Hostname: page-component-cd9895bd7-gvvz8 Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-12-21T16:02:59.411Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

Material insecurity and religiosity: A causal analysis

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  17 January 2024

Benjamin Grant Purzycki*
Affiliation:
Aarhus University, Denmark
Theiss Bendixen
Affiliation:
Aarhus University, Denmark
*
Corresponding author: Benjamin Grant Purzycki; Email: bgpurzycki@cas.au.dk

Abstract

Some cultural evolutionary models predict that under stressful reductions of well-being, individuals will be more attracted and fastidiously adhere to traditional systems of norms that promote solidarity and cooperation. As religious systems can bolster human relationships with a variety of mechanisms, the material insecurity hypothesis of religion posits that individual religiosity will increase under conditions of material insecurity. The bulk of the literature up to this point has been correlational and cross-national. Here, across 14 field sites, we examine the causal role that educational attainment and food insecurity play in religiosity. We find that years of formal education and food insecurity do not consistently contribute to individual religiosity cross-culturally. We conclude with a discussion of some theoretical and methodological implications. As a general workflow for cross-cultural causal research in the quantitative social sciences, the present work is a modest but necessary first step in reliably estimating causation in the material insecurity hypothesis of religiosity.

Type
Research Article
Creative Commons
Creative Common License - CCCreative Common License - BY
This is an Open Access article, distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution licence (http://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/4.0/), which permits unrestricted re-use, distribution and reproduction, provided the original article is properly cited.
Copyright
Copyright © The Author(s), 2024. Published by Cambridge University Press

Social media summary: This report finds that education or food security play no cross-culturally robust causal role in individual religiosity.

1. Introduction

Some models of cultural evolutionary processes posit that under stressful conditions, individuals will be attracted to and punish violations of cooperative norms (Roos et al., Reference Roos, Gelfand, Nau and Lun2015). As a cultural system that bolsters cooperation (Lang et al., Reference Lang, Purzycki, Apicella, Atkinson, Bolyanatz, Cohen and Henrich2019; Sosis & Bressler, Reference Sosis and Bressler2003; Sosis & Ruffle, Reference Sosis and Ruffle2003; cf. Major-Smith, Reference Major-Smith2023), we should expect to see that stress and insecurity increase religiosity, defined here as the mental and behavioural commitment to traditions and beliefs associated with spiritual agents. The bulk of the literature assessing this hypothesis typically exploits nation-level data from fully market-integrated societies and has yet to embrace formal causal inference methods. This report introduces causal inference to the material insecurity hypothesis literature and does so by applying it to individual-level data collected across 14 field sites from around the world.

2. Theory

2.1. Cultural evolution of commitment

Formal evolutionary game theoretical models show that increased threats to individual well-being contribute to the evolution of cooperative norm strength and policing; sudden reductions in within-group, individual payoffs induce stronger norm adherence and favour more costly punishment of norm violators (Roos et al., Reference Roos, Gelfand, Nau and Lun2015). Empirical evidence is consistent with these models; cross-culturally, such ‘cultural tightness’ corresponds to various threats to well-being (Gelfand et al., Reference Gelfand, Raver, Nishii, Leslie, Lun, Lim and Yamaguchi2011; Jackson et al., Reference Jackson, Gelfand and Ember2020). In general, then, it follows that under threatening conditions, individuals will flock towards and adhere more strongly to cultural systems that are especially adept at alleviating such stress.

As a social system that can (a) soothe anxiety (Lang et al., Reference Lang, Krátkỳ, Shaver, Jerotijević and Xygalatas2015; Pargament et al., Reference Pargament, Smith, Koenig and Perez1998; Sosis, Reference Sosis2007; Sosis & Handwerker, Reference Sosis and Handwerker2011), (b) bolster within-group cooperation and solidarity (Lang et al., Reference Lang, Purzycki, Apicella, Atkinson, Bolyanatz, Cohen and Henrich2019; Sosis & Bressler, Reference Sosis and Bressler2003; Sosis & Ruffle, Reference Sosis and Ruffle2003), (c) flexibly attend to novel perturbations in and threats to cooperation (Purzycki et al., Reference Purzycki, Stagnaro and Sasaki2020, Reference Purzycki, Bendixen, Lightner and Sosis2022a; cf. McNamara et al., Reference McNamara, Norenzayan and Henrich2016) and (d) include beliefs for the punishment of violations of moral and other social norms (Johnson, Reference Johnson2005, Reference Johnson2016; Norenzayan, Reference Norenzayan2013; Purzycki et al., Reference Purzycki, Willard, Klocová, Apicella, Atkinson, Bolyanatz and Ross2022c; Stark, Reference Stark2001), religion should be especially attractive for individuals enduring insecurity and stress. In other words, under duress, individuals are likely to intensify their commitment to the kinds of traditional norms and behaviours that have served prior generations and facilitated the kinds of social support that bolsters one's survival and reproduction, and religion has been shown to be especially supportive of these traits (Henrich et al., Reference Henrich, Bauer, Cassar, Chytilová and Purzycki2019; Inglehart, Reference Inglehart2020; Purzycki & Sosis, Reference Purzycki and Sosis2022; Sibley & Bulbulia, Reference Sibley and Bulbulia2012). As stressed by Inglehart (Reference Inglehart2020), the kinds of existential security that religion might uphold are both physical and intellectual (i.e. religious worldviews provide answers to life's bigger questions). We focus here on the former, hence, the ‘material insecurity hypothesis’.

2.2. Material insecurity hypothesis

While not without complications, a wide-ranging sociological literature is consistent with the material insecurity hypothesis (cf. Stark, Reference Stark1999). Yet a host of socio-demographic factors associated with material insecurity also appear to increase religiosity at the individual and group levels (see Storm, Reference Storm2017). For example, food insecurity, poverty and wealth inequality have been found to co-vary with religiosity (Baimel et al., Reference Baimel, Apicella, Atkinson, Bolyanatz, Cohen, Handley and Purzycki2022; Hekmatpour, Reference Hekmatpour2020; Höllinger & Muckenhuber, Reference Höllinger and Muckenhuber2019; Inglehart, Reference Inglehart2020; Norris & Inglehart, Reference Norris and Inglehart2011; Purzycki et al., Reference Purzycki, Ross, Apicella, Atkinson, Cohen, McNamara and Henrich2018b; Ruiter & Van Tubergen, Reference Ruiter and Van Tubergen2009; Solt et al., Reference Solt, Habel and Grant2011), but as we are presently interested in assessing whether or not food security causes changes in religiosity, we must declare a particular causal direction. Furthermore, in addition to the predicted rise in religiosity owing to insecurity associated with age and proximity to death (Jong & Halberstadt, Reference Jong and Halberstadt2018), some cross-cultural evidence suggests that important indices of religious commitment can change across the lifespan in important ways (Bengtson et al., Reference Bengtson, Silverstein, Putney and Harris2015; Purzycki & Bendixen, Reference Purzycki and Bendixen2020; Shaver & Sosis, Reference Shaver and Sosis2014).

Additionally, sex is variously associated with religiosity (Schnabel et al., Reference Schnabel, Hackett and McClendon2018; Vardy et al., Reference Vardy, Moya, Placek, Apicella, Bolyanatz, Cohen and Atkinson2022; Walter & Davie, Reference Walter and Davie1998) as are fertility and family size (Blume, Reference Blume, Voland and Schiefenhövel2009; Glavatskaya et al., Reference Glavatskaya, Borovik and Thorvaldsen2018; Inglehart, Reference Inglehart2020; Shaver et al., Reference Shaver, Power, Purzycki, Watts, Sear, Shenk and Bulbulia2020). On evolutionary theoretical grounds, family size in particular has a complicated relationship with material insecurity (Lawson et al., Reference Lawson, Alvergne and Gibson2012; Purzycki et al., Reference Purzycki, Ross, Apicella, Atkinson, Cohen, McNamara and Henrich2018b; Strassmann & Gillespie, Reference Strassmann and Gillespie2002), but evidence points to a relationship nonetheless. Similarly, education level is directly implicated in the number of children one has (Becker et al., Reference Becker, Cinnirella and Woessmann2010; Goodman et al., Reference Goodman, Koupil and Lawson2012) and shows a widely touted – but no less complicated – relationship with security and well-being (see Boarini & Strauss, Reference Boarini and Strauss2010; Desjardins, Reference Desjardins2008; Giambona et al., Reference Giambona, Porcu and Sulis2022).

In addition to its role in material security, educational attainment is also of particular interest in the study of religion. Exposure to formal, secular education has long been asserted to weaken religious faith owing to the cultivation of critical thinking skills (Dawkins, Reference Dawkins2016; Weber, Reference Weber1993 [1920]). In other words, educational attainment might both directly and indirectly decrease religiosity. However, while some cross-national assessments do indeed show negative associations between years of formal education and religiosity (e.g. Inglehart, Reference Inglehart2020; Norris & Inglehart, Reference Norris and Inglehart2011, pp. 271–274), others reveal that both the magnitude and even the direction of the estimated association exhibits substantial cross-cultural variability (e.g. Albrecht & Heaton, Reference Albrecht and Heaton1984; Pew Research Center, 2016; Ruiter & Van Tubergen, Reference Ruiter and Van Tubergen2009; Schwadel, Reference Schwadel2011, Reference Schwadel2015).

As insightful as this literature is, it has some important limitations worth addressing. Theoretically, despite the explicit causal language across the theories that motivate this work, there is a distinct lack of causal modelling in the literature. This makes it difficult to synthesise various works into a single causal framework. Indeed, the empirical efforts are primarily correlational and do not formally model the manifold causal pathways by which education and food security could affect individual religious commitments. So, while theory is causal, the models are often informal and analyses typically focus on significant correlations rather than counterfactual states.

In terms of data, the bulk of this work often focuses on group-level relationships. Yet, group-level correlations can mask or muddle individual-level effects (Robinson, Reference Robinson2009; Simpson, Reference Simpson1951; Storm, Reference Storm2017; see Supporting Information Section 2 of the present report for further discussion and demonstration of this point). As such, individual-level, high-resolution data are important for adequately assessing individual-level hypotheses. In terms of sampling, most of the data that populates these studies are collected among individuals in fully market-integrated industrialised societies. Given the variation in food security and access to education the world exhibits, we should be able to see the anticipated effects of these indices of material security on religiosity across the range of communities. Here, we offer a modest step towards rectifying these issues by (a) synthesising a host of hypotheses into a single causal model, (b) being explicit about and simulating the assumptions of our model, (c) applying this model to data sampled from a diverse range of societies and (d) attending to individual-level effects using a general and principled causal inference method.

3. Assessing causation

3.1. Causal inference

Across academic fields, there is increasing recognition that (a) causal explanatory accounts of natural phenomena require explicitly derived models and (b) estimating causation is best approached from an interventionist perspective (variously known as the manipulationist, counterfactual or potential outcomes frameworks; Hernan & Robins, Reference Hernan and Robins2020; Morgan & Winship, Reference Morgan and Winship2015; Pearl et al., Reference Pearl, Glymour and Jewell2016; Woodward, Reference Woodward2005). In this view, establishing causation requires that we not only examine what our data tell us about worlds that exist, but also form deliberately-calculated inferences of causal relationships derived from worlds that would exist, given particular conditions.

Quite often, the standard procedure of including a statistical model with all variables under consideration simply assumes that all variables are predictive of the dependent variable and ignores any internal causal structure. Yet, ‘controlling’ for everything can actually create more problems than it solves because otherwise ignored causal relationships that are internal to those predictors can introduce further confounding effects (e.g. conditioning on a common descendant of the exposure and outcome – collider bias) or suppress actual causal effects (e.g. conditioning on a mediator or a post-treatment variable). Put simply, under some conditions, holding some variables constant (i.e. controlling) can be inconsequential, while under others it can be disastrous to inference (Achen, Reference Achen2005; Cinelli et al., Reference Cinelli, Forney and Pearl2020; Westreich & Greenland, Reference Westreich and Greenland2013; Wysocki et al., Reference Wysocki, Lawson and Rhemtulla2022). The conditions that matter here are the causal structure of variables germane to testing hypotheses. Making these conditions explicit is the task of the researcher.

To obtain our results, we use a general causal inference method for eliciting and contrasting outcomes under hypothetical exposures in observational settings (e.g. Ahern et al., Reference Ahern, Hubbard and Galea2009; Naimi et al., Reference Naimi, Cole and Kennedy2017; Robins, Reference Robins1986; Snowden et al., Reference Snowden, Rose and Mortimer2011). This method – variously referred to as g-formula, g-computation and standardisation (see Bendixen, Reference Bendixen2023; Hernan & Robins Reference Hernan and Robins2020, chap. 13; VanderWeele et al. Reference VanderWeele, Jackson and Li2016) – involves three main steps: first, given a well-defined estimand and explicit causal assumptions, an appropriate statistical model is fitted that adjusts for relevant confounding variables. In the context of the present study, this first step is undertaken in Sections 3.2 and 4.3 below. Second, using the fitted model in the first step, we predict (or impute) outcome values under varying hypothetical levels of a focal predictor while holding all other variables as observed. This step follows directly from the interventionist approach to causal inference outlined above, in that it simulates changes in potential outcomes under manipulated, counter-factual exposures. Third, the imputed or predicted values from the previous step are contrasted and summarised by some appropriate inferential statistic, such as the mean and an interval. This step effectively amounts to averaging over the joint distribution of the observed covariates, yielding an average or marginal effect of the exposure in the sample. For the present analysis, for each individual and outcome response option, we compute the posterior mean under hypothetical exposure levels. For this effect to have a valid causal interpretation, a set of identifiability assumptions needs to be met (Hernan & Robins, Reference Hernan and Robins2020). We discuss these in Section 7 of the Supporting Information.

3.2. Causal model

Our guiding causal model (Figure 1) incorporates the hypotheses examined in the aforementioned literature (Section 2.2). The variables and causal relationships we consider are drawn from predicted directions of previous studies. The model is also informed and constrained by our knowledge of what variables we could use in the dataset.

Figure 1. Directed acyclic graph of the assumed causal structure. Bold variables are focal variables.

The focal causal hypotheses are that religiosity increases as a function of exposure to education and food insecurity. To the extent that years of formal education constitute exposure to a particular form of instruction that has some effect on the degree to which one is religious, and concern about access to food approximates material insecurity, we use these direct effects as our focal theoretical predictions and empirical estimands: Education (Amount) → Religiosity, and Food Insecurity → Religiosity. We also incorporate some of the other aforementioned individual-level predictors into our model, including sex, age, and number of children. The model wagers that they comprise a causal network of influence between amount of education and religiosity. Specifically, it posits an internal causal structure to these variables: Sex → Food Security ← Age. If Children ← Food Security, conditioning on children would not be necessary if Food Security were held constant (for an introduction to causal graph analysis, see Pearl et al., Reference Pearl, Glymour and Jewell2016). However, if Children → Food Security, this creates a collider bias with Food Security and Education (Amount). We therefore take this more conservative route and hold the number of children constant. We also posit that Education (Amount) → Children. While this creates an indirect path through Children, as we are conditioning on number of children already and there are no other posited confounds, we are avoiding this problem. Furthermore, the amount of education should contribute to one's food security which should, in turn, contribute to religiosity. Sex and age confound the focal causal path, and there is an indirect effect of education amount on religiosity that runs through food security.

It is important to note that the type of education might matter here. We recognise that individuals could have had religious education in some contexts whether by default or by choice, and that choice could be facilitated by food security, and so forth. In this model, we posit only that it has an effect on religiosity. This is a very important assumption for a couple of reasons. First, this is unmeasured in the dataset. According to this model structure, however, its absence is inconsequential to estimating the target effects; we have no need for conditioning on type of education. If we posit that it was caused by food security, not having measured it would be inconsequential to the target estimate as long as we hold food security constant (i.e. the indirect effect through education type would be blocked). However, if the type of education were posited to cause food security, this would both confound the effect through food security, but also open a confounding path between education amount through food security. With this important note in mind, we continue under the assumption that type of education only affects religiosity. In Section 3 in the Supporting Information, we walk through a simulation of this model that shows that we can recover target estimates with and without this particular variable.

4. Study

4.1. Participants

We used data from the publicly available Evolution of Religion and Morality Project dataset (for motivation, design details, and further details about each field site, see Lang et al., Reference Lang, Purzycki, Apicella, Atkinson, Bolyanatz, Cohen and Henrich2019; Purzycki et al., Reference Purzycki, Apicella, Atkinson, Cohen, McNamara, Willard and Henrich2016, Reference Purzycki, Henrich, Apicella, Atkinson, Baimel, Cohen and Norenzayan2018a, Reference Purzycki, Lang, Henrich and Norenzayan2022b). The entire dataset consists of 2228 participants from 15 diverse populations around the world (see Fig. 2), but owing to there being no data on the outcome, we dropped one site (Hadza). Participants were recruited in a variety of ways; some of them were recruited on the basis of self-identification with a locally relevant religious tradition. There are no indications that recruitment on this basis contributed to the selection of individuals who are more religiously committed. All participants were 17 years of age or older. All participants engaged in a variety of economic game experiments designed to examine the breadth of religiously motivated cooperation. Participants were informed that their participation would be anonymous, any identifying information would be kept under lock and key and they had the right to withdraw at any point. Table 1 details the key descriptive statistics of each field site.

Figure 2. Map of 14 field sites. Note that there are two field sites in Tanna (Inland and Coastal).

Table 1. Means (standard deviations) for some individual-level demographic variables and group-level properties of communities sampled. ‘Mjr trad.’ refers to the major colonial and/or overarching tradition of each site while ‘Mnr trad.’ refers to relatively minor – but locally salient – traditions in each site.

4.2. Methods

In addition to our focal variables of formal education, food security and religiosity, we also include the ancillary demographic factors (sex, age and number of children). As some traditions emphasise and signal religious commitment variously through a combination of belief and practice (Baimel et al., Reference Baimel, Apicella, Atkinson, Bolyanatz, Cohen, Handley and Purzycki2022; Cohen et al., Reference Cohen, Siegel and Rozin2003), we consider two questions that represent religiosity or religious commitment. One focuses on intrinsic or ideational commitment: How often do you think about [deity]? The other focuses on extrinsic or behavioural commitment: How often do you perform activities or practices to talk to or appease [deity]? Response options were on a five-point frequency scale (1 = very rarely/never, 2 = a few times per year, 3 = a few times per month, 4 = a few times per week, 5 = every day or multiple times per day). We measured current food security with a binary, yes/no question (no = 0; yes = 1): Do you worry that in the next month your household will have a time when it is not able to buy or produce enough food to eat? (Hruschka et al., Reference Hruschka, Efferson, Jiang, Falletta-Cowden, Sigurdsson, McNamara and Henrich2014).

4.3. Statistical model

To model the level of commitment across the two variables, we used Bayesian ordered-logistic regressions (Bürkner & Vuorre, Reference Bürkner and Vuorre2019). In each population, j, we asked each individual, i, the commitment questions, outcomes of which, K i, are ordered categorical responses. As such, we model them using a multilevel ordered categorical likelihood distribution that is parameterised using a linear model term, ξ i, and a vector of random cut-points, κ:

(1)$$K_i\sim {\rm Ordered\;Categorical}( {\xi_i, \;\kappa } ) $$

The linear model ξ i is then given by:

(2)$$\xi _i = \alpha _{{\rm SITE}[ i ] } + \beta _{{\rm SITE}[ i ] }^M M_i + \beta _{{\rm SITE}[ i ] }^A A_i + \beta _{{\rm SITE}[ i ] }^S S_i + \beta _{{\rm SITE}[ i ] }^E E_i + \beta _{{\rm SITE}[ i ] }^C C_i$$

where SITE[i] gives the cultural group of individual i, A i is the age of individual i, S i is a variable indicating if individual i is male, E i is the years of formal education completed by individual i, C i indicates an individual's number of children and M i indicates an individual's food insecurity. To account for the fact that increments in the predictor (e.g. an additional year of education and year of age or an additional child) might have differential associations with the outcome, we model the effects of years of education, age and number of children monotonically, such that a covariate's β coefficient represents the expected average difference between two adjacent levels of the predictor (Bürkner & Charpentier, Reference Bürkner and Charpentier2020). Note that we also modelled age and years of education with Gaussian processes (see Section 4 in the Supporting Information) but found the monotonic model to yield a better fit. Food insecurity and sex are modelled as indicator variables. Since unobserved site-specific factors are likely to affect our predicted effects in different ways across sites, our statistical model fully varies effects across sites. It is plausible that, for example, one year of education in one place deeply affects one's worldview whereas in others, it might have no effect. By-site clustering partly handles site-specific confounds.

We define our priors, which we checked through prior predictive simulation (see Section 4 in the Supporting Information) as follows. The thresholds κ are modelled with Normal(0, 10), all β coefficients with Normal(0, 0.5) and all monotonic covariates with a Dirichlet distribution with α denoting a series of 2s equalling the number of levels of the predictor. The Dirichlet prior encodes the a priori assumption that any of the levels of the predictor could be more or less likely than the others (McElreath, Reference McElreath2020, pp. 392–394). Variance components for the varying effects were set at Exponential(1) and the correlation matrix of the variance components to LKJCorr(4) (Lewandowski et al., Reference Lewandowski, Kurowicka and Joe2009). Taken together, the specified set of priors is weakly regularising.

We performed all analyses using the brms package (Bürkner, Reference Bürkner2017, Reference Bürkner2018, Reference Bürkner2021) for R (R Core Team, 2021), an interface to the probabilistic programming language Stan (Carpenter et al., Reference Carpenter, Gelman, Hoffman, Lee, Goodrich, Betancourt and Riddell2017). Chain and sampling diagnostics ($\hat{R} < 1.02$, well-mixing trace and rank plots, few divergent transitions, and relatively large effective sample sizes) were acceptable. Section 8 in the Supporting Information includes the full list of R packages used for this project as well as their dependencies and version number.

4.4. Results

4.4.1. Food insecurity

Figures 3 and 4 show predicted (posterior means) individual comparisons between reporting being worried about food and not being worried about food for each site and outcome response option. Consider that if food insecurity predicts religious commitment, we would expect that the higher outcome response options (i.e. thinking about and performing activities directed at the focal deity more often; the darker line colours) should become more probable (i.e. trend upwards), whereas the lower response options (i.e. thinking about and performing activities directed at the focal deity less often; the lighter line colours) should become less probable (i.e. trend downward) as a function of reporting food insecurity. Note that while we are plotting the functional relationship between food security and religious commitment linearly, this is for presentational purposes only; we statistically modelled food security as an indicator variable. While we see semblances of this pattern at a few sites (e.g. Figure 3 – Huatasani, Tyva Republic, Yasawa Fiji; Figure 4 – Kananga, Turkana), at other sites we see the opposite pattern (e.g. Figure 3: Coastal and Inland Tanna, Sursurunga) or, most commonly, no particular change in either direction.

Figure 3. Food insecurity and religious ideation. Posterior means for each individual and outcome response option under hypothetical exposures to food (in)security.

Figure 4. Food insecurity and religious behaviour. Posterior means for each individual and outcome response option under hypothetical exposures to food (in)security.

4.4.2. Education

Figures 5 and 6 show predicted (posterior means) individual comparisons between a range of educational attainment levels for each site and outcome response option. Specifically, we obtained predictions for years of formal education from 0 to 30 years (the observed range in the sample) in increments of five. Here, if educational attainment negatively predicts religious commitment, we would expect that the higher outcome response options (i.e. thinking about and performing activities directed at the focal deity more often; the darker line colours) become less probable (i.e. trend downwards), whereas the lower response options (i.e. thinking about and performing activities directed at the focal deity less often; the lighter line colours) would become more probable (i.e. trend upwards) as a function of additional years of formal education. At most sites, we in fact find the opposite of this predicted pattern, such that additional years of formal education predict more religious commitment, although the associations generally seem small or moderate. At a few other sites, we do find the hypothesised pattern (e.g. Figure 5: Lovu Fiji; Figure 6: Lovu Fiji, Mysore), while at other sites no particular change is observed. Overall, there is no stable relationship.

Figure 5. Years of formal education and religious ideation. Posterior means for each individual and outcome response option under hypothetical number of years of formal education.

Figure 6. Years of formal education and religious behaviour. Posterior means for each individual and outcome response option under hypothetical number of years of formal education.

5. Discussion

Overall and across sites, then, we find little-to-no reliable evidence that food security or educational attainment causes religious commitment. If anything, based on these data and our causal and statistical models, additional years of formal education tend to increase religious commitment at several sites, contrary to common theorising. We see the present work as taking some modest but necessary first steps in reliably estimating the causal relationship between educational attainment, food insecurity and religiosity specifically, and the material insecurity hypothesis more generally. We also hope this work can motivate a general workflow for cross-cultural causal research in the quantitative social sciences (cf. Bulbulia, Reference Bulbulia2022; Deffner et al., Reference Deffner, Rohrer and McElreath2022). As we turn to now, there are some issues from theory and model to analysis, prediction, and inference to which future work should attend.

In terms of modelling, while our statistical model and analyses directly follow from our causal model, our causal model does not directly follow from a formal theoretical model. What theory we do have is informal and recognised as underdeveloped in a host of ways (Storm, Reference Storm2017). Our approach gathered various hypotheses together, forged a synthetic causal model out of verbal theoretical synthesis and framed that theorising with an appeal to a set of formal cultural evolutionary models.

That said, as is often the case, the present study may be susceptible to biases from confounding variables our model did not consider. While it is unclear to us that recruitment was biased in terms of levels of religiosity, education, and the other factors we considered, our model is a necessary simplification of a complex process. Future efforts can build upon our model and consider further factors that may play an important role in estimating how education corresponds to religiosity. We anticipate that our causal model can function as foundation upon which to build. Our hypotheses are restricted to the posited direction of the effect of food security and education, but we have no precise sense of what the magnitude of the effect should be. More precision in theory would help here. It is also important to emphasise that the value of building and presenting a causal model, rather than attempting to get at one ‘true’ representation of the world, is to make one's assumptions transparent and explicit.

Indeed, making assumptions transparent and explicit is a critical step in causal inference, but it is only just a first step. As we showed using g-computation, calculating contrasts is a necessary follow-up to establish how outcomes fare in light of manipulating the focal predictor. Another next step is accounting for how generalisable results are. It is therefore important to stress that our main model and predictions yield inferences for a particular population, namely the sampled sites and participants with complete cases for the included variables, when hypothetically manipulating food security and years of formal education. While our dataset is both culturally and demographically diverse, our sample may not necessarily be characteristic of any given population for which one wishes to draw inference. For the sake of illustration, then, we also show how to go about obtaining predictions for a non-sampled but well-defined population using so-called poststratification (Deffner et al., Reference Deffner, Rohrer and McElreath2022; Kennedy & Gelman, Reference Kennedy and Gelman2021) in Section 6 in the Supporting Information. Briefly put, poststratification involves re-weighting model-based predictions using weights obtained from external data that are more representative of a population of interest. Drawing from another data sample, we show how to use the model to predict religiosity in the context of a secular, materially secure and wealthy country (Denmark).

Other steps remain on the horizon. In particular, we anticipate improving inference using different sets of assumptions. In our case, had we found a notable target effect and the issue of education type (or any other missing confounder) was compelling enough, sensitivity analyses might detect how sizeable this unmeasured confound's effects would have to be to upend the estimated causal effect (Ding & VanderWeele, Reference Ding and VanderWeele2016; VanderWeele & Ding, Reference VanderWeele and Ding2017). Recent efforts (Major-Smith, Reference Major-Smith2023) bring a range of sensitivity analyses to bear on inferring causation in observational data of religious prosociality. Of course, more discussion and attention are necessary to determine what exactly qualifies as ‘compelling enough’ and what the criteria are by which we arrive at such conclusions.

Methodologically, more robust measures of food and other forms of insecurity would be useful to examine the range of well-being types corresponding to religiosity (for review of various methods, see Manikas et al., Reference Manikas, Ali and Sundarakani2023). Alas, the dataset we used offers no such measures. More importantly perhaps is the specific aspects of formal education that might matter when it comes to playing a role in reducing (or increasing) religiosity. While our causal model suggests that we need not worry about the role of the type of education, there are other aspects of formal education that could play a clearer role in affecting religiosity. Rather than exposure to formal education in years, other individual-level factors such as performance, attendance and/or general enthusiasm in school might be pivotal for individual commitment to religion. Again, theory should help direct further inquiry.

More pragmatically, our results also have bearing on any potential interventions. For those committed to making higher education more available under the assumption that it will decrease commitment to religion, our results suggest that increasing the amount of education individuals obtain is not likely to do much. If our assumptions are realistic, the model is a worthy candidate for explanation and our data are representative of the kinds of variation we expect to see in the world, we can conclude that there are no general or reliable effects of education and food security on religiosity. It may be the case that educational attainment might lead to the kinds of social living that rely less on (or have less time for) religion and its benefits (Inglehart, Reference Inglehart2020). Our model certaintly oversimplifies this process, but takes the first steps towards formally modelling it in a causal framework.

In closing, we hope this report usefully provides a template for future work causally investigating the material security hypothesis with either cross-sectional or longitudinal data. With the goal of making causal assumptions, models and use of theory explicit, we identified areas for future research and outlined a workflow for facilitating inference-making beyond what simple descriptions of data offer.

Supplementary material

The supplementary material for this article can be found at https://doi.org/10.1017/ehs.2023.29

Acknowledgements

We thank Cristine Legare and the Culture in Schooling team for their feedback and encouragement. We also thank our reviewers and Charles Efferson for their careful and constructive feedback.

Author contributions

BGP contributed to designing and managing the original project that produced the data used herein, led writing the text, crafted the causal and statistical model, drafted the simulation and obtained funding for the effort. TB conducted data analysis, crafted results graphs and contributed to writing.

Financial support

We acknowledge support from the Aarhus University Research Foundation and a Culture in Schooling Grant (Issachar Fund #TIF0206) that directly funded this project.

Conflicts of interest declaration

The authors declare no conflicts of interests.

Data availability

The data, code and supplementary materials for this particular study are hosted at https://github.com/tbendixen/causal-inference-schooling. The greater project from which the present work drew is hosted at https://github.com/bgpurzycki/Evolution-of-Religion-and-Morality.

References

Achen, C. H. (2005). Let's put garbage-can regressions and garbage-can probits where they belong. Conflict Management and Peace Science, 22(4), 327339.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Ahern, J., Hubbard, A., & Galea, S. (2009). Estimating the effects of potential public health interventions on population disease burden: A step-by-step illustration of causal inference methods. American Journal of Epidemiology, 169(9), 11401147.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Albrecht, S. L., & Heaton, T. B. (1984). Secularization, higher education, and religiosity. Review of Religious Research, 26(1), 4358.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Baimel, A., Apicella, C., Atkinson, Q., Bolyanatz, A., Cohen, E., Handley, C., …, Purzycki, B. G. (2022). Material insecurity predicts greater commitment to moralistic and less commitment to local deities: a cross-cultural investigation. Religion, Brain & Behavior, 12(1–2), 417.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Becker, S. O., Cinnirella, F., & Woessmann, L. (2010). The trade-off between fertility and education: Evidence from before the demographic transition. Journal of Economic Growth, 15, 177204.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Bendixen, T. (2023). Causal inference in the social, health, and business sciences: A very brief practical primer in R and Stan. Working Paper. https://github.com/tbendixen/causal-inference-wpGoogle Scholar
Bengtson, V. L., Silverstein, M., Putney, N. M., & Harris, S. C. (2015). Does religiousness increase with age? age changes and generational differences over 35 years. Journal for the Scientific Study of Religion, 54(2), 363379.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Blume, M. (2009). The reproductive benefits of religious affiliation. In Voland, E., & Schiefenhövel, W. (Eds.), The Biological Evolution of Religious Mind and Behavior (pp. 117204). Springer.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Boarini, R., & Strauss, H. (2010). What is the private return to tertiary education? New evidence from 21 OECD countries. OECD Journal: Economic Studies, 2010, 125.Google Scholar
Bulbulia, J. A. (2022). A workflow for causal inference in cross-cultural psychology. Religion, Brain & Behavior, 13(3), 291306.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Bürkner, P.-C. (2017). brms: An R package for Bayesian multilevel models using Stan. Journal of Statistical Software, 80(1), 128.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Bürkner, P.-C. (2018). Advanced Bayesian multilevel modeling with the R package brms. The R Journal, 10(1), 395411.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Bürkner, P.-C. (2021). Bayesian item response modeling in R with brms and Stan. Journal of Statistical Software, 100(5), 154.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Bürkner, P.-C., & Charpentier, E. (2020). Modelling monotonic effects of ordinal predictors in Bayesian regression models. British Journal of Mathematical and Statistical Psychology, 73(3), 420451.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Bürkner, P.-C., & Vuorre, M. (2019). Ordinal regression models in psychology: A tutorial. Advances in Methods and Practices in Psychological Science, 2(1), 77101.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Carpenter, B., Gelman, A., Hoffman, M., Lee, D., Goodrich, B., Betancourt, M., …, Riddell, A. (2017). Stan: A probabilistic programming language. Journal of Statistical Software, 76(1), 132.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Cinelli, C., Forney, A., & Pearl, J. (2020). A crash course in good and bad controls. Sociological Methods Research, 00491241221099552.Google Scholar
Cohen, A. B., Siegel, J. I., & Rozin, P. (2003). Faith versus practice: Different bases for religiosity judgments by jews and protestants. European Journal of Social Psychology, 33(2), 287295.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Dawkins, R. (2016). The God delusion. Random House.Google Scholar
Deffner, D., Rohrer, J. M., & McElreath, R. (2022). A causal framework for cross-cultural generalizability. Advances in Methods and Practices in Psychological Science, 5(3), 25152459221106366.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Desjardins, R. (2008). Researching the links between education and well-being. European Journal of Education, 43(1), 2335.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Ding, P., & VanderWeele, T. J. (2016). Sensitivity analysis without assumptions. Epidemiology, 27(3), 368.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Gelfand, M. J., Raver, J. L., Nishii, L., Leslie, L. M., Lun, J., Lim, B. C., …, Yamaguchi, S. (2011). Differences between tight and loose cultures: A 33-nation study. Science, 332(6033), 11001104.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Giambona, F., Porcu, M., & Sulis, I. (2022). Does education protect families’ well-being in times of crisis? Measurement issues and empirical findings from IT-SILC data. Statistical Methods & Applications, 32, 299328.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Glavatskaya, E., Borovik, J., & Thorvaldsen, G. (2018). Urban infant mortality and religion at the end of the nineteenth and in the early twentieth century: The case of Ekaterinburg, Russia. The History of the Family, 23(1), 135153.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Goodman, A., Koupil, I., & Lawson, D. W. (2012). Low fertility increases descendant socioeconomic position but reduces long-term fitness in a modern post-industrial society. Proceedings of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences, 279(1746), 43424351.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Hekmatpour, P. (2020). Inequality and religiosity in a global context: Different secularization paths for developed and developing nations. International Journal of Sociology, 50(4), 286309.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Henrich, J., Bauer, M., Cassar, A., Chytilová, J., & Purzycki, B. G. (2019). War increases religiosity. Nature Human Behaviour, 3(2), 129135.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Hernan, M., & Robins, J. (2020). Causal inference: What if? Chapman & Hill/CRC.Google Scholar
Höllinger, F., & Muckenhuber, J. (2019). Religiousness and existential insecurity: A cross-national comparative analysis on the macro-and micro-level. International Sociology, 34(1), 1937.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Hruschka, D., Efferson, C., Jiang, T., Falletta-Cowden, A., Sigurdsson, S., McNamara, R., …, Henrich, J. (2014). Impartial institutions, pathogen stress and the expanding social network. Human Nature, 25(4), 567579.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Inglehart, R. F. (2020). Religion's sudden decline: What's causing it, and what comes next? Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Jackson, J. C., Gelfand, M., & Ember, C. R. (2020). A global analysis of cultural tightness in non-industrial societies. Proceedings of the Royal Society B, 287(1930), 20201036.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Johnson, D. D. P. (2005). God's punishment and public goods. Human Nature, 16(4), 410446.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Johnson, D. D. P. (2016). God is watching you: How the fear of god makes us human. Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Jong, J., & Halberstadt, J. (2018). Death anxiety and religious belief: An existential psychology of religion. Bloomsbury Publishing.Google Scholar
Kennedy, L., & Gelman, A. (2021). Know your population and know your model: Using model-based regression and poststratification to generalize findings beyond the observed sample. Psychological Methods, 26(5), 547558.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Lang, M., Krátkỳ, J., Shaver, J. H., Jerotijević, D., & Xygalatas, D. (2015). Effects of anxiety on spontaneous ritualized behavior. Current Biology, 25(14), 18921897.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Lang, M., Purzycki, B. G., Apicella, C. L., Atkinson, Q. D., Bolyanatz, A., Cohen, E., …, Henrich, J. (2019). Moralizing gods, impartiality and religious parochialism across 15 societies. Proceedings of the Royal Society B, 286(1898), 20190202.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Lawson, D. W., Alvergne, A., & Gibson, M. A. (2012). The life-history trade-off between fertility and child survival. Proceedings of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences, 279(1748), 47554764.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Lewandowski, D., Kurowicka, D., & Joe, H. (2009). Generating random correlation matrices based on vines and extended onion method. Journal of Multivariate Analysis, 100(9), 19892001.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Major-Smith, D. (2023). Exploring causality from observational data: An example assessing whether religiosity promotes cooperation. Evolutionary Human Sciences, 5, e22.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Manikas, I., Ali, B. M., & Sundarakani, B. (2023). A systematic literature review of indicators measuring food security. Agriculture & Food Security, 12(1), 10.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
McElreath, R. (2020). Statistical rethinking: A Bayesian course with examples in R and Stan, 2nd edn. CRC Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
McNamara, R. A., Norenzayan, A., & Henrich, J. (2016). Supernatural punishment, in-group biases, and material insecurity: Experiments and ethnography from Yasawa, Fiji. Religion, Brain & Behavior, 6(1), 3455. http://www.tandfonline.com/doi/full/10.1080/2153599X.2014.921235CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Morgan, S. L., & Winship, C. (2015). Counterfactuals and causal inference. Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
Naimi, A. I., Cole, S. R., & Kennedy, E. H. (2017). An introduction to g methods. International Journal of Epidemiology, 46(2), 756762.Google ScholarPubMed
Norenzayan, A. (2013). Big gods: How religion transformed cooperation and conflict. Princeton University Press.Google Scholar
Norris, P., & Inglehart, R. (2011). Sacred and secular: Religion and politics worldwide. Cambridge University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Pargament, K. I., Smith, B. W., Koenig, H. G., & Perez, L. (1998). Patterns of positive and negative religious coping with major life stressors. Journal for the Scientific Study of Religion, (4), 37(4), 710724.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Pearl, J., Glymour, M., & Jewell, N. P. (2016). Causal inference in statistics: A primer. John Wiley & Sons.Google Scholar
Pew Research Center (2016). Religion and education around the world: Large gaps in education levels persist, but all faiths are making gains – Particularly among women. OCLC: 968707242.Google Scholar
Purzycki, B. G., & Bendixen, T. (2020). Examining values, virtues, and tradition in the Tyva Republic with free-list and demographic data. The New Research in Tuva, 4, 20.Google Scholar
Purzycki, B. G., & Sosis, R. (2022). Religion evolving: The dynamics of culture, cognition and ecology. Equinox.Google Scholar
Purzycki, B. G., Apicella, C., Atkinson, Q. D., Cohen, E., McNamara, R. A., Willard, A. K., …, Henrich, J. (2016). Moralistic gods, supernatural punishment and the expansion of human sociality. Nature, 530(7590), 327330.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Purzycki, B. G., Henrich, J., Apicella, C., Atkinson, Q. D., Baimel, A., Cohen, E., …, Norenzayan, A. (2018a). The evolution of religion and morality: a synthesis of ethnographic and experimental evidence from eight societies. Religion, Brain & Behavior, 8(2), 101132.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Purzycki, B. G., Ross, C. T., Apicella, C., Atkinson, Q. D., Cohen, E., McNamara, R. A., …, Henrich, J. (2018b). Material security, life history, and moralistic religions: A cross-cultural examination. PloS One, 13(3), e0193856.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Purzycki, B. G., Stagnaro, M. N., & Sasaki, J. (2020). Breaches of trust change the content and structure of religious appeals. Journal for the Study of Religion, Nature and Culture, 14(1), 7194.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Purzycki, B. G., Bendixen, T., Lightner, A. D., & Sosis, R. (2022a). Gods, games, and the socioecological landscape. Current Research in Ecological and Social Psychology, 3, 100057.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Purzycki, B. G., Lang, M., Henrich, J., & Norenzayan, A. (2022b). The evolution of religion and morality: Reflections and looking ahead. Religion, Brain & Behavior, 12, 190211.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Purzycki, B. G., Willard, A. K., Klocová, E. K., Apicella, C., Atkinson, Q., Bolyanatz, A., …, Ross, C. T. (2022c). The moralization bias of Gods’ minds: A cross-cultural test. Religion, Brain and Behavior, 12(1–2), 3860.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
R Core Team (2021). R: A language and environment for statistical computing. R Foundation for Statistical Computing. https://www.R-project.org/Google Scholar
Robins, J. M. (1986). A new approach to causal inference in mortality studies with a sustained exposure period – Application to control of the healthy worker survivor effect. Mathematical Modelling, 7(9–12), 13931512.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Robinson, W. S. (2009). Ecological correlations and the behavior of individuals. International Journal of Epidemiology, 38(2), 337341.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Roos, P., Gelfand, M., Nau, D., & Lun, J. (2015). Societal threat and cultural variation in the strength of social norms: An evolutionary basis. Organizational Behavior and Human Decision Processes, 129, 1423.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Ruiter, S., & Van Tubergen, F. (2009). Religious attendance in cross-national perspective: A multilevel analysis of 60 countries. American Journal of Sociology, 115(3), 863–95.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Schnabel, L., Hackett, C., & McClendon, D. (2018). Where men appear more religious than women: Turning a gender lens on religion in Israel. Journal for the Scientific Study of Religion, 57(1), 8094.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Schwadel, P. (2011). The effects of education on americans’ religious practices, beliefs, and affiliations. Review of Religious Research, 53, 161182.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Schwadel, P. (2015). Explaining cross-national variation in the effect of higher educaiton on religiosity. Journal for the Scientific Study of Religion, 54(2), 402418.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Shaver, J. H., & Sosis, R. (2014). How does male ritual behavior vary across the lifespan? Human Nature, 25(1), 136160.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Shaver, J. H., Power, E. A., Purzycki, B. G., Watts, J., Sear, R., Shenk, M. K., …, Bulbulia, J. A. (2020). Church attendance and alloparenting: an analysis of fertility, social support and child development among english mothers. Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society B, 375(1805), 20190428.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Sibley, C. G., & Bulbulia, J. (2012). Faith after an earthquake: A longitudinal study of religion and perceived health before and after the 2011 Christchurch New Zealand earthquake. PloS One, 7(12), e49648.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Simpson, E. H. (1951). The interpretation of interaction in contingency tables. Journal of the Royal Statistical Society: Series B (Methodological), 13(2), 238241.Google Scholar
Snowden, J. M., Rose, S., & Mortimer, K. M. (2011). Implementation of g-computation on a simulated data set: Demonstration of a causal inference technique. American Journal of Epidemiology, 173(7), 731738.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Solt, F., Habel, P., & Grant, J. T. (2011). Economic inequality, relative power, and religiosity. Social Science Quarterly, 92(2), 447465.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Sosis, R. (2007). Psalms for safety: Magico-religious responses to threats of terror. Current Anthropology, 48(6), 903911.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Sosis, R., & Bressler, E. R. (2003). Cooperation and commune longevity: A test of the costly signaling theory of religion. Cross-Cultural Research, 37(2), 211239.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Sosis, R., & Ruffle, B. J. (2003). Religious ritual and cooperation: Testing for a relationship on Israeli religious and secular kibbutzim. Current Anthropology, 44(5), 713722.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Sosis, R., & Handwerker, W. P. (2011). Psalms and coping with uncertainty: Religious Israeli women's responses to the 2006 Lebanon war. American Anthropologist, 113(1), 4055.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Stark, R. (1999). Secularization, RIP. Sociology of Religion, 60(3), 249273.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Stark, R. (2001). Gods, rituals, and the moral order. Journal for the Scientific Study of Religion, 40(4), 619636.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Storm, I. (2017). Does economic insecurity predict religiosity? Evidence from the European social survey 2002–2014. Sociology of Religion, 78(2), 146172.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Strassmann, B. I., & Gillespie, B. (2002). Life-history theory, fertility and reproductive success in humans. Proceedings of the Royal Society of London. Series B: Biological Sciences, 269(1491), 553562.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
VanderWeele, T. J., & Ding, P. (2017). Sensitivity analysis in observational research: introducing the e-value. Annals of Internal Medicine 167(4), 268274.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
VanderWeele, T. J., Jackson, J. W., & Li, S. (2016). Causal inference and longitudinal data: a case study of religion and mental health. Social Psychiatry and Psychiatric Epidemiology, 51, 14571466.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Vardy, T., Moya, C., Placek, C. D., Apicella, C. L., Bolyanatz, A., Cohen, E., …, Atkinson, Q. D. (2022). The religiosity gender gap in 14 diverse societies. Religion, Brain & Behavior, 12(1–2), 1837.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Walter, T., & Davie, G. (1998). The religiosity of women in the modern West. B ritish Journal of Sociology, 49(4), 640660.Google Scholar
Weber, M. (1993) [1920]. The sociology of religion. Beacon Press.Google Scholar
Westreich, D., & Greenland, S. (2013). The Table 2 Fallacy: Presenting and interpreting confounder and modifier coefficients. American Journal of Epidemiology, 177(4), 292298.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Woodward, J. (2005). Making things happen: A theory of causal explanation. Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Wysocki, A. C., Lawson, K. M., & Rhemtulla, M. (2022). Statistical control requires causal justification. Advances in Methods and Practices in Psychological Science, 5(2), 25152459221095823.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Figure 0

Figure 1. Directed acyclic graph of the assumed causal structure. Bold variables are focal variables.

Figure 1

Figure 2. Map of 14 field sites. Note that there are two field sites in Tanna (Inland and Coastal).

Figure 2

Table 1. Means (standard deviations) for some individual-level demographic variables and group-level properties of communities sampled. ‘Mjr trad.’ refers to the major colonial and/or overarching tradition of each site while ‘Mnr trad.’ refers to relatively minor – but locally salient – traditions in each site.

Figure 3

Figure 3. Food insecurity and religious ideation. Posterior means for each individual and outcome response option under hypothetical exposures to food (in)security.

Figure 4

Figure 4. Food insecurity and religious behaviour. Posterior means for each individual and outcome response option under hypothetical exposures to food (in)security.

Figure 5

Figure 5. Years of formal education and religious ideation. Posterior means for each individual and outcome response option under hypothetical number of years of formal education.

Figure 6

Figure 6. Years of formal education and religious behaviour. Posterior means for each individual and outcome response option under hypothetical number of years of formal education.

Supplementary material: File

Purzycki and Bendixen supplementary material 1

Purzycki and Bendixen supplementary material
Download Purzycki and Bendixen supplementary material 1(File)
File 40.3 KB
Supplementary material: File

Purzycki and Bendixen supplementary material 2

Purzycki and Bendixen supplementary material
Download Purzycki and Bendixen supplementary material 2(File)
File 41.4 KB
Supplementary material: File

Purzycki and Bendixen supplementary material 3

Purzycki and Bendixen supplementary material
Download Purzycki and Bendixen supplementary material 3(File)
File 379.3 KB
Supplementary material: File

Purzycki and Bendixen supplementary material 4

Purzycki and Bendixen supplementary material
Download Purzycki and Bendixen supplementary material 4(File)
File 885.2 KB