Hostname: page-component-7c8c6479df-ws8qp Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-03-19T10:11:03.966Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

Associations between postprandial insulin and blood glucose responses, appetite sensations and energy intake in normal weight and overweight individuals: a meta-analysis of test meal studies

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  01 July 2007

Anne Flint
Affiliation:
Novo Nordisk A/S, Smørmosevej 2, DK-2880 Bagsværd, Denmark
Nikolaj T. Gregersen*
Affiliation:
Department of Human Nutrition, The Royal Veterinary and Agricultural University, Rolighedsvej 30, DK-1958 Frederiksberg C, Denmark
Lise L. Gluud
Affiliation:
Copenhagen Trial Unit, Centre for Clinical Intervention Research, Rigshospitalet, Dept 33.44, DK-2100 Copenhagen Ø, Denmark
Bente K. Møller
Affiliation:
Department of Human Nutrition, The Royal Veterinary and Agricultural University, Rolighedsvej 30, DK-1958 Frederiksberg C, Denmark
Anne Raben
Affiliation:
Novo Nordisk A/S, Smørmosevej 2, DK-2880 Bagsværd, Denmark
Inge Tetens
Affiliation:
Department of Nutrition, Danish Institute for Food and Veterinary Research, Søborg, Denmark
Camilla Verdich
Affiliation:
Institute of Preventive Medicine, Copenhagen University Hospital, Centre for Health and Society, Øster Søgade 18, DK-1357 Copenhagen K, Denmark
Arne Astrup
Affiliation:
Department of Human Nutrition, The Royal Veterinary and Agricultural University, Rolighedsvej 30, DK-1958 Frederiksberg C, Denmark
*
*Corresponding author: Nikolaj Ture Gregersen, PhD, fax +45 35282483, email ntg@life.ku.dk
Rights & Permissions [Opens in a new window]

Abstract

It is unclear whether postprandial blood glucose or insulin exerts a regulatory function in short-term appetite regulation in humans. The aim of this study was to investigate, by use of meta-analysis, the role of blood glucose and insulin in short-term appetite sensation and energy intake (EI) in normal weight and overweight participants. Data from seven test meal studies were used, including 136 healthy participants (ALL) (92 normal weight (NW) and 44 overweight or obese (OW)). All meals were served as breakfasts after an overnight fast, and appetite sensations and blood samples were obtained frequently in the postprandial period. Finally, an ad libitum lunch was served. Data were analysed by fixed effects study level (SL) meta-regression analysis and individual participant data (IPD) regression analysis, using STATA software. In SL analysis, postprandial insulin response was associated with decreased hunger in ALL, NW and OW (P < 0·019), and with increased satiety in NW (P = 0·004) and lower subsequent EI in OW (P = 0·022). Multivariate IPD analysis showed similar associations, but only in NW for hunger, satiety and EI (P < 0·028), and in ALL for EI (P = 0·016). The only association involving blood glucose was the multivariate IPD analysis showing an inverse association between blood glucose and EI in ALL (P = 0·032). Our results suggest that insulin, but not glucose, is associated with short-term appetite regulation in healthy participants, but the relationship is disrupted in the overweight and obese. We conclude that the postprandial insulin response may be an important satiety signal, and that central nervous system insulin resistance in overweight might explain the blunted effect on appetite.

Type
Review Article
Copyright
Copyright © The Authors 2007

Fifty years ago, Mayer (Reference Mayer1955) proposed that changes in blood glucose concentrations or arteriovenous glucose differences were registered by glucoreceptors situated in the parts of the hypothalamus that affect energy intake, and perhaps in the periphery as well. According to this theory, an increase in blood glucose concentrations results in increased feelings of satiety whereas a drop in blood glucose concentrations has the opposite effect. This glucostatic theory has received support from studies showing that transient declines in blood glucose concentrations preceded meal initiation (Campfield & Smith, Reference Campfield and Smith1990; Smith & Campfield, Reference Smith and Campfield1993; Anderson & Woodend, Reference Anderson and Woodend2003). In addition, blood glucose concentrations following a meal have been shown to be inversely associated with subjective appetite and food intake (Anderson et al. Reference Anderson, Catherine, Woodend and Wolever2002). However, another study found no associations between blood glucose concentrations and satiety, when blood glucose concentrations were raised by intravenous infusion (Lavin et al. Reference Lavin, Wittert, Sun, Horowitz, Morley and Read1996). Furthermore, several factors (e.g. insulin and incretin hormones) covary with postprandial blood glucose concentrations, which therefore makes it difficult to determine whether it is changes in blood glucose per se or other factors that are responsible for the satiety-promoting effect (Holt et al. Reference Holt, Brand Miller and Petocz1996; Raben et al. Reference Raben, Holst, Christensen and Astrup1996). Thus, it is not clear whether blood glucose per se is associated with short-term regulation of appetite.

Like blood glucose, insulin has been suggested to be involved in short-term appetite regulation (Lavin et al. Reference Lavin, Wittert, Sun, Horowitz, Morley and Read1996). However, the results of insulin infusion studies have been conflicting since one of these studies has shown that increased insulin concentrations promote hunger and thereby increase energy intake at a subsequent ad libitum meal (Rodin et al. Reference Rodin, Wack, Ferrannini and DeFronzo1985), whereas others found no associations between insulin concentrations and appetite sensations (Woo et al. Reference Woo, Kissileff and Pi-Sunyer1984; Gielkens et al. Reference Gielkens, Verkijk, Lam, Lamers and Masclee1998). Furthermore, a recent study has shown that insulin concentrations before a test meal were inversely related to subsequent ad libitum energy intake (EI) in normal weight, but not in obese males (Speechly & Buffenstein, Reference Speechly and Buffenstein2000). Thus, the role of insulin in short-term appetite regulation is controversial.

Since high concentrations of blood glucose and insulin are associated with obesity (Bonadonna et al. Reference Bonadonna, Groop, Kraemer, Ferrannini, Del Prato and DeFronzo1990), it seems possible that the potential effects of blood glucose and insulin in the short-term appetite regulation may differ between normal weight (NW) and overweight or obese (OW) participants.

Thus, the above-mentioned conflicting results call for more studies in order to clarify the roles of blood glucose and insulin in short-term appetite regulation. Therefore, the aim of the present study was to examine whether postprandial concentrations of blood glucose and insulin are associated with sensations of satiety and hunger, and subsequent ad libitum EI. Furthermore, we investigated whether these associations differ between NW and OW participants. A data set composed of participant data from six previously published studies and from one as yet unpublished study was used. To increase study power, these associations were analysed using meta-analysis.

Methods

Selection and inclusion of studies

We included all test meal studies that were conducted at the Department of Human Nutrition, the Royal Veterinary and Agricultural University, Denmark in the period 1990–2003, and fulfilled the criteria of inclusion of having individual participant data (IPD) on blood glucose, insulin and appetite scores (visual analogue scale (VAS)). From a total of 20 test meal studies, thirteen were excluded because they used ad libitum meals for breakfast, artificial meals, included only hyperinsulinaemic or diabetic participants, did not include appetite scores or because the data had not been analysed at the time of inclusion. Six of the seven studies that fulfilled all the above-mentioned criteria have been previously published (Flint et al. Reference Flint, Raben, Astrup and Holst1998, Reference Flint, Raben, Ersbøll, Holst and Astrup2001, Reference Flint, Møller, Raben, Pedersen, Tetens, Holst and Astrup2004a; Verdich et al. Reference Verdich, Toubro, Buemann, Madsen, Holst and Astrup2001b; Bakhøj et al. Reference Bakhøj, Flint, Holst and Tetens2003; Raben et al. Reference Raben, Agerholm-Larsen, Flint, Holst and Astrup2003), whereas the last study has been published as an abstract (Flint et al. Reference Flint, Tangaa, Rugbjerg, Raben, Astrup and Holst2004b).

Participants

From the seven studies, only the participants with complete data for blood glucose, insulin and appetite registrations were included in this meta-analysis. Eleven participants were excluded due to missing values, leaving 136 participants in the meta-analysis. Five of the participants were female. The sample size of the seven studies varied between ten and thirty-one. All of the participants were healthy, non-smoking non-athletes. The mean age of the participants was 29·4 years (sd 8·8). Ninety-two of the participants had a normal BMI ( ≤ 25 kg/m2) and were characterised as being NW, while the remaining 44 participants had a higher BMI (>25 kg/m2) and were characterised as being OW. All OW participants were male.

Design of the selected studies

In all studies, the participants were served a test meal in the morning after a 10–12 h fast. The test meal was followed by a postprandial period of 180–315 min (Table 1), during which insulin, blood glucose and sensations of hunger and satiety were measured frequently. In six of the studies, the participants were served an ad libitum meal at the end of the postprandial period. In the seventh study, the end of the postprandial period was the end of the study (Bakhøj et al. Reference Bakhøj, Flint, Holst and Tetens2003). The macronutrient composition of the test meals varied markedly between the seven studies (for further information on meal type and composition, see the seven studies (Flint et al. Reference Flint, Raben, Astrup and Holst1998, Reference Flint, Raben, Ersbøll, Holst and Astrup2001; Reference Flint, Møller, Raben, Pedersen, Tetens, Holst and Astrup2004a, Reference Flint, Tangaa, Rugbjerg, Raben, Astrup and Holstb; Verdich et al. Reference Verdich, Toubro, Buemann, Madsen, Holst and Astrup2001b; Bakhøj et al. Reference Bakhøj, Flint, Holst and Tetens2003; Raben et al. Reference Raben, Agerholm-Larsen, Flint, Holst and Astrup2003)).

Table 1 Characteristics of the seven test meal studies

NW, normal weight; OW, overweight; EItestmeal, energy intake in the test meal.

* This study included five women who were all normal weight.

In three of the studies, the effects of infused glucose-dependent insulinotropic polypeptide or glucagon-like peptide 1 (GLP-1) on different variables including appetite sensations and ad libitum EI were studied using a placebo-controlled randomised crossover design and blinding of participants (Flint et al. Reference Flint, Raben, Astrup and Holst1998, Reference Flint, Raben, Ersbøll, Holst and Astrup2001) or participants and researchers (Flint et al. Reference Flint, Tangaa, Rugbjerg, Raben, Astrup and Holst2004b). From these studies, only data from the control situations were used. In three of the other studies, the effects of different test meals on different outcomes were examined using randomised crossover designs (Bakhøj et al. Reference Bakhøj, Flint, Holst and Tetens2003; Raben et al. Reference Raben, Agerholm-Larsen, Flint, Holst and Astrup2003; Flint et al. Reference Flint, Møller, Raben, Pedersen, Tetens, Holst and Astrup2004a). In the seventh study, a test meal was given once to the NW participants and twice to the OW participants, namely before and after a 6-month weight loss period (Verdich et al. Reference Verdich, Toubro, Buemann, Madsen, Holst and Astrup2001b).

All studies were approved by the Municipal Ethical Committee of Copenhagen and Frederiksberg, Denmark, and all the participants gave informed written consent before entering the studies.

Plasma concentrations of insulin and blood glucose

In all seven studies, venous blood was drawn and analysed for plasma glucose and serum insulin. Serum insulin concentrations were measured using radioimmunoassay according to the principles described by Albano et al. (Reference Albano, Ekins, Maritz and Turner1972). Plasma glucose concentrations were measured by standard enzymatic methods on a Cobas Mira (Roche Diagnostic Systems, Basel, Switzerland) (Deeg et al. Reference Deeg, Kraemer and Ziegenhorn1980).

Subjective appetite ratings and ad libitum energy intake

In all seven studies, the participants rated their hunger and satiety using a VAS (Flint et al. Reference Flint, Raben, Blundell and Astrup2000). The VAS were 100 mm long horizontal lines with words anchored at each end, expressing the most negative and most positive rating (for hunger these were: ‘I am not hungry at all’ and ‘I have never been more hungry’). The participants were asked to make a univocal mark on the horizontal line that corresponds to their feelings.

In six of the studies (Flint et al. Reference Flint, Raben, Astrup and Holst1998, Reference Flint, Raben, Ersbøll, Holst and Astrup2001, Reference Flint, Møller, Raben, Pedersen, Tetens, Holst and Astrup2004a, Reference Flint, Tangaa, Rugbjerg, Raben, Astrup and Holstb; Verdich et al. Reference Verdich, Toubro, Buemann, Madsen, Holst and Astrup2001b; Raben et al. Reference Raben, Agerholm-Larsen, Flint, Holst and Astrup2003), participants were served an ad libitum lunch and told to eat until comfortably satisfied. In four of the studies, the ad libitum meal was a hot-pot consisting of pasta and meat sauce with vegetables (Flint et al. Reference Flint, Raben, Astrup and Holst1998, Reference Flint, Raben, Ersbøll, Holst and Astrup2001, Reference Flint, Tangaa, Rugbjerg, Raben, Astrup and Holst2004b; Raben et al. Reference Raben, Agerholm-Larsen, Flint, Holst and Astrup2003). In the two remaining studies, it was a cold pasta salad with vegetables, ham and dressing (Verdich et al. Reference Verdich, Toubro, Buemann, Madsen, Holst and Astrup2001b; Flint et al. Reference Flint, Møller, Raben, Pedersen, Tetens, Holst and Astrup2004a). The macronutrient composition of the ad libitum meals was quite similar, with 13–16 E% (percentage of energy) from protein, 48–50 E% from carbohydrates and 35–37 E% from fat. The ad libitum EI (EIad libitum) was measured by weighing and calculations based on the assumption of a homogenous distribution of energy and macronutrients in the meal.

Summary measures

Incremental area under the curve (iAUC) was calculated using the trapezoid rule leaving out negative values. iAUC was used as a summary measure for the variables insulin (iAUCinsulin), blood glucose (iAUCglucose) and satiety (iAUCsatiety). For hunger, the incremental area over the curve (iAOChunger) under the fasting level was used. This was done because the fasting level of hunger is most often higher than the levels of hunger during the postprandial period. Thus, calculating iAUC for hunger would lead to negative values. Furthermore, we used two additional variables for both insulin and blood glucose, namely fasting concentrations (FASTinsulin, FASTglucose) and concentrations immediately before the ad libitum meal (ENDinsulin, ENDglucose).

Statistics

Meta-analysis is a statistical technique used to integrate the results from separate studies (Alderson et al. 2003a). In this study, two different types of meta-analysis were conducted.

Weighted fixed effects study level (SL) meta-regression analyses models were chosen for the primary analyses because outcomes were measured in a standard way across studies. The mean effect size and variance were calculated for each study. Studies were then weighted according to the reciprocal of their variance, thereby giving more weight to the studies with the narrower confidence intervals. The SL analyses were conducted in order to examine whether potential effects of insulin and blood glucose on appetite sensations and ad libitum EI were consistent across studies. SL analyses were conducted on all participants (ALL) and as subgroup analyses on NW and OW participants separately. The analyses were conducted with iAUCsatiety, iAOChunger and EIad libitum as evaluated outcomes. Furthermore, the evaluated covariates were iAUCinsulin, iAUCglucose, EItest  meal, AGE and duration of the postprandial period. SL sensitivity analysis was conducted to test the effect of leaving out the five female participants. Furthermore, additional SL analyses were conducted to test the effect of FASTinsulin, FASTglucose, ENDinsulin and ENDglucose. All the SL analyses were univariate due to the limited number of studies. Furthermore, all the SL analyses were conducted using the fixed effects approach based on the assumption that only intra-study variations affected the results. However, to examine the degree of heterogeneity (inter-study variation) and stability, and thus whether the choice of using fixed effects models was reasonable, random effects SL analyses were also performed. This random effects approach is based on the assumption that both intra- and inter-study variations affect the results of the analyses.

To evaluate the effect of potential confounding factors, univariate and multivariate regression analyses with individual participant data (IPD) were performed. In order to perform IPD analyses, raw data from each of the included studies were collected. In the IPD analyses, the data were treated as if they originated from a single study. The IPD analyses were conducted on ALL participants, and as subgroup analyses on NW and OW. The evaluated outcomes were iAUCsatiety, iAOChunger and EIad libitum. Furthermore, iAUCinsulin, iAUCglucose, AGE, EItest meal and the duration of the postprandial period were included as covariates.

Apart from the meta-analyses, a Student's t test was conducted to examine whether there was a difference between the NW and OW with respect to insulin and blood glucose concentrations, satiety, hunger and EIad libitum. Furthermore, a Spearman correlation analysis was performed to test whether congruity existed between the VAS scores and the subsequent EIad libitum.

All the statistical analyses in the present report were conducted using STATA (version 8·0 for Windows, Stata Corporation, TX, USA). In order to reduce the risk of bias, all statistical analyses were pre-specified. All the continuous variables except BMI were ln-transformed. BMI was dichotomised with a cut-off value of 25, creating two categories classified as NW and OW, respectively. In some of the included studies, the participants received more than one test meal. Mean values were used for these participants, so that each participant was included only once in the analyses. Mean values were also used in one of the studies, where the participants received identical meals before and after a 6-month weight loss period (Verdich et al. Reference Verdich, Toubro, Buemann, Madsen, Holst and Astrup2001b). The results of the SL and IPD analyses were expressed by regression coefficients with corresponding se and level of significance (P). The level of significance was set to P < 0·05.

Results

Insulin and short-term appetite regulation

The SL analysis including ALL participants showed that iAUCinsulin was positively correlated with iAOChunger, meaning that a larger postprandial iAUCinsulin is associated with smaller increases in sensations of hunger (Table 2). However, only tendencies were seen for iAUCinsulin to be correlated with iAUCsatiety and EIad libitum for ALL participants (Table 2). According to the subgroup SL analyses of NW, iAUCinsulin was positively correlated with iAUCsatiety and iAOChunger, but not associated with EIad libitum (Fig. 1). This indicates that increased insulin levels in the NW group are associated with increased sensations of satiety and decreased sensations of hunger. The subgroup SL analyses of OW showed that iAUCinsulin was positively correlated with iAOChunger (Fig. 2) and inversely correlated with EIad libitum. Thus, the larger the iAUCinsulin, the smaller the sensations of hunger and the smaller the subsequent EIad libitum. The sensitivity SL analysis without the female participants showed results similar to the SL analysis including ALL participants, with iAUCinsulin being positively correlated to iAOChunger (Table 2). Additional SL analyses showed that FASTinsulin did not correlate with iAUCsatiety, iAOChunger or EIad libitum (P ≥ 0·3). Likewise, ENDinsulin did not correlate with EIad libitum (P>0·1).

Table 2 Study level meta-regression analyses: effects of insulin and glucose levels on satiety, hunger and ad libitum energy intake for all, normal weight, overweight male participants

ALL, all participants; NW, normal weight participants; OW, overweight or obese participants; iAUCinsulin, iAUCglucose and iAUCsatiety, incremental area under the insulin, glucose and satiety response curves; iAOChunger, incremental area over the hunger response curve; EIad libitum, ad libitum energy intake.

Data are expressed as regression coefficients with their standard error and corresponding level of significance (P). Level of significance was P < 0·05, Sensitivity analyses were conducted on male participants only.

Age, duration of the postprandial period and energy intake in the test meal were included as covariates in the analyses. Furthermore, iAUCglucose was included as a covariate in the analyses with iAUCinsulin as independent variable, and vice versa.

Fig. 1 Scatter plot of relationships between postprandial insulin concentrations and ratings of satiety for normal weight (NW) and overweight (OW) participants. The associations shown in this figure are: NW (r = 0·76; P = 0·004) and OW (r = 0·21; P = 0·351). iAUCinsulin and iAUCsatiety, incremental area under the insulin and satiety response curves; filled circles represent point estimates from the individual studies (reference numbers next to the circles refer to study numbers: 1, Bakhoj et al. Reference Bakhøj, Flint, Holst and Tetens2003; 2, Flint et al. Reference Flint, Møller, Raben, Pedersen, Tetens, Holst and Astrup2004a; 3, Flint et al. Reference Flint, Raben, Astrup and Holst1998; 4, Flint et al. Reference Flint, Raben, Ersbøll, Holst and Astrup2001; 5, Flint et al. Reference Flint, Tangaa, Rugbjerg, Raben, Astrup and Holst2004b; 6, Raben et al. Reference Raben, Agerholm-Larsen, Flint, Holst and Astrup2003; 7, Verdich et al. Reference Verdich, Toubro, Buemann, Madsen, Holst and Astrup2001b), whereas the slopes of the lines represent the regression coefficients with corresponding 95 % confidence intervals. The values on the x- and y-axes are not absolute values, but instead the values that resulted from the ln transformation of the data.

Fig. 2 Scatter plot of relationships between postprandial insulin concentrations and ratings of hunger for normal weight (NW) and overweight (OW) participants. The associations shown in this figure are: NW (r = 0·73; P = 0·018) and OW (r = 0·33; P = 0·012). iAUCinsulin, incremental area under the insulin response curve; iAOChunger, incremental area over the hunger response curve. The filled circles represent point estimates from the individual studies (reference numbers next to the circles refer to study numbers: 1, Bakhoj et al. Reference Bakhøj, Flint, Holst and Tetens2003; 2, Flint et al. Reference Flint, Møller, Raben, Pedersen, Tetens, Holst and Astrup2004a; 3, Flint et al. Reference Flint, Raben, Astrup and Holst1998; 4, Flint et al. Reference Flint, Raben, Ersbøll, Holst and Astrup2001; 5, Flint et al. Reference Flint, Tangaa, Rugbjerg, Raben, Astrup and Holst2004b; 6, Raben et al. Reference Raben, Agerholm-Larsen, Flint, Holst and Astrup2003; 7, Verdich et al. Reference Verdich, Toubro, Buemann, Madsen, Holst and Astrup2001b), whereas the slopes of the lines represent the regression coefficients with corresponding 95 % confidence intervals. The values on the x- and y-axes are not absolute values, but instead the values that resulted from the ln transformation of the data.

The univariate IPD analysis including ALL participants showed that iAUCinsulin was positively correlated to both iAUCsatiety and iAOChunger, and inversely associated with EIad libitum (Table 3). However, when tested in multivariate IPD analysis with ALL participants, only the inverse correlation between iAUCinsulin and EIad libitum remained significant. In the univariate subgroup IPD analysis, including NW iAUCinsulin was positively correlated to iAUCsatiety and iAOChunger, and inversely correlated to EIad libitum. When tested in multivariate analysis (adjusted for iAUCglucose, AGE, EItestmeal and the duration of the postprandial period), iAUCinsulin was correlated to all three outcomes in NW. According to the univariate subgroup IPD analysis, including OW participants' iAUCinsulin was only inversely correlated to EIad libitum. However, no associations were seen between iAUCinsulin and the outcomes when tested in multivariate analysis including OW participants.

Table 3 Univariate and multivariate individual participant data regression analyses: effects of insulin and glucose levels on satiety, hunger and ad libitum energy intake for all, normal weight and overweight participants

ALL, all participants; NW, normal weight participants; OW, overweight or obese participants; iAUCinsulin, iAUCglucose and iAUCsatiety,incremental area under the insulin, glucose and satiety response curves; iAOChunger, incremental area over the hunger response curve; EIad libitum, ad libitum energy intake.

Results are listed as regression coefficients with their standard error and corresponding level of significance (P). Level of significance was P < 0·05. Multivariate analyses included iAUCinsulin, iAUCglucose, AGE, EItest meal and the duration of the postprandial period as covariates.

Age, duration of the postprandial period and energy intake in the test meal were included as covariates in the multivariate analyses. Furthermore, iAUCglucose was included as a covariate in the multivariate analyses with iAUCinsulin as independent variable, and vice versa.

Blood glucose and short-term appetite regulation

The SL analysis including ALL participants showed that iAUCglucose was not associated with any of the three outcomes (Table 2). Likewise, when tested in sensitivity SL analysis without women, iAUCglucose was still not correlated with any of the outcomes. The subgroup SL analyses for NW and OW did not show any associations between iAUCglucose and the outcomes. Additional SL analyses showed that FASTglucose did not correlate with iAUCsatiety, iAOChunger or EIad libitum (P>0·8). Likewise, ENDglucose was not associated with EIad libitum (P>0·2).

Univariate IPD analysis including ALL participants showed that iAUCglucose was positively correlated to iAOChunger and inversely correlated to EIad libitum (Table 3). However, when tested in a multivariate analysis, only the inverse association between iAUCglucose and EIad libitum remained significant. According to the univariate subgroup IPD analysis, including NW iAUCglucose was only positively correlated to iAOChunger, but when tested in multivariate analysis no associations at all were seen. Univariate subgroup IPD analysis including OW showed that iAUCglucose correlated inversely to EIad libitum. However, the multivariate subgroup IPD analysis found no associations between iAUCglucose and the three outcomes.

The P-values and confidence intervals did not differ markedly between the random effects and fixed effects models in the meta-regression analyses, meaning that the heterogeneity seems small and therefore using the fixed effects models for the SL meta-regression analyses seems reasonable.

The t-test showed that OW participants had significantly larger levels of iAUCinsulin (P = 0·001) and iAUCglucose (P = 0·001) than their NW counterparts. However, NW participants had a larger EIad libitum than OW participants (P = 0·028).

Finally, the Spearman correlation analyses showed that for NW participants, EIad libitum correlated with iAUCsatiety (r = − 0·2234, P = 0·045) and iAOChunger (r = − 0·2476, P = 0·028). However, for ALL and OW participants no correlations were found.

Discussion

The results of the present meta-analysis suggest that insulin, but not blood glucose, might be an important satiety signal in short-term appetite regulation in NW, but not in OW participants.

The SL analysis including ALL participants showed that the insulin response was significantly and inversely related to hunger and, although this association was strong, only tendencies were seen between the insulin response, and satiety and ad libitum EI. However, according to our subgroup IPD analyses, it seems that these somewhat inconsistent findings from the SL analyses might be due to the pooling of participants with a broad range in BMI, since the subgroup multivariate IPD analyses consistently showed that the insulin response was associated with all three outcomes for NW (e.g. hunger; r = 0·88), but not for OW participants.

To our knowledge, this is the first meta-analysis of these questions of interest. Only a few smaller studies have examined differences between NW and OW individuals with respect to the associations between insulin and short-term appetite sensations (Rodin et al. Reference Rodin, Reed and Jamner1988; Speechly & Buffenstein, Reference Speechly and Buffenstein2000; Verdich et al. Reference Verdich, Toubro, Buemann, Madsen, Holst and Astrup2001b). In the study by Verdich et al. (Reference Verdich, Toubro, Buemann, Madsen, Holst and Astrup2001b), fasting insulin concentrations, total and incremental area under the insulin response curve and the insulin concentrations immediately before the ad libitum meal all correlated inversely with ad libitum EI in lean, but not in obese participants (Verdich et al. Reference Verdich, Toubro, Buemann, Madsen, Holst and Astrup2001b). Likewise, the insulin concentrations immediately before the ad libitum meal also correlated inversely with ad libitum EI in lean, but not in obese participants in the second study (Speechly & Buffenstein, Reference Speechly and Buffenstein2000). In contrast, a third study showed that insulin concentrations at 15, 30 and 45, but not at 95, 135 and 155 min after a test meal were positively correlated with subsequent ad libitum EI in obese, but not in lean participants (Flint et al. Reference Flint, Raben, Blundell and Astrup2000). In our present meta-analysis, the integrated postprandial insulin response seemed the best predictor of satiety and subsequent ad libitum EI, since it was correlated with the outcomes, as opposed to the fasting insulin concentrations and the insulin concentrations immediately before the ad libitum meal.

If insulin does act as a short-term satiety signal, then several mechanisms could be responsible for this effect. First of all, insulin could work directly through insulin receptors in the hypothalamic area, as suggested by Verdich et al. (Reference Verdich, Toubro, Buemann, Madsen, Holst and Astrup2001b). Extensive evidence supports a role for central insulin receptors in the regulation of appetite (Brüning et al. Reference Brüning, Gautam, Burks, Gillette, Schubert, Orban, Klein, Krone, Müller-Wieland and Kahn2000; Porte et al. Reference Porte, Baskin and Schwartz2002). However, the central effect of insulin could also be mediated indirectly through interaction with other satiety-inducing peptides, e.g. cholecystokinin (CCK), GLP-1 and peptide YY (PYY) (Figlewicz et al. Reference Figlewicz, Sipols, Seeley, Chavez, Woods and Porte1995; Schwartz & Morton, Reference Schwartz and Morton2002; Flint et al. Reference Flint, Møller, Raben, Pedersen, Tetens, Holst and Astrup2004a). Another possibility is that insulin affects the intracellular level of ATP in the liver by changing the transport of energy-yielding substrates into the liver. Several studies have proposed that glucose sensors and other metabolic sensors in the liver relay information about the ATP level in the liver through afferent nerves to the brain and thereby change food intake (Friedman, Reference Friedman1995; Langhans, Reference Langhans1996). On the other hand, the results of insulin infusion studies do not support a direct satiating effect of insulin (Woo et al. Reference Woo, Kissileff and Pi-Sunyer1984; Chapman et al. Reference Chapman, Goble, Wittert, Morley and Horowitz1998; Gielkens et al. Reference Gielkens, Verkijk, Lam, Lamers and Masclee1998). However, this may be due to the fact that when insulin is infused intravenously, glucose needs to be infused concomitantly to prevent hypoglycaemia. Consequently, the potential effect of insulin on the energy status of the liver is bypassed and therefore these infusion studies do not reflect normal in vivo circumstances.

The observed differences in insulin's association with short-term appetite regulation between NW and OW participants might be due to decreased central insulin sensitivity in the OW participants, as suggested by Verdich et al. (Reference Verdich, Toubro, Buemann, Madsen, Holst and Astrup2001b). This is supported by a study showing that genetic deletion of the brain insulin receptor causes central insulin resistance and obesity in rodents (Brüning et al. Reference Brüning, Gautam, Burks, Gillette, Schubert, Orban, Klein, Krone, Müller-Wieland and Kahn2000). Further, such decreased central insulin sensitivity could be similar to the decreased peripheral insulin sensitivity, which is a well-known complication of the obese state (Bonadonna et al. Reference Bonadonna, Groop, Kraemer, Ferrannini, Del Prato and DeFronzo1990). In our study, the OW participants had significantly larger iAUCinsulin and iAUCglucose than the NW participants, which supports the hypothesis of decreased insulin sensitivity, hyperinsulinaemia and hyperglycaemia in these participants. However, so far this is rather speculative and thus other mechanisms might be involved. Nevertheless, given that a potential satiating effect of insulin is reduced or diminished in the OW participants, it seems surprising that NW participants had a larger ad libitum EI than the OW participants. Our findings might be explained by the fact that several other mechanisms (e.g. psychological, sensory and cognitive) are involved in the regulation of appetite (Blundell, Reference Blundell1999) and thus might compensate for a reduced satiating effect of insulin and thereby explain the smaller ad libitum EI in the OW participants. In addition, the OW participants in our study might have been more affected by the experimental settings than the NW participants and therefore the ad libitum EI in the OW participants might not have reflected how hungry this group actually was. Furthermore, OW subjects might not be as good as NW subjects in sensing their hunger. This is supported by the results of the correlation analysis, which showed that in NW, but not in OW participants, sensations of hunger and satiety correlate with ad libitum EI.

The lack of association between insulin and appetite for OW subjects could be due to lack of statistical power in this group. However, although the analyses included more than twice as many NW as OW subjects, the sample size of the OW group was still large (n 44). Thus, it seems unlikely that lack of power was the reason for the lack of associations in the OW subjects

Only five of the subjects were female, which means that this group was too small for conducting subgroup analyses. In combination with the fact that the results of the analyses were unchanged by excluding the women, this therefore suggests that the results of this meta-analysis are only applicable to men.

According to the SL analyses, the blood glucose response was not related to any of the outcomes. Furthermore, only the multivariate IPD analysis including all participants revealed an inverse association between the blood glucose response and ad libitum EI. Thus, these results do not support a significant role for blood glucose in short-term appetite regulation. This is supported by other studies examining the role of blood glucose in relation to appetite sensations and EI (Lavin et al. Reference Lavin, Wittert, Sun, Horowitz, Morley and Read1996; Porte et al. Reference Porte, Baskin and Schwartz2002). However, it has been suggested that the potential effect of insulin as a satiety signal might require raised blood glucose concentrations (Chapman et al. Reference Chapman, Goble, Wittert, Morley and Horowitz1998). Nevertheless, in our meta-analysis, the associations between the insulin response and the outcomes do not seem to be due to blood glucose. On the other hand, a number of previous studies have suggested that a decline in blood glucose concentrations precedes meal initiation (Campfield & Smith, Reference Campfield and Smith1990; Smith & Campfield, Reference Smith and Campfield1993). Our study showed no correlation between the blood glucose concentrations immediately before the ad libitum meal and ad libitum EI. However, due to the fixed timing of the protocols, this does not necessarily exclude a role for blood glucose in meal initiation. Thus, whether this pre-meal decline in blood glucose actually is a causal factor in meal initiation is still controversial (De Graaf et al. Reference De Graaf, Blom, Smeets, Stafleu and Hendriks2004).

Even though the correlations between the insulin response and the three outcomes observed in the NW subjects did not depend on the examined covariates, this does not exclude that other potential confounding factors might have been responsible for these correlations. The most obvious potentially confounding factors seem to be the incretin hormones, glucose-dependent insulinotropic polypeptide and GLP-1. GLP-1 has been proposed to have a satiating effect, which does not depend on its effects on insulin (Lavin et al. Reference Lavin, Wittert, Andrews, Yeap, Wishart, Morris, Morley, Horowitz and Read1998). Other studies have supported the hypothesis of a satiety-promoting effect of GLP-1 (Verdich et al. Reference Verdich, Flint, Gutzwiller, Näslund, Beglinger, Hellström, Long, Morgan, Holst and Astrup2001a), whereas glucose-dependent insulinotropic polypeptide does not seem to influence appetite sensation (Flint et al. Reference Flint, Tangaa, Rugbjerg, Raben, Astrup and Holst2004b). However, one study found that insulin concentrations did not correlate with incretin hormones, but still explained 67 % of the variations between ad libitum EI in lean participants (Verdich et al. Reference Verdich, Toubro, Buemann, Madsen, Holst and Astrup2001b). Thus, further studies are required to clarify how insulin and the incretin hormones interact in the regulation of appetite.

A common problem when conducting several statistical analyses is that the risk of mass significance (type I errors) increases as the number of subgroup analyses and characteristics examined increases (Deeks et al. 2003). In this meta-analysis, six different subgroup analyses, each including three different outcomes and 2–5 covariates, were conducted. Therefore, it is possible that the analyses might have yielded false-positive results. However, several precautions were taken to reduce the risk of mass significance, including conducting sensitivity analyses and pre-specifying all analyses. A possible approach to reduce the risk of mass significance is to adjust the level of significance from P < 0·05 to P < 0·01 or P < 0·001, and thereby increase the demands for statistical significance (Alderson et al. 2003b). If we in this study set the level of statistical significance to P < 0·01, the overall results and conclusions, especially those pertaining to insulin, do not change. Thus, it seems unlikely that mass significance was responsible for the consistent findings of significant correlations between insulin and the three outcomes in this meta-analysis.

A limitation of this meta-analysis is that all the included studies were conducted at the Department of Human Nutrition in Copenhagen, which increases the risk of our inclusion criteria being biased (Egger & Smith, Reference Egger and Smith1998). Furthermore, the seven included studies were all rather small (n10–31). However, to our knowledge, only a few studies apart from those included in our meta-analysis have examined the roles of insulin (and blood glucose) in short-term appetite regulation using this methodology, and these studies were also rather small (n12–38; Speechly & Buffenstein, Reference Speechly and Buffenstein2000; Alderson et al. 2003a). Thus, our meta-analysis might be one of the most comprehensive studies to address this subject. Finally, the problem with the interpretation of the results of this meta-analysis is that the present design is an observational post hoc analysis where it is not possible to draw any conclusions about the causality of the associations (Thompson & Higgins, Reference Thompson and Higgins2002). Nevertheless, in line with the study by Verdich et al. (Reference Verdich, Toubro, Buemann, Madsen, Holst and Astrup2001b), we suggest that insulin might be a mediator of satiety sensations in the postprandial period, and thereby decrease ad libitum EI in NW participants.

Conclusion

In summary, our results suggest that insulin, but not glucose, is associated with short-term appetite regulation in healthy NW participants, and that the association seems to weaken or disappear when body weight increases. We conclude that the postprandial insulin response may be an important satiety signal, and propose that central nervous system insulin resistance in overweight individuals might explain the blunted effect on appetite.

Acknowledgements

The authors gratefully thank Inge Timmermann, Charlotte Kostecki, Karina G. Larsen and Christina Cuthbertson for expert technical assistance.

At the time of the study, A. F. and A. R. were employed at the Department of Human Nutrition, The Royal Veterinary and Agricultural University, Copenhagen. Now they are employed at Novo Nordisk A/S, a company that manufactures and markets pharmaceuticals related to the treatment of diabetes and its complications.

This research was supported by the Danish Research Agency (5013-00-0031) and the European Commission FP6 (contract no. LSHM-CT-2003-503041).

This publication reflects the authors' views and not necessarily those of the EC. The authors had no conflict of interest to report.

References

Albano, JDM, Ekins, RP, Maritz, G & Turner, RC (1972) A sensitive, precise radioimmunoassay of serum insulin relying on charcoal separation of bound and free hormone moieties. Acta Endocrinol 70, 487509.Google ScholarPubMed
Alderson P, Green S & Higgins JPT, eds. (2003a) Cochrane Reviewers' Handbook 4.2.2 (updated December 2003). Glossary. http://www.cochrane.org/resources/handbook/hbook.htm (Accessed January 2004)..Google Scholar
Alderson P, Green S & Higgins JPT, eds. (2003b) Cochrane Reviewers' Handbook 4.2.1 (updated December 2003). http://www.cochrane.org/resources/handbook/hbook.htm (Accessed January 2004)..Google Scholar
Anderson, GH, Catherine, NLA, Woodend, DM & Wolever, TMS (2002) Inverse association between the effect of carbohydrates on blood glucose and subsequent short-term food intake in young men. Am J Clin Nutr 76, 10231030.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Anderson, GH & Woodend, D (2003) Effect of glycemic carbohydrates on short-term satiety and food intake. Nutr Rev 61, 1726.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Bakhøj, S, Flint, A, Holst, JJ & Tetens, I (2003) Lower glucose-dependent insulinotropic polypeptide (GIP) response but similar glucagon-like peptide 1 (GLP-1), glycaemic, and insulinaemic response to ancient wheat compared to modern wheat depends on processing. Eur J Clin Nutr 57, 12541261.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Blundell, JE (1999) The control of appetite: basic concepts and practical implications. Schweiz Med Wochenschr 129, 182188.Google ScholarPubMed
Bonadonna, RC, Groop, L, Kraemer, N, Ferrannini, E, Del Prato, S & DeFronzo, RA (1990) Obesity and insulin resistance in humans: a dose-response study. Metabolism 39, 452459.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Brüning, JC, Gautam, D, Burks, DJ, Gillette, J, Schubert, M, Orban, PC, Klein, R, Krone, W, Müller-Wieland, D & Kahn, CR (2000) Role of brain insulin receptor in control of body weight and reproduction. Science 289, 21222125.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Campfield, LA & Smith, FJ (1990) Transient declines in blood glucose signal meal initiation. Int J Obes 14, Suppl 3, 1533.Google ScholarPubMed
Chapman, IM, Goble, EA, Wittert, GA, Morley, JE & Horowitz, M (1998) Effect of intravenous glucose and euglycemic insulin infusions on short-term appetite and food intake. Am J Physiol 274, R596R603.Google ScholarPubMed
De Graaf, C, Blom, WAM, Smeets, PAM, Stafleu, A & Hendriks, HFJ (2004) Biomarkers of satiation and satiety. Am J Clin Nutr 79, 946961.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Deeg, R, Kraemer, W & Ziegenhorn, J (1980) Kinetic determination of serum glucose by use of the hexokinase/glucose-6-phosphate dehydrogenase method. J Clin Chem Clin Biochem 18, 4952.Google ScholarPubMed
Deeks J, Higgins JPT & Altman D (2003) Analysing and presenting results. In: Alderson P, Green S, Higgins JPT, eds. Cochrane Reviewers' Handbook 4.2.2 (updated December 2003). Section 8. http://www.cochrane.org/resources/handbook/hbook.htm (Accessed January 2004)..Google Scholar
Egger, M & Smith, GD (1998) Meta-analysis bias in location and selection of studies. Br Med J 316, 6166.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Figlewicz, DP, Sipols, AJ, Seeley, RJ, Chavez, M, Woods, SC & Porte, D Jr (1995) Intraventricular insulin enhances the meal-suppressive efficacy of intraventricular cholecystokinin octapeptide in the baboon. Behav Neurosci 109, 567569.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Flint, A, Møller, BK, Raben, A, Pedersen, D, Tetens, I, Holst, JJ & Astrup, A (2004 a) The use of glycemic index tables to predict glycemic index of composite breakfast meals. Br J Nutr 91, 979989.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Flint, A, Raben, A, Astrup, A & Holst, JJ (1998) Glucagon-like peptide 1 promotes satiety and suppresses energy intake in humans. J Clin Invest 101, 515520.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Flint, A, Raben, A, Blundell, JE & Astrup, A (2000) Reproducibility, power and validity of visual analogue scales in assessment of appetite sensations in single test meal studies. Int J Obes 24, 3848.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Flint, A, Raben, A, Ersbøll, AK, Holst, JJ & Astrup, A (2001) The effect of physiological levels of glucagon-like peptide-1 on appetite, gastric emptying, energy and substrate metabolism in obesity. Int J Obes 25, 781792.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Flint, A, Tangaa, W, Rugbjerg, K, Raben, A, Astrup, A & Holst, JJ (2004 b) The effect of GIP on postprandial appetite and energy expenditure. Int J Obes 28, Suppl 1, 168.Google Scholar
Friedman, MI (1995) Control of energy intake by energy metabolism. Am J Clin Nutr 62, Suppl, 1096S1100S.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Gielkens, HAJ, Verkijk, M, Lam, WF, Lamers, CBHW & Masclee, AM (1998) Effects of hyperglycemia and hyperinsulinemia on satiety in humans. Metabolism 47, 321–324.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Holt, SHA, Brand Miller, JC & Petocz, P (1996) Interrelationships among postprandial satiety, glucose and insulin responses and changes in subsequent food intake. Eur J Clin Nutr 50, 788797.Google ScholarPubMed
Langhans, W (1996) Role of the liver in the metabolic control of eating: what we know—and what we do not know. Neurosci Biobehav Rev 20, 145–153.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Lavin, JH, Wittert, GA, Andrews, J, Yeap, B, Wishart, JM, Morris, HA, Morley, JE, Horowitz, M & Read, NW (1998) Interaction of insulin, glucagon-like peptide 1, gastric inhibitory polypeptide, and appetite in response to intraduodenal carbohydrate. Am J Clin Nutr 68, 591598.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Lavin, JH, Wittert, G, Sun, W-M, Horowitz, M, Morley, JE & Read, NW (1996) Appetite regulation by carbohydrate: role for blood glucose and gastrointestinal hormones. Am J Physiol 271, E209–E214.Google ScholarPubMed
Mayer, J (1955) Regulation of energy intake and the body weight: the glucostatic theory and the lipostatic hypothesis. Ann NY Acad Sci 63, 1543.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Porte, D Jr, Baskin, DG & Schwartz, MW (2002) Leptin and insulin action in the central nervous system. Nutr Rev 60, S20–S29.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Raben, A, Agerholm-Larsen, L, Flint, A, Holst, JJ & Astrup, A (2003) Meals with similar energy densities but rich in protein, fat, carbohydrate, or alcohol have different effects on energy expenditure and substrate metabolism but not on appetite and energy intake. Am J Clin Nutr 77, 91100.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Raben, A, Holst, JJ, Christensen, NJ & Astrup, A (1996) Determinants of postprandial appetite sensations: macronutrient intake and glucose metabolism. Int J Obes 20, 161169.Google ScholarPubMed
Rodin, J, Reed, D & Jamner, L (1988) Metabolic effects of fructose and glucose: implications for food intake. Am J Clin Nutr 47, 683689.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Rodin, J, Wack, J, Ferrannini, E & DeFronzo, RA (1985) Effect of insulin and glucose on feeding behaviour. Metabolism 34, 826831.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Schwartz, MW & Morton, GJ (2002) Obesity: keeping hunger at bay. Nature 418, 595–597.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Smith, FJ & Campfield, LA (1993) Meal initiation occurs after experimental induction of transient declines in blood glucose. Am J Physiol 265, R1423–R1429.Google ScholarPubMed
Speechly, DP & Buffenstein, R (2000) Appetite dysfunction in obese males: evidence for role of hyperinsulinemia in passive overconsumption with a high fat diet. Eur J Clin Nutr 54, 225233.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Thompson, SG & Higgins, JPT (2002) How should meta-regression analyses be undertaken and interpreted? Stat Med 21, 15591573.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Verdich, C, Flint, A, Gutzwiller, J-P, Näslund, E, Beglinger, C, Hellström, PM, Long, SJ, Morgan, LM, Holst, JJ & Astrup, A (2001 a) A meta-analysis of the effect of glucagon-like peptide-1 (7–36) amide on ad libitum energy intake in humans. J Clin Endocrinol Metab 86, 43825389.Google ScholarPubMed
Verdich, C, Toubro, S, Buemann, B, Madsen, JL, Holst, JJ & Astrup, A (2001 b) The role of postprandial releases of insulin and incretin hormones in meal-induced satiety—effect of obesity and weight reduction. Int J Obes 25, 1206–1214.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Woo, R, Kissileff, HR & Pi-Sunyer, FX (1984) Elevated postprandial insulin levels do not induce satiety in normal-weight humans. Am J Physiol 247, R745–R749.Google Scholar
Figure 0

Table 1 Characteristics of the seven test meal studies

Figure 1

Table 2 Study level meta-regression analyses: effects of insulin and glucose levels on satiety, hunger and ad libitum energy intake for all, normal weight, overweight male participants

Figure 2

Fig. 1 Scatter plot of relationships between postprandial insulin concentrations and ratings of satiety for normal weight (NW) and overweight (OW) participants. The associations shown in this figure are: NW (r = 0·76; P = 0·004) and OW (r = 0·21; P = 0·351). iAUCinsulin and iAUCsatiety, incremental area under the insulin and satiety response curves; filled circles represent point estimates from the individual studies (reference numbers next to the circles refer to study numbers: 1, Bakhoj et al.2003; 2, Flint et al.2004a; 3, Flint et al.1998; 4, Flint et al.2001; 5, Flint et al.2004b; 6, Raben et al.2003; 7, Verdich et al.2001b), whereas the slopes of the lines represent the regression coefficients with corresponding 95 % confidence intervals. The values on the x- and y-axes are not absolute values, but instead the values that resulted from the ln transformation of the data.

Figure 3

Fig. 2 Scatter plot of relationships between postprandial insulin concentrations and ratings of hunger for normal weight (NW) and overweight (OW) participants. The associations shown in this figure are: NW (r = 0·73; P = 0·018) and OW (r = 0·33; P = 0·012). iAUCinsulin, incremental area under the insulin response curve; iAOChunger, incremental area over the hunger response curve. The filled circles represent point estimates from the individual studies (reference numbers next to the circles refer to study numbers: 1, Bakhoj et al.2003; 2, Flint et al.2004a; 3, Flint et al.1998; 4, Flint et al.2001; 5, Flint et al.2004b; 6, Raben et al.2003; 7, Verdich et al.2001b), whereas the slopes of the lines represent the regression coefficients with corresponding 95 % confidence intervals. The values on the x- and y-axes are not absolute values, but instead the values that resulted from the ln transformation of the data.

Figure 4

Table 3 Univariate and multivariate individual participant data regression analyses: effects of insulin and glucose levels on satiety, hunger and ad libitum energy intake for all, normal weight and overweight participants