Hostname: page-component-7bb8b95d7b-pwrkn Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-09-27T03:32:14.309Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

Cultural technologies for peace may have shaped our social cognition

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  15 January 2024

Amine Sijilmassi*
Affiliation:
Département d’études cognitives, Institut Jean Nicod, ENS, EHESS, PSL University, CNRS, Paris, France amine.sij@gmail.com lou.safra@sciencespo.fr https://sites.google.com/site/lousafra/home nbaumard@gmail.com https://nicolasbaumards.org/
Lou Safra
Affiliation:
Département d’études cognitives, Institut Jean Nicod, ENS, EHESS, PSL University, CNRS, Paris, France amine.sij@gmail.com lou.safra@sciencespo.fr https://sites.google.com/site/lousafra/home nbaumard@gmail.com https://nicolasbaumards.org/
Nicolas Baumard
Affiliation:
Département d’études cognitives, Institut Jean Nicod, ENS, EHESS, PSL University, CNRS, Paris, France amine.sij@gmail.com lou.safra@sciencespo.fr https://sites.google.com/site/lousafra/home nbaumard@gmail.com https://nicolasbaumards.org/
*
*Corresponding author.

Abstract

Peace, the article shows, is achieved by culturally evolved institutions that incentivize positive-sum relationships. We propose that this insight has important consequences for the design of human social cognition. Cues that signal the existence of such institutions should play a prominent role in detecting group membership. We show how this accounts for previous findings and suggest avenues for future research.

Type
Open Peer Commentary
Copyright
Copyright © The Author(s), 2024. Published by Cambridge University Press

Peace, as this article reminds us, is nothing but a particular manifestation of a more general ability of humans to reap the benefits of mutually beneficial exchange – often called positive-sum relationships – instead of engaging in the war of all against all. Perhaps the most fundamental insight to be gained from this article is that the extent of this ability is supported by a highly complex culturally evolved system of institutions that incentivize cooperation by a great diversity of means. Cultural technologies like age-sets, peace ceremonies, efficient monitoring systems, or trading rituals are all highly complex, require very high levels of coordination, and typically emerge as a result of a long history of cultural accumulation over time (Alvard, Reference Alvard2003; Wiessner, Reference Wiessner2019; Wiessner & Tumu, Reference Wiessner and Tumu1998). In this commentary, we propose to extend the author's insight by highlighting how it can fit within the existing literature in social cognition and contribute further to this field.

Indeed, from an evolutionary perspective, the great variability in the ability of humans to achieve positive-sum relationships – that is, they sometimes achieve peace, but often cannot escape war – creates a selective pressure for an alliance detection system designed to keep track of the social relationships that structure one's particular social landscape. This includes both positive-sum relationships – for instance, the detection of friendships and coalitional alliances – and zero-sum ones – for instance, the detection of rivals or enemies (Liberman, Kinzler, & Woodward, Reference Liberman, Kinzler and Woodward2014; Pietraszewski, Cosmides, & Tooby, Reference Pietraszewski, Cosmides and Tooby2014; Pietraszewski, Curry, Petersen, Cosmides, & Tooby, Reference Pietraszewski, Curry, Petersen, Cosmides and Tooby2015).

This system is, of course, sensitive to direct evidence of positive-sum relationships, such as when people can directly observe instances of cooperation between individuals (Chalik & Rhodes, Reference Chalik and Rhodes2014; Kurzban, Tooby, & Cosmides, Reference Kurzban, Tooby and Cosmides2001; Liberman & Shaw, Reference Liberman and Shaw2017, Reference Liberman and Shaw2018). But, importantly, it also pays attention to indirect cues that are good predictors of social relationships: For instance, sharing goals, intentions, preferences, accents, or adhesion to norms can all facilitate cooperation and are indeed interpreted by human social cognition as predictors of social relationships (Basyouni & Parkinson, Reference Basyouni and Parkinson2022; Kinzler, Reference Kinzler2021; Liberman & Shaw, Reference Liberman and Shaw2017, Reference Liberman and Shaw2018; Liberman, Woodward, & Kinzler, Reference Liberman, Woodward and Kinzler2017; Liberman, Kinzler, & Woodward, Reference Liberman, Kinzler and Woodward2021; Liberman et al., Reference Liberman, Kinzler and Woodward2014; Noyes & Dunham, Reference Noyes and Dunham2017; Wilson, Bassiou, Denli, Dolan, & Watson, Reference Wilson, Bassiou, Denli, Dolan and Watson2018).

The insight that culturally evolved technologies are fundamental to stabilize positive-sum relationships thus has the potential to make a substantial contribution to the field of social cognition. Cultural technologies are not just abstract constructions that humans make, they should also constitute a major input for detecting group membership and cooperative networks in their environment. Observable cues suggesting that a group of individuals is embedded in a set of well-functioning institutions should trigger the alliance detection system and be encoded as a predictor of positive-sum relationships within the group.

This idea resonates with an already consistent body of evidence in the behavioral and political sciences suggesting that shared institutions play a fundamental role in group behavior (Bowles & Gintis, Reference Bowles and Gintis2004). Indeed, numerous lab experiments suggest that impartial and efficient sanctioning institutions can significantly increase social trust and prosocial behavior (Cassar, d'Adda, & Grosjean, Reference Cassar, d'Adda and Grosjean2014; Fabbri, Reference Fabbri2022; Hruschka et al., Reference Hruschka, Efferson, Jiang, Falletta-Cowden, Sigurdsson, McNamara and Henrich2014; Spadaro, Gangl, Van Prooijen, Van Lange, & Mosso, Reference Spadaro, Gangl, Van Prooijen, Van Lange and Mosso2020). Critically for the author's perspective, fair and effective institutions can even suppress prejudice between two rival groups (Bartoš & Levely, Reference Bartoš and Levely2021; Cassar et al., Reference Cassar, d'Adda and Grosjean2014; Fabbri, Reference Fabbri2022; Lin & Packer, Reference Lin and Packer2017; Van Bavel & Packer, Reference Van Bavel and Packer2021). Such institutions are usually operationalized as abstract entities that punish cheaters in lab experiments, or as state-like institutions in field studies. However, as Glowacki notes, institutions that favor the sanctioning of free-riders can take a wide variety of cultural forms – including much less formal ones. One important institutional strategy to stabilize cooperation is that societies tend to structure social networks in a way that facilitates the monitoring and sanctioning of cheaters, notably by encouraging the circulation of reputational information (Hechter, Reference Hechter1987; Ostrom, Reference Ostrom1990). In line with this idea, an influential field study in Uganda showed that ethnic preferences are merely a reflection of the belief in shared and efficient sanctioning institutions – typically through dense social networks that facilitate the detection of free-riders (Habyarimana, Humphreys, Posner, & Weinstein, Reference Habyarimana, Humphreys, Posner and Weinstein2007; see also Bartoš & Levely, Reference Bartoš and Levely2021, for a similar result in Afghanistan).

We invite researchers in social cognition to view this target article as an invitation to extend this line of research. More empirical work is needed to investigate how the presence of certain shared features – such as the presence of strong leaders, the existence of shared rituals, and other forms of cultural arrangements identified by Glowacki and others – can be used as cues by our social cognition to infer cooperative networks in our environment. This question is especially interesting when investigated from a “third-party” perspective (see, for instance, Noyes & Dunham, Reference Noyes and Dunham2020). For instance, are groups with effective leaders or with an institutionalized tradition of peace ceremonies perceived as more cohesive than groups without these features? Understanding how the perception of cultural technologies affects the way humans construct group boundaries has two important consequences.

First, it can contribute to understanding some peculiar cultural phenomena, such as why people often seem to take pride in the ancient roots of their culture (for illustrations of this tendency in modern nationalism, see Hobsbawm & Ranger, Reference Hobsbawm and Ranger1983). One answer may be that the ancient cultural history of a group signals that it has had the time to craft highly complex cultural technologies over the course of generations. A group that can claim ancient cultural roots may be perceived as having institutions that are better at solving the coordination and cooperation problems inherent in human social life, and thus at stabilizing positive-sum relationships.

Second, it can better guide the policy recommendations for peacebuilding. As noted by the author, many peacebuilding initiatives are attempts to create or rejuvenate cultural technologies that favor reconciliation and cooperation. Focusing on social cognition opens an additional avenue: Peace could be favored by exposing people to reliable cues that they evolve under cultural systems that are effective in resolving social dilemmas. For instance, making punishment, peace ceremonies, or any other functional institution more visible to citizens might considerably increase their willingness to interact peacefully.

Financial support

This research was funded by Agence Nationale pour la Recherche (ANR-17-EURE-0017 and ANR-10- IDEX-0001-02). A CC-BY public copyright license has been applied by the authors to the present document and will be applied to all subsequent versions up to the Author Accepted Manuscript arising from this submission, in accordance with the grant's open access conditions.

Competing interest

None.

Footnotes

These authors contributed equally.

References

Alvard, M. S. (2003). Kinship, lineage, and an evolutionary perspective on cooperative hunting groups in Indonesia. Human Nature, 14, 129163.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Bartoš, V., & Levely, I. (2021). Sanctioning and trustworthiness across ethnic groups: Experimental evidence from Afghanistan. Journal of Public Economics, 194, 104347. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jpubeco.2020.104347CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Basyouni, R., & Parkinson, C. (2022). Mapping the social landscape: Tracking patterns of interpersonal relationships. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 26(3), 204221.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Bowles, S., & Gintis, H. (2004). Persistent parochialism: Trust and exclusion in ethnic networks. Journal of Economic Behavior & Organization, 55(1), 123.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Cassar, A., d'Adda, G., & Grosjean, P. (2014). Institutional quality, culture, and norms of cooperation: Evidence from behavioral field experiments. The Journal of Law and Economics, 57(3), 821863. https://doi.org/10.1086/678331CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Chalik, L., & Rhodes, M. (2014). Preschoolers use social allegiances to predict behavior. Journal of Cognition and Development, 15(1), 136160.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Fabbri, M. (2022). Institutional quality shapes cooperation with out-group strangers. Evolution and Human Behavior, 43(1), 5370. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.evolhumbehav.2021.11.003CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Habyarimana, J., Humphreys, M., Posner, D. N., & Weinstein, J. M. (2007). Why does ethnic diversity undermine public goods provision?. American Political Science Review, 101(4), 709725.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Hechter, M. (1987). Principles of group solidarity. University of California Press.Google Scholar
Hobsbawm, E., & Ranger, T. (1983). The invention of tradition. Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
Hruschka, D., Efferson, C., Jiang, T., Falletta-Cowden, A., Sigurdsson, S., McNamara, R., … Henrich, J. (2014). Impartial institutions, pathogen stress and the expanding social network. Human Nature, 25(4), 567579. https://doi.org/10.1007/s12110-014-9217-0CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Kinzler, K. D. (2021). Language as a social cue. Annual Review of Psychology, 72, 241264.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Kurzban, R., Tooby, J., & Cosmides, L. (2001). Can race be erased? Coalitional computation and social categorization. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America, 98(26), 1538715392.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Liberman, Z., Kinzler, K. D., & Woodward, A. L. (2014). Friends or foes: Infants use shared evaluations to infer others’ social relationships. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 143(3), 966.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Liberman, Z., Kinzler, K. D., & Woodward, A. L. (2021). Origins of homophily: Infants expect people with shared preferences to affiliate. Cognition, 212, 104695.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Liberman, Z., & Shaw, A. (2017). Children use partial resource sharing as a cue to friendship. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 159, 96109.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Liberman, Z., & Shaw, A. (2018). Secret to friendship: Children make inferences about friendship based on secret sharing. Developmental Psychology, 54(11), 2139.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Liberman, Z., Woodward, A. L., & Kinzler, K. D. (2017). Preverbal infants infer third-party social relationships based on language. Cognitive Science, 41, 622634.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Lin, S. Y., & Packer, D. J. (2017). Dynamic tuning of evaluations: Implicit racial attitudes are sensitive to incentives for intergroup cooperation. Social Cognition, 35(3), 245272.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Noyes, A., & Dunham, Y. (2017). Mutual intentions as a causal framework for social groups. Cognition, 162, 133142.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Noyes, A., & Dunham, Y. (2020). Groups as institutions: The use of constitutive rules to attribute group membership. Cognition, 196, 104143.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Ostrom, E. (1990). Governing the commons: The evolution of institutions for collective action. Cambridge University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Pietraszewski, D., Cosmides, L., & Tooby, J. (2014). The content of our cooperation, not the color of our skin: An alliance detection system regulates categorization by coalition and race, but not sex. PLoS ONE, 9(2), e88534.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Pietraszewski, D., Curry, O. S., Petersen, M. B., Cosmides, L., & Tooby, J. (2015). Constituents of political cognition: Race, party politics, and the alliance detection system. Cognition, 140, 2439.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Spadaro, G., Gangl, K., Van Prooijen, J.-W., Van Lange, P. A. M., & Mosso, C. O. (2020). Enhancing feelings of security: How institutional trust promotes interpersonal trust. PLoS ONE, 15(9), e0237934. https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0237934CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Van Bavel, J. J., & Packer, D. J. (2021). The power of us: Harnessing our shared identities to improve performance, increase cooperation, and promote social harmony. Little, Brown Spark.Google Scholar
Wiessner, P. (2019). Collective action for war and for peace: A case study among the Enga of Papua New Guinea. Current Anthropology, 60(2), 224244.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Wiessner, P. W., & Tumu, A. (1998). Historical vines: Enga networks of exchange, ritual, and warfare in Papua New Guinea. Smithsonian Institution Press.Google Scholar
Wilson, S., Bassiou, E., Denli, A., Dolan, L. C., & Watson, M. (2018). Traveling groups stick together: How collective directional movement influences social cohesion. Evolutionary Psychology, 16(3), 1474704918792134.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed