levin, E. D., & Seidler, F. J. (2015). Developmental neurotoxicity of tobacco smoke directed toward cholinergic and serotonergic systems: More than just nicotine. Toxicological Sciences, 147, 178189. doi:10.1093/toxsci/kfv123CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Smith, L. M., LaGasse, L. L., Derauf, C., Grant, P., Shah, R., Arria, A., … Lester, B. M. (2006). The infant development, environment, and lifestyle study: Effects of prenatal methamphetamine exposure, polydrug exposure, and poverty on intrauterine growth. Pediatrics, 118, 11491156. doi:10.1542/peds.2005-2564CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Sobell, L., & Sobell, M. (1992). Timeline followback: A technique for assessing self reported ethanol consumption Vol. 17. Totowa, NJ: Humana Press.Google Scholar
Somer, S. J. H., Sinkey, R. G., & Bryant, A. S. (2017). Epidemiology of racial/ethnic disparities in severe maternal morbidity and mortality. Seminars in Perinatology, 41, 258265. doi:10.1053/j.semperi.2017.04.001CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Spry, E. A., Aarsman, S. R., Youssef, G. J., Patton, G. C., Macdonald, J. A., Sanson, A., … Olsson, C. A. (2020). Maternal and paternal depression and anxiety and offspring infant negative affectivity: A systematic review and meta-analysis. Developmental Review, 58, 100934. doi:10.1016/j.dr.2020.100934CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Stekhoven, D. J., & Bühlmann, P. (2012). Missforest—Non-parametric missing value imputation for mixed-type data. Bioinformatics, 28, 112118. doi:10.1093/bioinformatics/btr597CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Stifter, C. A., Putnam, S. P., & Jahromi, L. (2008). Exuberant and inhibited toddlers: Stability of temperament and risk for problem behavior. Development and Psychopathology, 20, 401421. doi:10.1017/S0954579408000199CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Stroud, L. R., Papandonatos, G. D., McCallum, M., Kehoe, T., Salisbury, A., & Huestis, M. (2018). Prenatal tobacco and marijuana co-use: Impact on newborn neurobehavior and stress response. Neurotoxicology and Teratology, 70, 2839.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Stroud, L. R., Papandonatos, G. D., Salisbury, A. L., Phipps, M. G., Huestis, M. A., Niaura, R., … Lester, B. (2016). Epigenetic regulation of placental NR3C1: Mechanism underlying prenatal programming of infant neurobehavior by maternal smoking? Child Development, 87, 4960. doi:10.1111/cdev.12482CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Stroud, L. R., Paster, R. L., Papandonatos, G. D., Niaura, R., Salisbury, A. L., Battle, C., … Lester, B. M. (2009). Maternal smoking during pregnancy and newborn neurobehavior: Effects at 10 to 27 days. Journal of Pediatrics, 154, 1016. doi:10.1016/j.jpeds.2008.07.048CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Tang, F., & Ishwaran, H. (2017). Random forest missing data algorithms. Statistical Analysis and Data Mining, 10, 363377. doi:10.1002/sam.11348CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Thomas, A., & Chess, S. (1977). Temperament and development. New York: Brunner/Mazel.Google Scholar
US Department of Health and Human Services, Centers for Disease Control and Prevention, National Center for Chronic Disease Prevention and Health Promotion, Office on Smoking and Health. (2014). The health consequences of smoking –50 years of progress: A report of the Surgeon General. 17, Atlanta, GA.Google Scholar
Van Den Akker, A. L., Deković, M., Prinzie, P., & Asscher, J. J. (2010). Toddlers’ temperament profiles: Stability and relations to negative and positive parenting. Journal of Abnormal Child Psychology, 38, 485495. doi:10.1007/s10802-009-9379-0CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Van den Bergh, B. R. H., van den Heuvel, M. I., Lahti, M., Braeken, M. A. K. A., de Rooij, S., Entringer, S., … Schwab, M. (2017). Prenatal developmental origins of behavior and mental health: The influence of maternal stress in pregnancy. Neuroscience and Biobehavioral Reviews, 117, 2664. doi:10.1016/j.neubiorev.2017.07.003CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Walsh, K., Mccormack, C. A., Webster, R., Pinto, A., Lee, S., Feng, T., … Monk, C. (2019). Maternal prenatal stress phenotypes associate with fetal neurodevelopment and birth outcomes. Annals of the New York Academy of Sciences, 116, 2399624005. doi:10.1073/pnas.1905890116Google ScholarPubMed
Whiteman, M. C., Fowkes, F. G. R., Deary, I. J., & Lee, A. J. (1997). Hostility, cigarette smoking and alcohol consumption in the general population. Social Science & Medicine, 44, 10891096.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Wycoff, A. M., Metrik, J., & Trull, T. J. (2018). Affect and cannabis use in daily life: A review and recommendations for future research. Drug and Alcohol Dependence, 191, 223233. doi:10.1016/j.drugalcdep.2018.07.001CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Figure 0

Table 1. Demographic information.

Figure 1

Table 2. Descriptive information and correlations among temperament dimensions.

Figure 2

Figure 1. Temperament profiles at 16 months of age. Mean standardized scores on temperament dimension from the Toddler Behavior Assessment Questionnaire (Goldsmith, 1996) and an arm restraint task (Goldsmith & Rothbart, 1999) are presented. “Struggle” = behavioral reactivity to an arm restraint task at 16 months of age. “Obj. fear” = object fear. “Inh. control” = inhibitory control. “Perc. sensitivity” = perceptual sensitivity.

Figure 3

Table 3. Summary of fit statistics for latent class analysis of infant temperament.

Figure 4

Figure 2. Trajectories of self-reported maternal hostility from the third trimester of pregnancy to 16 months postnatal. Data from each mother across time are represented by a unique color (a); the solid red line indicates linear fit. Data from a random 10% of mothers who had data at each time point are presented in black to exemplify maternal trajectories (b); the remainder of the sample is presented in gray.

Figure 5

Figure 3. Final path model with standardized path coefficients. Solid lines reflect significant associations. A mothers’ prenatal substance use was effects coded, yielding two comparisons: (a) pregnant women who used both marijuana and tobacco or tobacco only (“THC + Tob. & Tob. only”) were compared to pregnant women who did not use either substance (“none”); and (b) pregnant women who used tobacco but not marijuana (“tobacco only”) were compared to women who used both marijuana and tobacco (“THC + Tob.”). Time coding for the latent growth variables reflects the number of months from the prenatal assessment divided by a constant (4). Temperament profiles were dummy coded; the well-regulated profile served as the reference group for all comparisons.

Figure 6

Figure 4. Post hoc comparisons among infant temperament profiles and the (A) problem behavior and (B) competence scales of the Brief Infant-Toddler Social and Emotional Assessment (BITSEA) (Carter & Briggs-Gowan, 2005).

Supplementary material: File

Ostlund et al. supplementary material

Ostlund et al. supplementary material

Download Ostlund et al. supplementary material(File)
File 305.1 KB