Hostname: page-component-76fb5796d-25wd4 Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-04-25T20:45:56.270Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

Socioemotional profiles of autism spectrum disorders, attention deficit hyperactivity disorder, and disinhibited and reactive attachment disorders: a symptom comparison and network approach

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  12 November 2021

Barry Coughlan*
Affiliation:
Department of Public Health and Primary Care, University of Cambridge, Cambridge, UK
Matt Woolgar
Affiliation:
Institute of Psychiatry, Psychology and Neuroscience, King’s College London, London, UK
Marinus H. van IJzendoorn
Affiliation:
Department of Psychology, Education, and Child Studies, Erasmus University Rotterdam, Rotterdam, Netherlands Research Department of Clinical, Educational and Health Psychology, Division on Psychology and Language Sciences, Faculty of Brain Sciences, UCL, London, UK
Robbie Duschinsky
Affiliation:
Department of Public Health and Primary Care, University of Cambridge, Cambridge, UK
*
Corresponding author: Barry Coughlan, email: bc471@medschl.cam.ac.uk
Rights & Permissions [Opens in a new window]

Abstract

Children with autism spectrum disorders (ASDs), attention deficit hyperactivity disorder (ADHD) and disinhibited and reactive attachment disorders (RAD/DAD) often experience socioemotional problems. Elucidating a clear picture of these profiles is essential. Strengths and Difficulties Questionnaires (SDQs) were analysed from cohort of children with ASD (n = 1430), ADHD (n = 1193), and RAD/DAD (n = 39). Kruskal–Wallis Tests and network analytic techniques were used to investigate symptom profiles. Children with ASD experienced more emotional problems, peer problems and fewer prosocial behaviours. Children with ADHD and RAD/DAD had higher levels of hyperactivity and conduct problems. Overall, ASD and ADHD networks were highly correlated (rs = 0.82), and we did not observe a statistically significant difference in terms of global Strength.

Type
Regular Article
Creative Commons
Creative Common License - CCCreative Common License - BY
This is an Open Access article, distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution licence (https://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/4.0/), which permits unrestricted re-use, distribution, and reproduction in any medium, provided the original work is properly cited.
Copyright
© The Author(s), 2021. Published by Cambridge University Press

Delineating the clinical profiles associated with autism spectrum disorders (ASDs), attention deficit hyperactivity disorder (ADHD) and disinhibited and reactive attachment disorders (RAD/DADFootnote 1 ) has been a significant concern for healthcare professionals and researchers alike (Davidson et al., Reference Davidson, O’Hare, Mactaggart, Green, Young, Gillberg and Minnis2015; Follan et al., Reference Follan, Anderson, Huline-Dickens, Lidstone, Young, Brown and Minnis2011; Gargaro et al., Reference Gargaro, Rinehart, Bradshaw, Tonge and Sheppard2011; Sadiq et al., Reference Sadiq, Slator, Skuse, Law, Gillberg and Minnis2012). It is not uncommon to hear health care professionals discuss the challenges untangling these conditions in practice (Klein et al., Reference Klein, Damiani-Taraba, Koster, Campbell and Scholz2015; McKenzie & Dallos, Reference McKenzie and Dallos2017). Social and communication atypicalities are a core part of the diagnostic criteria for ASD and RAD/DAD (American Psychiatric Association, 2013; First et al., Reference First, Rebello, Keeley, Bhargava, Dai, Kulygina, Matsumoto, Robles, Stona and Reed2018; World Health Organization, 2018). Although it is not part of the diagnostic criteria, impairments in social functioning are also common in ADHD (Ros & Graziano, Reference Ros and Graziano2018). Furthermore, child institutionalisation (a key risk factor for RAD/DAD; American Psychiatric Association, 2013) is also linked with attentional, cognitive, and socioemotional difficulties (van IJzendoorn et al., Reference van IJzendoorn, Bakermans-Kranenburg, Duschinsky, Fox, Goldman, Gunnar, Johnson, Nelson, Reijman, Skinner, Zeanah and Sonuga-Barke2020) and in some cases quasi-autistic behaviours (Rutter et al., Reference Rutter, Kreppner, Croft, Murin, Colvert, Beckett, Castle and Sonuga-Barke2007). Indeed, each of these conditions, albeit to varying degrees, are associated with socioemotional and behavioural impairments (Totsika et al., Reference Totsika, Hastings, Emerson, Lancaster and Berridge2011; Wehmeier et al., Reference Wehmeier, Schacht and Barkley2010; Charles H. Zeanah et al., Reference Zeanah, Chesher and Boris2016).

Understanding how these socioemotional and behavioural problems manifest is crucial not only in terms of case conceptualisation but also in terms of structuring supports and guiding interventions. For instance, if a child’s neurodevelopmental profile is such that they require movement breaks in class, then that would seem like an entirely reasonable adjustment. However, if the child is leaving the classroom frequently because of peer victimisation, then naturally, the intervention changes. By contrast, if a child with ASD has a neurodevelopmental preference/inclination to avoid eye-contact, it would be a mistake to interpret this through a socioemotional lens as a marker for mood problem such as depression.

For clinicians, this challenge is compounded by the fact that mental healthcare provision tends to be diagnostically-focused. Indeed, recent survey work with healthcare professionals (n=1335), from 92 countries, indicates that practitioners most often use psychiatric classifications for administrative reasons (First et al., Reference First, Rebello, Keeley, Bhargava, Dai, Kulygina, Matsumoto, Robles, Stona and Reed2018). Practitioners were found to regard diagnosis as helpful for assigning a diagnosis and facilitating a shared understanding between practitioners, but less informative in terms of treatment and prognosis (First et al., Reference First, Rebello, Keeley, Bhargava, Dai, Kulygina, Matsumoto, Robles, Stona and Reed2018).

One popular dimensional approach to psychopathology is network analysis (Borsboom & Cramer, Reference Borsboom and Cramer2013; Borsboom et al., Reference Borsboom, Cramer, Schmittmann, Epskamp and Waldorp2011; Cramer et al., Reference Cramer, Waldorp, van der Maas and Borsboom2010). To date, network techniques have been applied to a myriad of topics including depression (Mullarkey et al., Reference Mullarkey, Marchetti and Beevers2019), anxiety (Fried et al., Reference Fried, Epskamp, Nesse, Tuerlinckx and Borsboom2016), post-traumatic stress disorder (Armour et al., Reference Armour, Fried, Deserno, Tsai and Pietrzak2017), personality research (Costantini et al., Reference Costantini, Epskamp, Borsboom, Perugini, Mõttus, Waldorp and Cramer2015), neurodevelopmental conditions including ASD and ADHD (Anderson et al., Reference Anderson, Montazeri and de Bildt2015; Ruzzano et al., Reference Ruzzano, Borsboom and Geurts2015; Silk et al., Reference Silk, Malpas, Beare, Efron, Anderson, Hazell, Jongeling, Nicholson and Sciberras2019), and social psychological assessments of attachment (McWilliams & Fried, Reference McWilliams and Fried2019). Broadly speaking, the network approach contends that psychological constructs such as disorders as expressions of an interconnected system made up of various elements (e.g. symptoms, genes; Borsboom & Cramer, Reference Borsboom and Cramer2013). It follows, therefore, that understanding how the constituent parts of the system interact with one another is important for understanding the phenomenon of interest. In constituting networks, symptoms, attributes or behaviours are conceptualised as ‘nodes’, and the existence of relationships between variables is indicated by edges and the magnitude of the relationships is represented by the thickness of the edge.

The current study aims at exploring socioemotional symptom profiles in a clinical cohort of children with a diagnosis of either ASD, ADHD, or RAD/DAD. First, we will compare Strengths and Difficulties Questionnaire (SDQ) scale scores (i.e. Emotional Problems, Conduct Problems, Hyperactivity, Peer Problems, and Prosocial behaviour) across the groups. Then we will present network models based on the individual items of the SDQ. Our overarching objective is to generate hypotheses that might help signpost future research on sharper differentiation between ASD, ADHD, or RAD/DAD.

Method

Ethical approval

Ethical approval for analysis of anonymised healthcare records using the Clinical Record Interactive System (CRIS) was granted by The Oxfordshire Research Ethics Committee C (08/H606/71). The current project [Project number 18-039] was approved by the CRIS Oversight committee, which is comprised of patients and professionals. As per the CRIS security model, we did not analyse data in cell sizes lower than n=10 to reduce the threat of inadvertent deanonymisation of participants.

Setting

Data for this study were drawn from anonymised mental health records at the South London and Maudsley NHS Foundation Trust (slam) Case Register using the CRIS. SLAM provides mental health supports and services to a catchment area of over 1.2 million people in the following four London boroughs: Croydon, Lambeth, Lewisham and Southwark (Perera et al., Reference Perera, Broadbent, Callard, Chang, Downs, Dutta, Fernandes, Hayes, Henderson, Jackson, Jewell, Kadra, Little, Pritchard, Shetty, Tulloch and Stewart2016). Recent estimates indicate that CRIS accesses over 320,000 patient records. For a description of the cohort profile, see Perera et al. (Reference Perera, Broadbent, Callard, Chang, Downs, Dutta, Fernandes, Hayes, Henderson, Jackson, Jewell, Kadra, Little, Pritchard, Shetty, Tulloch and Stewart2016).

Measures

To explore socioemotional symptomology, we analysed SDQ (Goodman et al., Reference Goodman, Ford, Simmons, Gatward and Meltzer2000) data from the patient records. The SDQ is a popular, freely available screening tool for psychopathology in children. In terms of psychometric properties, the SDQ demonstrates adequate reliability and validity (Goodman, Reference Goodman2001). This questionnaire contains 25 items, each with three response categories (“Not True”, “Somewhat True”, and “Certainly True”). Scores are collected into the following scales: Emotional Problems, Conduct Problems, Hyperactivity, Peer Problems, and Prosocial behaviour (strengths scale). Emotional Problems, Conduct Problems, Hyperactivity and Peer Problems can be combined to calculate Total Difficulties. In the current study, we used data from 2-4-year-old and 4-17-year-old parent/caregiver-report versions. In the current sample, the Cronbach’s alpha value on the subscales was as follows: Emotional Problems = 0.75; Conduct Problems = 0.72; Hyperactivity = 0.74; Peer problems = 0.64; Prosocial behaviour = 0.77.

Data extraction

Data were extracted for all children with a primary diagnosis relating to ASD (Codes: 84.0; 84.1;84.3; 84.5; 84.8; 84.9), ADHD (All codes: F90) and RAD/DAD (i.e. F90.1, F90.2) according to World Health Organisation ICD-10 (World Health Organization, 2010). All patients were aged 17 years or under at the index diagnosis date (i.e. The date diagnosis was entered into the structured fields). Demographic data (i.e. gender and age), diagnostic status, and SDQ scores were extracted from structured fields in CRIS. Extraction took place in June 2020. To support meaningful linkage between SDQ scores and diagnosis, we treated the date of diagnosis as the index date and only selected SDQ scores which were entered six months before diagnosis or up to one month after. It is therefore important to note that we cannot confirm whether the diagnosis was historic or carried over from another service. Additionally, we only included SDQs that were coded as initial assessments. SDQ that were coded as follow up were excluded because these were often conducted following an intervention.

Statistical analysis

First, we collected descriptive statistics, including co-occurrence, for all children with a diagnosis of ASD, ADHD, or RAD and DAD which are presented in the supplementary materials (see S7). We defined co-occurrence as ever having either a primary or secondary diagnosis of ASD, ADHD, RAD/DAD. We decided to collapse RAD and DAD into one category (i.e. RAD/DAD). Several factors contributed to this decision. RAD and DAD are often considered together under the umbrella of attachment difficulties in other studies (e.g. Davidson et al., Reference Davidson, O’Hare, Mactaggart, Green, Young, Gillberg and Minnis2015; Mayes et al., Reference Mayes, Calhoun, Waschbusch and Baweja2017; Minnis et al., Reference Minnis, Messow, McConnachie, Bradshaw, Briggs, Wilson and Gillberg2020). We also judged that combining the two conditions would safeguard against the possibility of small cell sizes and risk of patient deanonymisation.

Where children had more than one SDQ in this timeframe, we selected the SDQ with the fewest missing items and nearest (in months) the index date of diagnosis. In total, k = 289 additional SDQs were removed so that each child was only represented by one SDQ. In addition, n = 382 cases were removed due to missing data or incomplete assessments. As our aim was to explore these symptom profiles, we excluded cases from the analysis where a child had two or more of the conditions of interest (n = 508). See supplementary materials (S8) for participant flow chart.

Item-level SDQ descriptive statistics for each of the groups are presented in supplementary materials. We then compared groups (i.e. ASD, ADHD, RAD/DAD) on SDQ scales (i.e. Emotional Problems, Conduct Problems, Hyperactivity, Peer Problems, Prosocial scale, and Total difficulties) using Kruskal–Willis Tests. Post-hoc analysis was conducted using a Dunn’s test (Dunn, Reference Dunn1964)with Bonferroni adjustment. After performing this analysis, we prepared the data for network analysis.

Many previous network analytical studies of psychopathology have used questionnaire data. An important issue has been how best to consider items which measure overlapping phenomena (Fried & Cramer, Reference Fried and Cramer2017). For instance, taking an example from research on depression, it could be reasonably speculated that “feeling low” might overlap conceptually with “feeling down”. It has been suggested that the presence of highly correlated items might bias the network structure if they are, in fact, measuring the same construct (Fried & Cramer, Reference Fried and Cramer2017). In the current paper, we adopt an approach similar to that of Burger et al. (Reference Burger, Stroebe, Perrig-Chiello, Schut, Spahni, Eisma and Fried2020); we explored inter-correlations between items in the full data set (n=2662) and any symptom pairs with correlations of rs >=0.6 were combined into a single variable. We identified one pair of items which had a correlation r >.6. These were “restless/overactive” (i.e. Hyp1) and “Consistently fidgeting or squirming” (i.e. Hyp 2); we collapsed these variables into one variable (i.e. Hyp1.2) by dividing the sum of the scores by two. Combined scores were then rounded to the nearest integer to ensure variables were discrete. After this, we estimated the networks.

First, we estimated a series of partial correlation networks based on Spearman rank correlations. Specifically, we estimated separate networks for each of the groups. Like Burger et al. (Reference Burger, Stroebe, Perrig-Chiello, Schut, Spahni, Eisma and Fried2020), we used Spearman rank correlations as opposed to polychoric correlations in an effort to obtain more stable network estimations. Next, in line with reference standard psychometric work (S. Epskamp & Fried, Reference Epskamp and Fried2018), we applied Least Absolute Shrinkage and Selection Operarator (LASSO) Regularisation with Extended Bayesian Information Criterium (EBIC) model selection to the networks in an effort to limit the number of spurious edges in the network. Initially, we set the hyperparameter to 0.5. However, preliminary analysis using this hyperparameter estimated a dense regularised network when applied to the ASD group, which might signal reduced specificity (Epskamp et al., Reference Epskamp, Cramer, Waldorp, Schmittmann and Borsboom2012), and a core assumption of the LASSO Regularisation method of model selection is the assumption of sparsity (Epskamp et al., Reference Epskamp, Kruis and Marsman2017). We increased the parameter to 0.55, which did not yield a dense network. For consistency and in preparation for the network comparison test, we also adjusted the hyperparameter for the ADHD group to 0.55.

By contrast, and potentially a consequence of sample size, LASSO Regularisation with a hyperparameter 0.55 yielded an empty network for the RAD/DAD sample. In response, we decreased the hyperparameter to a more liberal .05. As described by Epskamp and Fried (Reference Epskamp and Fried2018), although reducing the LASSO regularisation hyperparameter to lower values is likely to include more spurious edges, it still yields a sparser network than a partial correlation network and is aligned with our hypothesis-generating aims (Epskamp & Fried, Reference Epskamp and Fried2018). Given that there are almost as many nodes as participants in the RAD/DAD group it should be considered to have a more exploratory status than analyses of the ASD and ADHD groups and is therefore presented in the supplementary materials (S 1-6).

Centrality estimates were then collected. Specifically, we collected estimates for Strength, Closeness, and Betweenness. Briefly, the Strength centrality refers to the sum of absolute weights of edges Opsahl et al., Reference Opsahl, Agneessens and Skvoretz2010). Thus, the most central node in terms of Strength is the node with the highest sum of absolute weights of all edges connecting to a node all edges all edges. By contrast, Closeness refers to the is the inverse of the sum of the shortest paths to all other nodes. Betweenness refers to the is the number of times a node lies on the shortest path between other pairs of nodes in the network (Bringmann et al., Reference Bringmann, Elmer, Epskamp, Krause, Schoch, Wichers, Wigman and Snippe2019; Opsahl et al., Reference Opsahl, Agneessens and Skvoretz2010). Recent work suggests that centrality estimates, Betweenness and Closeness, in particular, might be unstable in cross-sectional data (Bringmann et al., Reference Bringmann, Elmer, Epskamp, Krause, Schoch, Wichers, Wigman and Snippe2019). In the current analysis, therefore, we focus predominantly on Strength centrality.

Next, using the approach described by Epskamp et al. (Reference Epskamp, Borsboom and Fried2018) we applied nonparametric bootstrapping techniques to check the stability and accuracy of each of the regularised networks. We bootstrapped 2000 rounds for each network. Bootstrapped edge weights and confidence intervals were visually inspected for each of the networks. We then plotted significant differences between edges. Finally, we used case-dropping Bootstrap to explore the stability of the centrality indices, plotted the stability results, and collected correlation stability co-efficient (Epskamp et al., Reference Epskamp, Borsboom and Fried2018).

Finally, we compared the networks. First, we plotted and compared the network visually. Then we used network comparisons tests (van Borkulo et al., Reference van Borkulo, Boschloo, Kossakowski, Tio, Schoevers, Borsboom and Waldorp2017) to compare the ASD and ADHD networks. Given the uneven sample sizes and the less conservative approach to estimating the RAD/DAD network, we did not run network comparison tests between RAD/DAD and either ASD or ADHD.

Data were prepared, and analysis was conducted in R (R. Core Team, 2020) using the following packages: “psych” (Revelle, Reference Revelle2019); “qgraph” (Epskamp et al., Reference Epskamp, Cramer, Waldorp, Schmittmann and Borsboom2012); “plyr” (Wickham, Reference Wickham2011); “dplyr” (Wickham et al., Reference Wickham, François, Henry and Müller2020), “ggpubr” (Kassambara, Reference Kassambara2020a), “reshape2” (Wickham, Reference Wickham2007); “ggplot2”; (Wickham, Reference Wickham2016); “bootnet” (Epskamp et al., Reference Epskamp, Kruis and Marsman2017); “rstatix” (Kassambara, Reference Kassambara2020b); “OpenMx“(Boker et al., Reference Boker, Neale, Maes, Wilde, Spiegel, Brick, Estabrook, Bates, Mehta, von Oertzen, Gore, Hunter, Hackett, Karch, Brandmaier, Pritikin, Zahery, Kirkpatrick, Wang, Goodrich, Driver, Johnson, Kraft, Wilhelm, Medland, Falk, Keller, Manjunath, Ingber, Shao Voon, Palacios, Yang, Guennebaud and Niesen2020; Hunter, Reference Hunter2018; Neale et al., Reference Neale, Hunter, Pritikin, Zahery, Brick, Kirkpatrick, Estabrook, Bates, Maes and Boker2016; Pritikin et al., Reference Pritikin, Hunter and Boker2015); “tidyverse” (Wickham, Reference Wickham2019); “car” (Fox & Weisberg, Reference Fox and Weisberg2019); “RColorBrewer” (Neuwirth, Reference Neuwirth2014), “jmv” (Selker et al., Reference Selker, Love and Dropmann2020), “MASS” (Venables & Ripley, Reference Venables and Ripley2002); “effects“(Fox & Weisberg, Reference Fox and Weisberg2019); “jmvcore“(Love, Reference Love2020); “igraph” (Csardi & Nepusz, Reference Csardi and Nepusz2006); “EGAnet” (Golino & Christensen, Reference Golino and Christensen2020); “NetworkComparisionTest” (van Borkulo et al., Reference van Borkulo, Boschloo, Kossakowski, Tio, Schoevers, Borsboom and Waldorp2017).

Results

Descriptive statistics and analysis of SDQ scales

Mean values on SDQ subscales can be found in Figure 1. For item-level means, standard deviations, skewness and Kurtosis see supplement (S9). Differences between SDQ scales were explored in a series of Kruskal–Wallis tests and Dunn’s Tests with a Bonferroni correction (see Table 2). For an illustration of scale scores with total difficulties, see supplement (S10)

Error bars represent standard deviations. ASD (n = 1430), ADHD (n = 1193), RAD/DAD (n = 39).

Figure 1. Mean scores SDQ scales.

In terms of demographics, we did not observe a significant association between gender and diagnosis, X 2(NAFootnote 2 , n = 2662) = 8.56, p = 0.1, though each group was heavily skewed male (see Table 1). A Kruskal–Wallis test showed significant group differences in terms of age H(2) = 46.8, p < .001. Post-hoc Dunn’s test with a Bonferroni correction identified significant differences between ASD and ADHD (p < .001), ASD and RAD/DAD (p < .001), and ADHD and RAD/DAD (p < .001). Consequently, we conducted a MANCOVA to examine the effect of age on SDQ subscale scores in the ASD and ADHD groups. We did not include RAD/DAD in the multivariate model due to the differences in sample size. A Shapiro Wilks test indicated that the data were not normally distributed (W = .998, p = 0.007) and a Box’s M-Test for homogeneity of covariance test identified a statistically significant result (X 2 = 334, p < .001), and regression lines were not parallel. Thus, the outcomes of parametric tests should be interpreted with caution. Nevertheless, we observed a significant interaction between age and diagnosis (F = 9.67, df = 5, 2615, p < .001, partial n2 = 0.018). Age (the covariate) was statistically significant (F = 47.856, df = 5, 2615, p < 0.001, partial n2 = 0.084). We identified a statistically significant difference between ASD and ADHD (F = 291.963, df = 5, 2615, p < .001, partial n2 = 0. 358). Children with ASD had higher mean scores for emotional problems, peer problems and lower prosocial social behaviour, whereas those with ADHD had greater conduct problems and hyperactivity. For adjusted means, see supplementary materials S11).

Table 1. SDQ Sample Descriptive Statistics for the SDQ Sample (n = 2662)

SD = Standard Deviations.

Table 2. SDQ Scale scores

Post-hoc Test were a Dunn’s Test with a Bonferroni adjustment. SD = Standard Deviations; N = number of patients; df = Degrees of freedom; ES = effect size; H = H test statistic; p.adj = adjusted p value. A = positive Bartlett’s test.

Network estimates

Regularised network graphs for each condition are shown in Figure 2. There are some conspicuous similarities and differences between the networks. For instance, there is a positive edge between “temper” (Con1) and “unhappy or downhearted” (Emo3) in each of the network structures. By contrast, we can see a strong negative association between “lies or argumentative” (Con4) and “considerate” (Pro1) in the ADHD network but not in the ASD network. Edge Strength centrality estimates for each of the networks are presented in supplementary materials (S12). Centrality estimates including Betweenness and Closeness for each group can be found supplementary materials (S2).

In these network graphs, blue lines represent positive edges, whereas red lines indicate negative edges. The magnitude of the association is represented by the width and vividness of the line. Nodes are coloured according to their respective SDQ scales. Thus blue nodes represent items from the Emotional Problems Scale. Orange nodes represent items from the Conduct Problems Scale. Green nodes represent items from the Hyperactivity Scale; Yellow nodes represent items from the Peer Problems Scale. Pink nodes represent Prosocial behaviour. Please note EBIC hyperparameter for the ASD and ADHD regularised networks is set to = 0.55.

Figure 2. LASSO Regularised Networks for ASD (n = 1430) and ADHD (n = 1193).

Network accuracy and stability

Our first step to explore the stability and accuracy of the network was to explore edge weight stability using nonparametric bootstrapping (number of bootstraps = 2000). In the ASD and ADHD groups, the bootstrapped confidence intervals for edge weights were fairly narrow, and the sample means and the bootstrapped means were close (see supplementary materials: S3-S4). This suggests that both ASD and ADHD networks are fairly stable.

After investigating edge weight stability, we analysed the stability of centrality estimates using case-dropping Bootstrap (number of bootstraps = 2000). First, regarding the ASD network, correlation stability analysis (i.e. CS-coefficients) indicated sound levels of stability for the centrality estimates: Betweenness (CS-coefficient = .517) and Closeness (CS-coefficient = .594). Although these are both acceptable levels of stability, the highest level of stability was reported for Strength (CS-coefficient = .75). Taken together this means that 50% of the data could be excluded and we would still be 95% confident that there would be a correlation of at least 0.7 with all of the original centrality coefficients in the ASD network. Further in terms of Strength centrality, analysis indicates that 75% of the ASD sample could be dropped and we would still be confident (95%) that there would be a correlation with the original sample. In summary, centrality stability analysis indicated that the ASD network structures were stable. Turning to ADHD, centrality stability analysis found comparable CS-coefficients for Betweenness (CS-coefficient = .516), Closeness (CS-coefficient = .672) and Strength = (CS-coefficient = .672). Therefore, like the ASD network, the centrality estimates in the ADHD network appear to be stable. Centrality analysis for each group is presented in supplementary materials (S13).

ASD network strength centrality

Moving now to consider nodes with the strongest Strength centrality in the ASD network, “considerate” (pros1), “worries” (Emo2), “obedient” (Con2), “unhappy or Downhearted” (Emo3) and “helpful when someone is hurt” (pros3) were the nodes with the greatest degree of Strength centrality. Nonparametric bootstrapped differences in Strength centrality for each of the nodes in the ASD network can be found in the supplementary materials (S14). Briefly, “considerate”, “worries”, and “obedient” seemed to be particularly central nodes in the ASD network. By contrast, items such as “better relationship with adults than peers” (prp5), ”aches” (Emo1), and “steals/spiteful” (Con5) appeared to have little influence in terms of Strength centrality. Further, Strength centrality seemed to occupy a distinct role from the mean level of symptomology (see supplementary materials for item-level SDQ scores). That is, the highest scores in terms of mean symptomology tended to be associated with Hyperactivity. Moreover, the three most central nodes (i.e. Pros1, Emo2, and Con2) were 16th, 8th, 12th, respectively.

ADHD network strength centrality

Regarding ADHD, the nodes with the highest Strength centrality were “(un)popular with peers” (prp3), “unhappy or downhearted” (Emo3), “lies/argumentative with adults” (Con4), “fights with peers” (Con3) and “helpful when someone is hurt” (pros3). Non-parametric differences (alpha = 0.05) for Strength centrality are presented in the supplementary materials (S15). Prp3 (“popular with peers”) appeared to have a stronger influence in the network than other items. However, “unhappy or downhearted”, “lies/argumentative with adults”, “fights with peers”, “helpful when someone is hurt”, “considerate”, “temper” each seemed to have comparable influences in terms of Strength centrality. In terms of mean symptomology, the five Hyperactivity items had the highest mean values (and lowest standard deviations) in the ADHD network. By contrast, (un)popularity, the most central node, was 22nd in terms of mean symptomology.

Network comparisons

In order to explore differences between each of the network structures, first, we correlated the adjacency matrices for the networks structures of ASD and ADHD. Regarding ASD and ADHD, we observed a correlation of rs = 0.82, which suggests remarkably strong similarities between these networks. Next, we conducted a permutation test using Network Comparison Test (van Borkulo et al., Reference van Borkulo, Boschloo, Kossakowski, Tio, Schoevers, Borsboom and Waldorp2017) for the ASD and ADHD networks. This permutation test did not identify a significant difference between the two networks in terms of global Strength (p = 0.78). However, a test of network structure invariance was significant (M = 0.20, permutations = 2000, p < .001).

Communities of nodes

Upon visual inspection, broadly speaking, it seems that nodes tended to cluster according to their respective SDQ subscales. To investigate this further, we applied a spin glass algorithm (Reichardt & Bornholdt, Reference Reichardt and Bornholdt2006) using igraph (Csardi & Nepusz, Reference Csardi and Nepusz2006) to construct networks. We the number of possible communities was set to ten. This algorithm identified five communities in the ASD network and only four in the ADHD network. In both graphs, we see a cluster of emotional problems, prosocial behavioural and peer problems. Yet we also see some loose clustering of hyperactivity and conduct problems in the ASD network and a more pervasive clustering of these items in the ADHD network (see supplementary materials S16).

Subgroup analysis: Gender and age

We also conducted a subgroup analysis to explore whether gender or age had a significant impact on the network structure. First, ASD networks for male (n = 1062) and non-male (n = 368) were constructed. Comparison of the respective adjacency matrices for the ASDmale and ASDnon-male found a strong correlation (rs = 0.76). In addition, the permutation did not identify a significant difference between the networks in terms of global Strength (p = 0.59). Along similar lines, a test of network structure invariance was not significant (M = 1.97, permutations = 2000, p = 0.59). Therefore, it seems the networks were reasonably similar. Yet these should be interpreted with some caution due to differences in sample size and relatively small n in the non-male group. ADHD male (n = 941) and ADHD non-male (n = 252) were also constructed. Between these networks, the correlation adjacency matrices were moderate to high (rs = 0.67). Permutation tests on the estimated network objects selected an empty network, and again there were notable differences in sample size between the two groups and the non-male network did not appear to be stable. As such, these networks should be interpreted with caution.

For both ASD and ADHD, age was dichotomised into two groups: i) nine years and under ii) ten years and over. In terms of ASD, network comparisons permutation test (van Borkulo et al., Reference van Borkulo, Boschloo, Kossakowski, Tio, Schoevers, Borsboom and Waldorp2017) not identify a significant difference in global Strength (p = 0.83) between the nine and under group(n = 576) and the ten and over (n = 854) groups, and there was a strong correlation between the age groups (rs = 0.81). Similarly, we did not observe a significant difference in global Strength (p = 0.62) between the ADHD nine years and under (n = 636) and the ten years and older groups (n = 557). Again, there was a strong correlation between the networks (rs = 0.78). See supplementary materials for subgroup networks, centrality, and stability details (S17-36).

Discussion

The aim of this study was to explore socioemotional and behavioural symptom profiles in a clinical cohort of children with a diagnosis of ASD, ADHD, or RAD/DAD. We identified differences in SDQ scales between the groups, with children with ASD experiencing more emotional problems, difficulties with peers and few prosocial strengths than those with ADHD or RAD/DAD. By contrast, children with ADHD and RAD experienced by hyperactivity and conduct problems than their peers with ASD. Regarding ASD and ADHD, the diagnosis seemed to have a considerable impact on the SDQ profile. We then applied network analytic methods to build network models of the symptom profiles in the ASD and ADHD groups. We identified some differences in terms of central nodes in each of these networks; however, overall, there was a strong correlation between the ASD and ADHD networks. This was, to our knowledge, the first study to investigate socioemotional profiles in children with ASD and ADHD using a network approach.

Concerning SDQ scales, we did not observe significant differences between either ASD, ADHD, or RAD/DAD in terms of total difficulties. We did, however, identify some areas of divergence. For instance, children with ASD were more likely to experience emotional problems, peer problems, and have fewer prosocial strengths than children with ADHD or RAD/DAD. This is consistent with previous work comparing SDQ profiles in ASD and ADHD (Iizuka et al., Reference Iizuka, Yamashita, Nagamitsu, Yamashita, Araki, Ohya, Hara, Shibuya, Kakuma and Matsuishi2010; Russell et al., Reference Russell, Rodgers and Ford2013). The finding that children with ASD had significantly more peer problems and fewer prosocial behaviours than children with RAD/DAD is potentially illuminating. Elsewhere, Davidson et al. (Reference Davidson, O’Hare, Mactaggart, Green, Young, Gillberg and Minnis2015) has described the difficulties in social functioning between RAD/DAD and ASD as “superficially similar”. Yet, our findings suggest that even a brief screening assessment was able to identify significant differences in symptom profiles. One explanation might be that the SDQ measures symptoms that are more direct indices of ASD than RAD/DAD (e.g. “shares readily with other children”). Nevertheless, this does indicate that some differences between attachment-related and ASD profiles can be identified even using brief screening tools.

Considering that RAD/DAD are conditions diagnosed based on impaired social functioning, it was striking that we did not observe statistically significant differences between RAD/DAD and ADHD in terms of peer problems and prosocial behaviour. Although both groups did seem to experience elevated peer problems according to the SDQ thresholds (https://www.sdqinfo.org/py/sdqinfo/c0.py) it might have been expected that social issues would be more pronounced in RAD/DAD. However, peer problems are well documented in the ADHD literature (Ros & Graziano, Reference Ros and Graziano2018). It might be the case that children with a diagnosis of RAD/DAD might experience comparable difficulties in this regard. It is likely that other symptoms, not captured by the SDQ, help differentiate these presentations in practice (e.g. cuddles with strangers; Follan et al., Reference Follan, Anderson, Huline-Dickens, Lidstone, Young, Brown and Minnis2011). It follows, therefore, that it is attachment-specific behaviours that may differentiate RAD/DAD from ASD and ADHD. However, core to the theoretical conceptualisation of RAD/DAD is the idea that the social and emotional difficulties have relationship-specific elements (e.g. minimal comfort-seeking towards primary caregiver) but also extend beyond the context of the relationship (e.g. limited positive affect; American Psychiatric Association, 2013). Therefore, the current findings raise a question about the extent to which social and emotional difficulties associated with RAD/DAD extend beyond the relationship-specific context.

Turning to conduct problems, these were significantly higher in the ADHD and RAD/DAD groups than in the ASD group. Mean values for ADHD (M = 5.26) and RAD/DAD (M = 5.05) are considered ‘high’ according to SDQ scoring criteria (https://www.sdqinfo.org/py/sdqinfo/c0.py). Taken together, the finding that children with ADHD experience greater levels of hyperactivity and conduct problems in comparison to peers with ASD appear to be in concert with previous work on this topic (Russell et al., Reference Russell, Rodgers and Ford2013). One explanation for the increased rates of conduct problems in the ADHD group might be that hyperkinetic conduct disorder was a distinct subgroup of ADHD in ICD-10 (World Health Organization, 2018). The reason for the elevated rates of conduct problems in children with RAD/DAD is perhaps less clear. Previous work (Allen & Schuengel, Reference Allen and Schuengel2020; Woolgar & Baldock, Reference Woolgar and Baldock2015) has drawn attention to the fact that conduct problems are sometimes mistakenly identified as markers for attachment disorders and RAD, in particular. As such, it would be beneficial for future studies to explore how practitioners perceive the role of conduct problems in the respective phenotypes of RAD and DAD.

The results of the network analysis tests help shed light on the relationships between socioemotional symptoms in children. Taking each of the networks in turn: in the ASD network, the degree to which the child was described as “considerate” (pros1) was the most central aspect of the network in terms of Strength centrality. As we can see from Figure 2, being characterised as considerate was, to varying degrees, positively associated with all of the other items on the prosocial scale and was negatively associated with distinct issues such as problems with “obedience” (Con2), “having at least one good friend” (prp2), “difficulties managing temper” (Con1), “thinking things out before acting” (Hyp4). This is particularly intriguing as few topics in ASD research have generated more debate and discussion than issues surrounding empathy (Baron-Cohen, Reference Baron-Cohen2009; Baron-Cohen & Wheelwright, Reference Baron-Cohen and Wheelwright2004; Fletcher-Watson & Bird, Reference Fletcher-Watson and Bird2020). Elsewhere, Fletcher-Watson and Happé (Reference Fletcher-Watson and Happé2019) draw an important distinction between feeling and expressing empathy in ASD. Though we are unable to draw conclusions about causality, our findings might tentatively suggest that support and interventions designed to promote expressions of empathy in children with ASD might be particularly helpful targets for intervention.

In terms of the ADHD network, peer (un)popularity (prp3) had the highest levels of Strength centrality. Here we see robust associations between peer popularity and other items from the peer problems scale, a negative association between (un)popularity and sharing, and a positive association with fighting with peers. Yet a number of other nodes appeared to be almost as influential in terms of Strength centrality. For instance, “unhappiness or downhearted” (emo3) seemed to occupy a critical role in the network. Moreover, in addition to links with other items from the emotion problems scale, we also see positive associations between unhappiness and several items from the conduct problems scale including “temper” (con1) and “fighting with peers” (con3), as well peer problems such as “solitary play” (prp1) and “being victimised by other children” (prp4). The link between conduct and mood problems is well established in the child psychiatric literature (Angold & Costello, Reference Angold and Costello1993; Polier et al., Reference Polier, Vloet, Herpertz-Dahlmann, Laurens and Hodgins2012). Analysis of the SDQ subscales suggested that children with ADHD had close to average or slightly above average emotional problems. And yet in terms of centrality, it seems that unhappiness had an important role in terms of Strength. However, on a more general note, it signals an advantage of network analytic models, which is to identify meaningful connections between symptoms that might have been otherwise overlooked. Lower levels of wellbeing and happiness are commonly associated with ADHD (Peasgood et al., Reference Peasgood, Bhardwaj, Biggs, Brazier, Coghill, Cooper, Daley, De Silva, Harpin, Hodgkins, Nadkarni, Setyawan and Sonuga-Barke2016; Stickley et al., Reference Stickley, Koyanagi, Takahashi, Ruchkin, Inoue, Yazawa and Kamio2018). Therefore, it seems that identifying the drivers of unhappiness in ADHD could be a particularly helpful target for intervention.

Previous network analytic work has found variations in symptom profiles in children with ADHD (Silk et al., Reference Silk, Malpas, Beare, Efron, Anderson, Hazell, Jongeling, Nicholson and Sciberras2019). One unexpected finding was that although the mean scores for Hyperactivity were high in the ADHD group, the ADHD items were not particularly influential in terms of Strength centrality. Indeed, by and large, nor were they particularly strong nodes in the ASD networks. Moreover, ASD items such as “gets on better with adults” were not highly central. It could be argued that these features have less influence on other areas because they are distinctly neurodevelopmental, whereas the other areas are socioemotional. That would be an intriguing finding and would lend support to nosological assertions regarding the nature of hyperactivity (i.e. distinctly neurodevelopmental; American Psychiatric Association, 2013; World Health Organization, 2018). Then again, some items in the ASD network regarding prosocial behaviour (e.g. “considerate”) were central in the network and are, according to nosological descriptions, neurodevelopmental. Delineating the boundaries between what is socioemotional, behavioural and neurodevelopmental is one of the core challenges in differential conceptualisation.

Within this context, it is interesting to note that the community analysis identified five communities of nodes in the ASD sample and four in the ADHD group. In both networks, items from the emotional problems, peer problems and prosocial behaviour scales seemed to cluster into their respective scales. Indeed, on these scales, there were significant differences between the ASD and ADHD group. Yet, regarding conduct problems and hyperactivity, there was a more complicated picture. In the ASD sample, we see a clustering of hyperactivity items and two items from the conduct problems scale: “temper” and “(dis)obedience”. Meanwhile, in the ADHD group, all items from the conduct problems scale and the hyperactivity scales seemed to cluster together. This could reflect a hyperkinetic conduct constellation for both groups, with some differences regarding latent conduct disorder symptoms such as lying and stealing. This raises the question of whether conduct items could be sorted along the lines of oppositional and conduct behaviours.

It was somewhat unanticipated that such high correlations would be observed in the adjacency matrices between the ASD and the ADHD groups. A network permutations test (van Borkulo et al., Reference van Borkulo, Boschloo, Kossakowski, Tio, Schoevers, Borsboom and Waldorp2017) did not find significant differences between the ASD and ADHD groups in terms of global Strength. This was surprising considering that significant differences were found in terms of the SDQ subscales. One explanation is that although the subscales are different, the ways in which the symptoms interact in terms of global Strength is similar. And yet a test of network structure invariance identified a significant difference in terms of structure. When thinking differentially about these profiles; therefore, it might be helpful to think about the qualitative intensity of behaviours rather than strictly the presence of absence of certain features. Another possibility is that the high correlations between SDQ subscale items within each network disguised more subtle significant differences in the network structures. However, checks were conducted for highly correlated items before running the analysis.

Limitations

Firstly, given the sample size for RAD/DAD and the CRIS guidance regarding small cell sizes, we were limited in the number of covariates we could include. It is possible that groups might have differed on other factors such as ethnicity and socioeconomic status or involvement in the care system. We think this would be a crucial area of investigation for future work on the topic of differential diagnosis. Elsewhere (Woolgar & Baldock, Reference Woolgar and Baldock2015) and others (Allen, Reference Allen2016; Allen & Schuengel, Reference Allen and Schuengel2020) have spotlighted problems with the overuse of attachment concepts in children in care. Thus, it would be illuminating for future work to explore whether symptom profiles covary by involvement in the care system. One way to do this would be to compare networks of children with a diagnosis of ASD, ADHD, and RAD/DAD who have also been involved with the care system.

Another limitation was the number of RAD/DAD cases, which led us to follow common practice and combine RAD and DAD into one group. Yet it is generally accepted that that RAD is more aligned with internalising difficulties, whereas DAD is more aligned with externalising problems (Zeanah & Gleason, Reference Zeanah and Gleason2015). Further, recent work suggests that although both are associated with lower general social functioning and social competence, there might be relevant differences, such as peer victimisation (Guyon-Harris et al., Reference Guyon-Harris, Humphreys, Fox, Nelson and Zeanah2019). Moreover, work conducted by Lehmann et al. (Reference Lehmann, Breivik, Heiervang, Havik and Havik2016) indicated a two factor model of socioemotional and behavioural profiles on the SDQ for children with RAD and DAD.

Another limitation is that we only included children with a diagnosis entered into structured fields. Thus we do not have context for the diagnosis being entered into the structured field and the quality of the diagnostic assessment. In addition, the diagnosis might have, for instance, been historical or the diagnosis might have been made in another service. Furthermore, the children might have had other co-occurring mental health conditions and thus might have contributed to the symptom profiles. Finally, due to practical constraints, it was not possible to extract data from unstructured fields such as clinical notes.

An additional limitation of this study is that children who had a dual diagnosis of ASD, ADHD, or RAD/DAD were excluded. This decision was made in order to help us better delineate between profiles and increase confidence in the findings. Yet, at the same time, this does impose certain limitations on the generalisability. It would be beneficial for future work to explore whether children with ADHD and ASD, for instance, experienced a particular profile of socioemotional and behaviours difficulties that is distinct from children without a dual diagnosis of these conditions.

Finally, the data in this study comes from a clinical cohort of children attending mental health services. Therefore, our results might not be applicable to those not involved with mental health services. Other mental health factors (Luo et al., Reference Luo, Weibman, Halperin and Li2019; Simonoff et al., Reference Simonoff, Pickles, Charman, Chandler, Loucas and Baird2008; Thapar & Cooper, Reference Thapar and Cooper2016) might have been relevant in terms of shaping the socioemotional and behavioural profiles in a community sample.

Conclusions

This study explored socio-emotional symptom profiles in a clinical cohort of children with a diagnosis of ASD, ADHD, and RAD/DAD. Despite presenting with comparable levels of ‘Total difficulties”, we observed significant differences between the subscales. Emotional problems, peer problems, and fewer prosocial strengths were more aligned with autism, whereas hyperactivity and conduct problems played a more significant role in ADHD and RAD/DAD. We observed considerable effect sizes in terms of ADHD and ASD and SDQ subscales. Yet ASD and ADHD had comparable network structures. Taken together, this illustrates the complex nature of socioemotional and behavioural difficulties in children with ASD, ADHD, RAD/DAD and whilst also identify some specific lines of divergence.

Supplementary material

For supplementary material accompanying this paper visit https://doi.org/10.1017/S0954579421000882

Data availability statement

CRIS is supported by the NIHR Biomedical Research Centre for Mental Health BRC Nucleus at the South London and Maudsley NHS Foundation Trust and Institute of Psychiatry, Psychology and Neuroscience, King’s College London jointly funded by the Guy’s and St Thomas’ Trustees and the South London and Maudsley Trustees. Data are owned by a third party, Maudsley Biomedical Research Centre (BRC) CRIS tool, which provides access to anonymised data derived from SLaM electronic medical records. These data can only be accessed by permitted individuals from within a secure firewall (i.e. the data cannot be sent elsewhere), in the same manner as the authors. For more information please contact: .

Author contributions

All authors contributed to the development of the study concept and contributed to the study design. Data analysis was conducted by BC. BC drafted the initial manuscript. MW, MvIJ and RD authors contributed critical revisions. The final draft was approved by all the authors.

Funding statement

The authors wish to thank NIHR School for Primary Care Research (RG94577) and Wellcome Trust (WT103343MA; 218025/A/19/Z) for their support for work on this paper. Marinus H. van IJzendoorn is supported by an award from the Netherlands Organization for Scientific Research (Spinoza prize) and he is additionally supported by the Gravitation program of the Dutch Ministry of Education, Culture, and Science and the Netherlands Organization for Scientific Research (NWO grant number 024.001.003). The views expressed are those of the authors and not necessarily those of the CRN, NHS, the NIHR or the Department of Health.

Conflicts of interest

No conflicts of interest to declare.

Footnotes

1 Until recently, disinhibited and reactive attachment disorders were considered part of the same condition. However recent work does suggest that they are distinct phenomenon. Nevertheless much of the research comparing symptom profiles across these conditions (i.e. RAD/DAD and ASD or ADHD) has used the RAD/DAD criteria. Thus, in this study the term RAD/DAD is used to refer to children with either condition. This is discussed further in the limitations.

2 Simulated p-value (2000 replicates).

References

Allen, B. (2016). A RADical idea: A call to eliminate “attachment disorder” and “attachment therapy” from the clinical lexicon. Evidence-Based Practice in Child and Adolescent Mental Health, 1, 6071. https://doi.org/10.1080/23794925.2016.1172945 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Allen, B., & Schuengel, C. (2020). Attachment disorders diagnosed by community practitioners: A replication and extension. Child and Adolescent Mental Health, 25, 410. https://doi.org/10.1111/camh.12338CrossRefGoogle Scholar
American Psychiatric Association. (2013). Diagnostic and statistical manual of mental disorders (5th ed.). Arlington, VA: American Psychiatric Publishing.Google Scholar
Anderson, G. M., Montazeri, F., & de Bildt, A. (2015). Network approach to autistic traits: Group and subgroup analyses of ADOS item scores. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 45, 31153132. https://doi.org/10.1007/s10803-015-2537-z CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Angold, A., & Costello, E. (1993). Depressive comorbidity in children and adolescents. American Journal of Psychiatry, 150, 17791791.Google ScholarPubMed
Armour, C., Fried, E. I., Deserno, M. K., Tsai, J., & Pietrzak, R. H. (2017). A network analysis of DSM-5 posttraumatic stress disorder symptoms and correlates in US military veterans. Journal of Anxiety Disorders, 45, 4959. https://doi.org/10.31234-2Fosf.io-2Fp69m7 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Baron-Cohen, S. (2009). Autism: The empathizing-systemizing (E-S) theory. Annals of the New York Academy of Sciences, 1156, 6880. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1749-6632.2009.04467.x CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Baron-Cohen, S., & Wheelwright, S. (2004). The Empathy Quotient: An Investigation of Adults with Asperger Syndrome or High Functioning Autism, and Normal Sex Differences. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 34, 163175. https://doi.org/10.1023/B:JADD.0000022607.19833.00 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Boker, S. M., Neale, M. C., Maes, H. H., Wilde, M. J., Spiegel, M., Brick, T. R., Estabrook, R., Bates, T. C., Mehta, P., von Oertzen, T., Gore, R. J., Hunter, M. D., Hackett, D. C., Karch, J., Brandmaier, A. M., Pritikin, J. N., Zahery, M., Kirkpatrick, R. M., Wang, Y., Goodrich, B., Driver, C., Massachusetts Institute of Technology, Johnson, S., Association for Computing Machinery, Kraft, D., Wilhelm, S., Medland, S., Falk, C. F., Keller, M., Manjunath, B. G., The Regents of the University of California, Ingber, L., Shao Voon, W., Palacios, J., Yang, J., Guennebaud, G., & Niesen, J. (2020). OpenMx 2.18.1 User Guide.Google Scholar
Borsboom, D., & Cramer, A. O. (2013). Network analysis: An integrative approach to the structure of psychopathology. Annual Review of Clinical Psychology, 9, 91121. https://doi.org/10.1146/annurev-clinpsy-050212-185608 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Borsboom, D., Cramer, A. O. J., Schmittmann, V. D., Epskamp, S., & Waldorp, L. J. (2011). The small world of psychopathology. PLOS ONE, 6, e27407. https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0027407CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Bringmann, L. F., Elmer, T., Epskamp, S., Krause, R. W., Schoch, D., Wichers, M., Wigman, J. T. W., & Snippe, E. (2019). What do centrality measures measure in psychological networks? Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 128, 892903. https://doi.org/10.1037/abn0000446 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Burger, J., Stroebe, M. S., Perrig-Chiello, P., Schut, H. A. W., Spahni, S., Eisma, M. C., & Fried, E. I. (2020). Bereavement or breakup: Differences in networks of depression. Journal of Affective Disorders, 267, 18. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jad.2020.01.157 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Costantini, G., Epskamp, S., Borsboom, D., Perugini, M., Mõttus, R., Waldorp, L. J., & Cramer, A. O. (2015). State of the aRt personality research: A tutorial on network analysis of personality data in R. Journal of Research in Personality, 54, 1329. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jrp.2014.07.003 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Cramer, A. O., Waldorp, L. J., van der Maas, H. L., & Borsboom, D. (2010). Comorbidity: A network perspective. The Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 33, 137150; discussion 150-193. https://doi.org/10.1017/s0140525x09991567 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Csardi, G., & Nepusz, T. (2006). The igraph software package for complex network research. InterJournal, Complex Systems, 16951695. Retrieved from http://igraph.org Google Scholar
Davidson, C., O’Hare, A., Mactaggart, F., Green, J., Young, D., Gillberg, C., & Minnis, H. (2015). Social relationship difficulties in autism and reactive attachment disorder: Improving diagnostic validity through structured assessment. Research in Developmental Disabilities, 40, 6372. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.ridd.2015.01.007 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Dunn, O. J. (1964). Multiple comparisons using rank sums. Technometrics, 6, 241252. https://doi.org/10.1080/00401706.1964.10490181 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Epskamp, S., & Fried, E. I. (2018). A tutorial on regularized partial correlation networks. Psychol Methods, 23, 617634. https://doi.org/10.1037/met0000167 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Epskamp, S., Borsboom, D., & Fried, E. I. (2018). Estimating psychological networks and their accuracy: A tutorial paper. Behavior Research Methods, 50, 195212. https://doi.org/10.3758/s13428-017-0862-1 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Epskamp, S., Cramer, A. O., Waldorp, L. J., Schmittmann, V. D., & Borsboom, D. (2012). qgraph: Network visualizations of relationships in psychometric data. Journal of Statistical Software, 48, 118. https://doi.org/10.18637/jss.v048.i04 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Epskamp, S., Kruis, J., & Marsman, M. (2017). Estimating psychopathological networks: Be careful what you wish for. PLOS ONE, 12, e0179891. https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0179891 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
First, M. B., Rebello, T. J., Keeley, J. W., Bhargava, R., Dai, Y., Kulygina, M., Matsumoto, C., Robles, R., Stona, A. C., & Reed, G. M. (2018). Do mental health professionals use diagnostic classifications the way we think they do? A global survey. World Psychiatry: Official Journal of the World Psychiatric Association (WPA), 17, 187195. https://doi.org/10.1002/wps.20525 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Fletcher-Watson, S., & Bird, G. (2020). Autism and empathy: What are the real links? Autism, 24, 36. https://doi.org/10.1177/1362361319883506 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Fletcher-Watson, S., & Happé, F. (2019). Autism: A New Introduction to Psychological Theory and Current Debate. Routledge.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Follan, M., Anderson, S., Huline-Dickens, S., Lidstone, E., Young, D., Brown, G., & Minnis, H. (2011). Discrimination between attention deficit hyperactivity disorder and reactive attachment disorder in school aged children. Research in Developmental Disabilities, 32, 520526. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.ridd.2010.12.031 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Fox, J., & Weisberg, S. (2019). An R Companion to Applied Regression. Sage.Google Scholar
Fried, E. I., & Cramer, A. O. J. (2017). Moving forward: Challenges and directions for psychopathological network theory and methodology. Perspectives on Psychological Science, 12, 9991020. https://doi.org/10.1177/1745691617705892 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Fried, E. I., Epskamp, S., Nesse, R. M., Tuerlinckx, F., & Borsboom, D. (2016). What are’good’depression symptoms? Comparing the centrality of DSM and non-DSM symptoms of depression in a network analysis. Journal of Affective Disorders, 189, 314320. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jad.2015.09.005 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Gargaro, B. A., Rinehart, N. J., Bradshaw, J. L., Tonge, B. J., & Sheppard, D. M. (2011). Autism and ADHD: How far have we come in the comorbidity debate? Neuroscience & Biobehavioral Reviews, 35, 10811088. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neubiorev.2010.11.002 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Golino, H., & Christensen, A. (2020). EGAnet: Exploratory Graph Analysis - A Framework for Estimating the Number of Dimensions in Multivariate Data Using Network Psychometrics. Retrieved from https://CRAN.R-project.org/package=EGAnet Google Scholar
Goodman, R. (2001). Psychometric properties of the strengths and difficulties questionnaire. Journal of the American Academy of Child and Adolescent Psychiatry, 40, 13371345. https://doi.org/10.1097/00004583-200111000-00015 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Goodman, R., Ford, T., Simmons, H., Gatward, R., & Meltzer, H. (2000). Using the strengths and difficulties questionnaire (SDQ) to screen for child psychiatric disorders in a community sample. The British Journal of Psychiatry, 177, 534539. https://doi.org/10.1192/bjp.177.6.534 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Guyon-Harris, K. L., Humphreys, K. L., Fox, N. A., Nelson, C. A., & Zeanah, C. H. (2019). Signs of attachment disorders and social functioning among early adolescents with a history of institutional care. Child Abuse & Neglect, 88, 96106. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.chiabu.2018.11.005 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Hunter, M. D. (2018). State space modeling in an open source, modular, structural equation modeling environment. Structural Equation Modeling, 25, 307324. https://doi.org/10.1080/10705511.2017.1369354 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Iizuka, C., Yamashita, Y., Nagamitsu, S., Yamashita, T., Araki, Y., Ohya, T., Hara, M., Shibuya, I., Kakuma, T., & Matsuishi, T. (2010). Comparison of the strengths and difficulties questionnaire (SDQ) scores between children with high-functioning autism spectrum disorder (HFASD) and attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder (AD/HD). Brain & Development, 32, 609612. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.braindev.2009.09.009 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Kassambara, A. (2020a). ggpubr: ggplot2 Based Publication Ready Plots. Retrieved from https://rpkgs.datanovia.com/ggpubr/ Google Scholar
Kassambara, A. (2020b). rstatix: Pipe-Friendly Framework for Basic Statistical Tests. Retrieved from https://rpkgs.datanovia.com/rstatix/ Google Scholar
Klein, B., Damiani-Taraba, G., Koster, A., Campbell, J., & Scholz, C. (2015). Diagnosing attention-deficit hyperactivity disorder (ADHD) in children involved with child protection services: Are current diagnostic guidelines acceptable for vulnerable populations? Child Care, Health and Development, 41, 178185. https://doi.org/10.1111/cch.12168 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Lehmann, S., Breivik, K., Heiervang, E. R., Havik, T., & Havik, O. E. (2016). Reactive attachment disorder and disinhibited social engagement disorder in school-aged foster children--a confirmatory approach to dimensional measures. Journal of Abnormal Child Psychology, 44, 445457. https://doi.org/10.1007/s10802-015-0045-4 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Love, J. (2020). jmvcore: Dependencies for the jamovi Framework. Retrieved from https://www.jamovi.org Google Scholar
Luo, Y. Y., Weibman, D., Halperin, J. M., & Li, X. B. (2019). A review of heterogeneity in attention deficit/hyperactivity disorder (ADHD). Frontiers in Human Neuroscience, 13, 12. https://doi.org/10.3389/fnhum.2019.00042 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Mayes, S. D., Calhoun, S. L., Waschbusch, D. A., & Baweja, R. (2017). Autism and reactive attachment/disinhibited social engagement disorders: Co-occurrence and differentiation. Clinical Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 22, 620631. https://doi.org/10.1177/1359104516678039 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
McKenzie, R., & Dallos, R. (2017). Autism and attachment difficulties: Overlap of symptoms, implications and innovative solutions. Clinical Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 22, 632648. https://doi.org/10.1177/1359104517707323 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
McWilliams, L. A., & Fried, E. I. (2019). Reconceptualizing adult attachment relationships: A network perspective. Personal Relationships, 26, 2141. https://doi.org/10.1111/pere.12263 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Minnis, H., Messow, C.-M., McConnachie, A., Bradshaw, P., Briggs, A., Wilson, P., & Gillberg, C. (2020). Autism and attachment disorder symptoms in the general population: Prevalence, overlap, and burden. Developmental Child Welfare, 2, 3751. https://doi.org/10.1177/2516103220902778 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Mullarkey, M. C., Marchetti, I., & Beevers, C. G. (2019). Using network analysis to identify central symptoms of adolescent depression. Journal of Clinical Child & Adolescent Psychology, 48, 656668. https://doi.org/10.31234/osf.io/kp7xd CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Neale, M. C., Hunter, M. D., Pritikin, J. N., Zahery, M., Brick, T. R., Kirkpatrick, R. M., Estabrook, R., Bates, T. C., Maes, H. H., & Boker, S. M. (2016). OpenMx 2.0: Extended structural equation and statistical modeling. Psychometrika, 81, 535549. https://doi.org/10.1007/s11336-014-9435-8 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Neuwirth, E. (2014). RColorBrewer: ColorBrewer Palettes. Retrieved from https://CRAN.R-project.org/package=RColorBrewer Google Scholar
Opsahl, T., Agneessens, F., & Skvoretz, J. (2010). Node centrality in weighted networks: Generalizing degree and shortest paths. Social Networks, 32, 245251. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.socnet.2010.03.006 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Peasgood, T., Bhardwaj, A., Biggs, K., Brazier, J. E., Coghill, D., Cooper, C. L., Daley, D., De Silva, C., Harpin, V., Hodgkins, P., Nadkarni, A., Setyawan, J., & Sonuga-Barke, E. J. S. (2016). The impact of ADHD on the health and well-being of ADHD children and their siblings. European Child & Adolescent Psychiatry, 25, 12171231. https://doi.org/10.1007/s00787-016-0841-6 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Perera, G., Broadbent, M., Callard, F., Chang, C.-K., Downs, J., Dutta, R., Fernandes, A., Hayes, R. D., Henderson, M., Jackson, R., Jewell, A., Kadra, G., Little, R., Pritchard, M., Shetty, H., Tulloch, A., & Stewart, R. (2016). Cohort profile of the South London and Maudsley NHS Foundation Trust Biomedical Research Centre (SLaM BRC) Case Register: Current status and recent enhancement of an Electronic Mental Health Record-derived data resource. BMJ Open, 6, e008721. https://doi.org/10.1136/bmjopen-2015-008721 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Polier, G. G., Vloet, T. D., Herpertz-Dahlmann, B., Laurens, K. R., & Hodgins, S. (2012). Comorbidity of conduct disorder symptoms and internalising problems in children: Investigating a community and a clinical sample. European Child & Adolescent Psychiatry, 21, 3138. https://doi.org/10.1007/s00787-011-0229-6 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Pritikin, J. N., Hunter, M. D., & Boker, S. M. (2015). Modular open-source software for Item Factor Analysis. Educational and Psychological Measurement, 75, 458474. https://doi.org/10.1177/0013164414554615 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
R. Core Team. (2020). R: A Language and Environment for Statistical Computing. Retrieved from https://www.R-project.org/ Google Scholar
Reichardt, J., & Bornholdt, S. (2006). Statistical mechanics of community detection. Physical Review E, 74, 016110. https://doi.org/10.1103/PhysRevE.74.016110 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Revelle, W. (2019). psych: Procedures for Personality and Psychological Research, Northwestern University, Evanston, Illinois, USA,. Retrieved from https://CRAN.R-project.org/package=psych Google Scholar
Ros, R., & Graziano, P. A. (2018). Social functioning in children with or at risk for attention deficit/hyperactivity disorder: A meta-analytic review. Journal of Clinical Child & Adolescent Psychology, 47, 213235. https://doi.org/10.1080/15374416.2016.1266644 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Russell, G., Rodgers, L. R., & Ford, T. (2013). The Strengths and Difficulties Questionnaire as a predictor of parent-reported diagnosis of autism spectrum disorder and attention deficit hyperactivity disorder. PLOS ONE, 8. https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0080247CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Rutter, M., Kreppner, J., Croft, C., Murin, M., Colvert, E., Beckett, C., Castle, J., & Sonuga-Barke, E. (2007). Early adolescent outcomes of institutionally deprived and non-deprived adoptees. III. Quasi-autism. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, Applied Disciplines, 48, 12001207. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1469-7610.2007.01792.x CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Ruzzano, L., Borsboom, D., & Geurts, H. M. (2015). Repetitive behaviors in autism and obsessive–compulsive disorder: New perspectives from a network analysis. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 45, 192202. https://doi.org/10.1007/s10803-014-2204-9 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Sadiq, F. A., Slator, L., Skuse, D., Law, J., Gillberg, C., & Minnis, H. (2012). Social use of language in children with reactive attachment disorder and autism spectrum disorders. European Child & Adolescent Psychiatry, 21, 267276. https://doi.org/10.1007/s00787-012-0259-8 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Selker, R., Love, J., & Dropmann, D. (2020). jmv: The jamovi Analyses. Retrieved from https://CRAN.R-project.org/package=jmv Google Scholar
Silk, T. J., Malpas, C. B., Beare, R., Efron, D., Anderson, V., Hazell, P., Jongeling, B., Nicholson, J. M., & Sciberras, E. (2019). A network analysis approach to ADHD symptoms: More than the sum of its parts. PLOS ONE, 14, e0211053. https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0211053 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Simonoff, E., Pickles, A., Charman, T., Chandler, S., Loucas, T., & Baird, G. (2008). Psychiatric disorders in children with autism spectrum disorders: Prevalence, comorbidity, and associated factors in a population-derived sample. Journal of the American Academy of Child and Adolescent Psychiatry, 47, 921929. https://doi.org/10.1097/CHI.0b013e318179964f CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Stickley, A., Koyanagi, A., Takahashi, H., Ruchkin, V., Inoue, Y., Yazawa, A., & Kamio, Y. (2018). Attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder symptoms and happiness among adults in the general population. Psychiatry Research, 265, 317323. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.psychres.2018.05.004 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Thapar, A., & Cooper, M. (2016). Attention deficit hyperactivity disorder. Lancet, 387, 12401250. https://doi.org/10.1016/s0140-6736(15)00238-x CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Totsika, V., Hastings, R. P., Emerson, E., Lancaster, G. A., & Berridge, D. M. (2011). A population-based investigation of behavioural and emotional problems and maternal mental health: Associations with autism spectrum disorder and intellectual disability. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 52, 9199. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1469-7610.2010.02295.x CrossRefGoogle Scholar
van Borkulo, C. D., Boschloo, L., Kossakowski, J. L., Tio, P., Schoevers, R. A., Borsboom, D., & Waldorp, L. J. (2017). Comparing network structures on three aspects: A permutation test. Journal of Statistical Software. https://doi.org/10.13140/RG.2.2.29455.38569 Google Scholar
van IJzendoorn, M. H., Bakermans-Kranenburg, M. J., Duschinsky, R., Fox, N. A., Goldman, P. S., Gunnar, M. R., Johnson, D. E., Nelson, C. A., Reijman, S., Skinner, G., Zeanah, C. H., & Sonuga-Barke, E. J. S. (2020). Institutionalisation and deinstitutionalisation of children 1: A systematic and integrative review of evidence regarding effects on development. The Lancet Psychiatry, 7, 703720. https://doi.org/10.1016/s2215-0366(19)30399-2 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Venables, W. N., & Ripley, B. D. (2002). Modern Applied Statistics with S. Springer.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Wehmeier, P. M., Schacht, A., & Barkley, R. A. (2010). Social and Emotional Impairment in Children and Adolescents with ADHD and the Impact on Quality of Life. Journal of Adolescent Health, 46, 209217. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jadohealth.2009.09.009 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Wickham, H. (2007). Reshaping Data with the reshape Package. Journal of Statistical Software, 21, 1–20–21–20. Retrieved from http://www.jstatsoft.org/v21/i12/ CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Wickham, H. (2011). The split-apply-combine strategy for data analysis. Journal of Statistical Software, 40, 1–29-21–29. Retrieved from http://www.jstatsoft.org/v40/i01/ CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Wickham, H. (2016). ggplot2: Elegant Graphics for Data Analysis. Springer-Verlag New York.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Wickham, H. (2019). tidyverse: Easily Install and Load the Tidyverse. Retrieved from https://CRAN.R-project.org/package=tidyverse Google Scholar
Wickham, H., François, R., Henry, L., & Müller, K. (2020). dplyr: A Grammar of Data Manipulation. Retrieved from https://CRAN.R-project.org/package=dplyr Google Scholar
Woolgar, M., & Baldock, E. (2015). Attachment disorders versus more common problems in looked after and adopted children: Comparing community and expert assessments. Child and Adolescent Mental Health, 20, 3440. https://doi.org/10.1111/camh.12052 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
World Health Organization. (2010). The ICD-10: Version 2010. Classification of Mental and Behavioral Disorders: Diagnostic Criteria for Research. Geneva.Google Scholar
World Health Organization. (2018). International classification of diseases for mortality and morbidity statistics (11th Revision). Retrieved from https://icd.who.int/browse11/l-m/en Google Scholar
Zeanah, C. H., & Gleason, M. M. (2015). Annual research review: Attachment disorders in early childhood--clinical presentation, causes, correlates, and treatment. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, and Allied Disciplines, 56, 207222. https://doi.org/10.1111/jcpp.12347 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Zeanah, C. H., Chesher, T., Boris, N. W., & American Academy of Child and Adolescent Psychiatry (AACAP) Committee on Quality Issues (CQI) (2016). Practice parameter for the assessment and treatment of children and adolescents with reactive attachment disorder and disinhibited social engagement disorder. Journal of the American Academy of Child & Adolescent Psychiatry, 55, 9901003. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jaac.2016.08.004 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Figure 0

Figure 1. Mean scores SDQ scales.

Error bars represent standard deviations. ASD (n = 1430), ADHD (n = 1193), RAD/DAD (n = 39).
Figure 1

Table 1. SDQ Sample Descriptive Statistics for the SDQ Sample (n = 2662)

Figure 2

Table 2. SDQ Scale scores

Figure 3

Figure 2. LASSO Regularised Networks for ASD (n = 1430) and ADHD (n = 1193).

In these network graphs, blue lines represent positive edges, whereas red lines indicate negative edges. The magnitude of the association is represented by the width and vividness of the line. Nodes are coloured according to their respective SDQ scales. Thus blue nodes represent items from the Emotional Problems Scale. Orange nodes represent items from the Conduct Problems Scale. Green nodes represent items from the Hyperactivity Scale; Yellow nodes represent items from the Peer Problems Scale. Pink nodes represent Prosocial behaviour. Please note EBIC hyperparameter for the ASD and ADHD regularised networks is set to = 0.55.
Supplementary material: File

Coughlan et al. supplementary material

Coughlan et al. supplementary material

Download Coughlan et al. supplementary material(File)
File 2.5 MB