Hostname: page-component-8448b6f56d-c4f8m Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-04-16T18:32:32.493Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

Topic and focus in Old French V1 and V2 structures

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  25 January 2018

Marie Labelle*
Affiliation:
Université du Québec à Montréal
Paul Hirschbühler*
Affiliation:
Université d'Ottawa
Rights & Permissions [Opens in a new window]

Abstract

The hypothesis that Old French was not a verb-second (V2) language, but rather a Topic-initial language, is evaluated in a corpus of verb initial (V1) and V2 matrix clauses extracted from a corpus of 12th- and 13th-century texts. It is shown that the initial constituent of V2 clauses is not always a Topic; it may be part of the informational Focus, or it could be an element that is neither Topic nor Focus. In addition, in V1 and V2 sentences with subject inversion, the postverbal subject may be an informational Topic, contrary to the hypothesis that subjects must move to the preverbal position to avoid being interpreted as part of the informational Focus. Therefore, from an Information-Structure point of view, Old French is similar to a standard V2 language like German. However, certain differences between 12th- and 13th-century texts could suggest that the use of the left periphery evolved during the period considered.

Résumé

L'hypothèse que l'ancien français n'est pas une langue de type verbe-second (V2), mais plutôt une langue à topique initial est confrontée aux données d'un corpus de textes des 12e et 13e siècles, dont on a extrait les propositions matrices ayant le verbe en première (V1) ou en deuxième position (V2). Il est montré que le constituant initial des phrases V2 n'est pas nécessairement un topique ; ce peut être un élément faisant partie du focus de la phrase ou un élément qui n'est ni topique ni focus. De plus, dans les propositions avec inversion du sujet, le sujet peut être le topique discursif, contrairement à l'hypothèse selon laquelle un sujet postverbal est nécessairement interprété comme faisant partie du focus. Du point de vue de la structure informationnelle, l'ancien français n'est donc pas une langue à topique initial, mais est semblable à une langue V2 comme l'allemand. Certaines différences entre les données du 12e et du 13e siècles pourraient toutefois suggérer une évolution dans l'utilisation de la périphérie gauche de la phrase au cours de la période considérée.

Type
Articles
Copyright
© Canadian Linguistic Association/Association canadienne de linguistique 2018 

1. Introduction

A growing body of work explores the role of information structure on word order variation and change.Footnote 1 Information structure refers to the way languages structure discourse information like topic and focus. The cartographic approach to syntactic structures (e.g., Rizzi Reference Rizzi and Haegeman1997, Reference Rizzi2004; Cinque Reference Cinque2002, Reference Cinque2006; Cinque and Rizzi Reference Cinque and Rizzi2008), despite its shortcomings (van Craenenbroeck Reference van Craenenbroeck and van Craenenbroeck2009), has shown that specific positions in the clause appear to be dedicated to topic and focus elements (Benincà Reference Benincà, Cinque and Salvi1999, Reference Benincà, Zanuttini, Campos, Herberger and Portner2006; Belletti Reference Belletti and Rizzi2004; Benincà and Poletto Reference Benincà, Poletto and Rizzi2004; Benincà and Munaro Reference Benincà and Munaro2010), and a number of authors argue that changes in word order reflect changes in the way a language expresses these notions (e.g., Benincà Reference Benincà, Zanuttini, Campos, Herberger and Portner2006; Laenzlinger Reference Laenzlinger2006; Hinterhölzl Reference Hinterhölzl, Hinterhölzl and Petrova2009; Hinterhölzl and Petrova Reference Hinterhölzl and Petrova2009, Reference Hinterhölzl and Petrova2010; Kroch and Santorini Reference Kroch and Santorini2009; and the various papers in Batllori and Hernanz Reference Batllori and Hernanz2011).

The present article was spurred by Rinke and Meisel's (Reference Rinke, Meisel, Kaiser and Remberger2009) claim that Old French is not a V2 language, and more specifically, that it differs from German in the structuring of information structure. These authors develop an analysis in which Old French, contrary to German, is a topic-initial language. We undertook the task of verifying their claims by studying the informational role of preverbal elements and postverbal subjects in Old French V1 and V2 structures. Our research refutes Rinke and Meisel's analysis. We show that the distribution of Old French preverbal elements is similar to that of typical V2 languages like German. However, as discussed in section 7, we observed an evolution in the statistical distribution of preverbal constituents which suggests that the use of the left periphery changed during the period considered.

2. The Corpus

The data are extracted from 19 parsed Old French texts of the MCVF corpus (Martineau et al. Reference Martineau, Hirschbühler, Kroch and Morin2010) and the Penn supplement to this corpus (Kroch and Santorini Reference Kroch and Santorini2012) (see Appendix). In order to be able to create tables and figures, a date of composition was assigned to each text, ensuring that no two texts had the same date. These dates should, of course, be taken only as an approximation of the year the text was written.

We used Corpus Search (Randall Reference Randall2005) to automatically extract all positive declarative matrix clauses with a full subject (i.e., excluding null and clitic subjects) and to code the data. We report here our analysis of V1 and V2 clauses only. V ≥ 3 clauses were kept for later investigation.

One characteristic of the corpus used is that the 12th-century texts are all in verse except for those of Li Quatre Livre des Reis, 1170 (henceforth QLR). This is due to the lack of 12th-century prose texts, all surviving texts of some length being in verse. Conversely, all of the 13th-century texts in the corpus are in prose, with the unfortunate consequence of a near conflation of text genre and time variables.Footnote 2 In the figures to be presented, both the date and the genre are indicated.

3. Operational Definitions Relative to Information Structure

The following operational definitions of topic and focus are adopted:

Information topic: The information topic, or I-topic, also called aboutness topic (Reinhart Reference Reinhart1981), is the subject of discourse, the entity on which the comment is predicated. It corresponds to “the entity or set of entities under which the information expressed in the comment constituent should be stored in the [common ground] content” (Krifka Reference Krifka2008), the common ground being the information shared by the discourse participants. The I-topic is a discourse-old entity, already present in the discourse or accessible in the common ground, and it is therefore typically a definite constituent. This notion corresponds to Vallduví’s (Reference Vallduví1993) link.

Information focus: The notion of information focus, or I-focus, is linked to the pragmatic principle of Progression (e.g., Charolles Reference Charolles1978) stating that if a sentence is to be informative, it must contain new material. The I-focus of the sentence is brand-new information that enriches the common ground. It is both discourse-new and hearer-new. Thetic sentences are all-focus sentences, that is, sentences containing only new information. In the dialogue in (1), the answer is all-focus.

  1. (1) What happened?

    The telephone rang.

An I-focus is not necessarily a contrastive focus, which corresponds to “material which the speaker calls to the addressee's attention, thereby often evoking a contrast with other entities that might fill the same position” (Gundel and Fretheim Reference Gundel, Fretheim, Horn and Ward2004; Krifka Reference Krifka2008). A contrastive focus may be marked by position, by prosody, or by expressions like even, only, and also.

According to Gundel and Fretheim (Reference Gundel, Fretheim, Horn and Ward2004), the notions of I-topic and I-focus correspond to relationally given/new information respectively, and are equivalent to the notions of theme/rheme, or topic/comment. This is largely correct, with some caveats: the comment may contain background, discourse-old, information (shared information that is already in the common ground) in addition to new information (the I-focus per se). For example, in (2), taken from Krifka (Reference Krifka2008: 42, ex. 41), married her in (2b) is background information within the comment (Büring Reference Büring, Ramchand and Reiss2007: 5; Vallduví’s Reference Vallduví1993 tail).

  1. (2) a.  When did [Aristotle Onassis]I-topic marry Jacqueline Kennedy?

    b.  [He]I-topic [married her [in 1968]I-focus]]Comment

4. Old French Information Structure and the Verb Second (V2) Problem

Since the end of the 19th century, it has been observed that the Old French finite verb tends to be found in the second position in main clauses: when an element precedes the verb, the subject is postverbal (Thurneysen Reference Thurneysen1892, Foulet Reference Foulet1928, Skårup Reference Skårup1975). Accordingly, many authors analyze the Old French clause with the V2 structure typical of Germanic languages, as shown in (4) (e.g., Adams Reference Adams1987, Reference Adams1988; Roberts Reference Roberts1993; Platzack Reference Platzack, Battye and Roberts1995; Vance Reference Vance, Battye and Roberts1995). In the Government and Binding framework, the verb was assumed to occupy C and the preverbal constituent [Spec,CP]. With the development of an articulated left periphery (Rizzi Reference Rizzi and Haegeman1997), the verb tends to be analyzed as filling Fin, with the preverbal constituent occupying [Spec,FinP] or a higher position (see Holmberg Reference Holmberg, Kiss and Alexiadou2015 for a review; see also Ledgeway Reference Ledgeway2008, Labelle and Hirschbühler Reference Labelle, Hirschbühler, Mathieu and Truswell2017, Zaring Reference Zaringthis issue). [Spec,CP] (or [Spec,FinP]) corresponds to Skårup's (Reference Skårup1975) place du fondement, or the Vorfeld in German.

  1. (3) messe  e  matines  ad  li  reis  escultet

    mass  and  matins  has  the  king  heard

    ‘The king has attended mass and matins.’ (1100, Roland 11;139)Footnote 3

  2. (4) [CP/FinP messe e matines ad [TP li reis ad escultet messe e matines]

This V2 analysis has been challenged by a number of authors, in part because V1 and V3 constructions are more frequent in Old French than in either modern German (Kaiser Reference Kaiser2000, Reference Kaiser2002; Ferraresi and Goldbach Reference Ferraresi and Goldbach2002; Rinke and Meisel Reference Rinke, Meisel, Kaiser and Remberger2009; Kaiser and Zimmermann Reference Kaiser, Zimmermann, Rinke and Kupisch2011) or Middle High German (1050–1350) (Elsig Reference Elsig2009). Rinke and Meisel (Reference Rinke, Meisel, Kaiser and Remberger2009), for instance, argue, on the basis of a study of two early 13th century texts (Villehardouin and Les 7 sages de Rome), that Old French is not a V2 language but a topic-initial language. They derive Old French matrix clauses within TP, with [Spec,TP] a topic position. The main claim of Rinke and Meisel is that the preverbal position hosts the aboutness topic – I-topic in our terms – and the verb phrase contains the I-focus portion of the clause. Old French is said to differ from contemporary German, where the preverbal position is not restricted to topics but may also host an I-focus or an adverb that is neither a topic nor (part of the) I-focus. If their analysis proves correct, Old French would be similar to Old High German, given Hinterhölzl and Petrova's (Reference Hinterhölzl and Petrova2010, Reference Hinterhölzl, Petrova, Chiarcos, Claus and Grabski2011) claim that the Old High German verb served to separate the I-topic from the comment.

For Rinke and Meisel (Reference Rinke, Meisel, Kaiser and Remberger2009: 109), subjects move to the preverbal position to avoid being interpreted as I-focus: “An incompatibility of the post-verbal subject with an interpretation as information focus or as part of a thetic sentence would cause the subject to move to the pre-verbal position.” This claim would be disproved if the I-topic of the clause may appear postverbally; this is the subject of sections 5 and 6, where postverbal I-topics are shown to be frequent in the corpus.Footnote 4

Scene-setting elements, namely, elements specifying the time and location of the situation under which the proposition is evaluated, are often considered to be types of topics (Speyer Reference Speyer, Shaer, Frey and Maienborn2004: 534–535). Their discourse role, however, differs from that of I-topics, and the two types of elements may co-occur in a clause (Nikolaeva Reference Nikolaeva2001; Andréasson Reference Andréasson, Butt and King2007), as (5) illustrates.

  1. (5) John had a busy day. At twelve[Scene-setting], he[I-topic] was eating pizza with his brother…

In the following quote, Rinke and Meisel suggest that, in Old French, scene-setting adverbials fill the preverbal position only when no I-topic is overtly realized:

In addition, sentence-initial (scene setting) adverbial phrases may serve a special discourse function. According to Reinhart (Reference Reinhart1981), they can establish discourse cohesion by linking the sentence to the previous discourse. Preverbal adverbial phrases thus fulfill the same discourse function as preverbal topics. These adverbial expressions are therefore frequently found in sentences in which no topic is present that would normally assume the discourse-linking function.

(Rinke and Meisel Reference Rinke, Meisel, Kaiser and Remberger2009: 115)

If this is correct, Old French differs significantly from German. In German, a sentence may contain both a scene-setting element and an I-topic, and Speyer (Reference Speyer, Shaer, Frey and Maienborn2004, Reference Speyer, Benz and Kühnlein2008, Reference Speyer, Tanskanen, Helasvuo, Johansson and Raitaniemi2010) established that, in a sentence where both are present, the scene-setting element preferentially occupies the Vorfeld, while the I-topic remains postverbal. Consequently, in section 6.2, we study the informational role of postverbal subjects when a scene-setting element is preverbal. We will see that there are many V2 sentences with a preverbal scene-setting element and a postverbal I-topic subject.

Both in Old French and in German, I-topics are often preverbal. This is expected because they are cohesive devices linking with the previous discourse. According to Speyer's (Reference Speyer, Shaer, Frey and Maienborn2004, Reference Speyer, Benz and Kühnlein2008, Reference Speyer, Tanskanen, Helasvuo, Johansson and Raitaniemi2010) research on German, about 82% of the referential elements filling the Vorfeld in written German are elements establishing a link with the preceding discourse (scene-setting, I-topics, and elements anaphoric with a discourse-old set). The remaining cases include brand-new elements (i.e., I-focus in our terms). Hence, placing the I-focus in clause-initial position is not the dominant option in German, although it is grammatical. We will see that I-focus elements may fill the preverbal position in Old French as well. This evidence goes against the hypothesis that Old French is a topic-initial language.

Finally, if the Vorfeld in Germanic V2 languages is the host of various types of informational elements, including those pertaining to the I-Focus of the clause, but the preverbal field in Old French is a Topic position (holding I-Topics and scene-setting elements), we would expect the statistical distribution of preverbal elements to differ when the two are compared. The comparisons discussed in Section 7 do not bear out this prediction.

Our conclusion will be that, from the informational point of view, Old French does not appear to differ markedly from standard Germanic V2 languages.

5. Verb-initial (V1) Clauses

We begin with V1 clauses. If the Old French postverbal field is interpreted as the I-focus of the clause, V1 clauses should be thetic (all-focus). In other words, if subjects must move to the preverbal position in order to avoid being interpreted as part of the I-focus, the postverbal subjects of V1 clauses should be construed as belonging to the I-focus portion of the clause. To verify this prediction, we first considered sentences other than those where the verb introduces direct discourse (section 5.1), and then turned our attention to verbs of saying (section 5.2).

5.1 Verbs other than those introducing direct discourse

We extracted the strict V1 clauses of the corpus (i.e., those not introduced by a coordinator) having a full subject and a verb distinct from those introducing direct discourse. Taking the context into account, we hand-coded the subjects as being I-topic, I-focus, or UnclearFootnote 5. Table 1 shows that, while there is a dominance of I-focus subjects, over a third of the postverbal subjects were coded as I-topic. Examples (6) and (7) illustrate postverbal subjects coded as I-topics.

  1. (6) (The Philistines ask a question of the Jews and the answer doesn't please them.)

    Curecerent  s’  en  les  princes  des  Philistiens

    got-angry  refl  gen  the  princes  of.the  Philistines

    ‘The princes of the Philistians got angry at this.’ (1170; QLR, 1–2; 1332)

  2. (7) (Brendan is the main protagonist. He and his followers land, and hope to relax after a long journey. However, a tempest approaches.)

    Cunuit  Brandans  a  l’  air  pluius/Que  li  tens ert mult annüus.

    knew  Brendan  from  the  air  rainy/that  the  weather was very worrisome

    ‘Brendan knew from the wet wind that the weather was worrisome’ (1120; Brendan, 56; 675)

Table 1: Informational role of postverbal subjects in V1 declaratives.

The results in Table 1 counter Rinke and Meisel's claim that, in Old French, postverbal subjects are internal to the verb phrase and interpreted as part of the I-focus. Rather, they confirm Rouveret's (Reference Rouveret2004: 196) comment that “It does not seem that the postverbal position in [Old French] V1 sentences is pragmatically specialized.”

Figure 1 illustrates the evolution of V1 declaratives over time. In Roland (1100), the vast majority of postverbal subjects were coded as being I-focus, whereas in Brendan (1120) and the QLR (1170), they were more often I-topics. The V1 construction dropped out of the language at the beginning of the 13th century, when the language became more strictly V2. This is why there are few examples of V1 declaratives after 1170; three texts dated between 1205 and 1267 contain no V1 declaratives. After 1267, V1 declaratives reappear. They contain no clear I-topic subject, but there are only seven examples. Thus, at least for the period before the 13th century, when V1 declaratives were productively used, I-topic subjects did not need to move to the left of the verb; they could remain postverbal in V1 declaratives.

Figure 1: V1 declaratives–Informational nature of subjects: Number of postverbal subjects that are I-topic, I-focus or Unclear. The numbers at the bottom indicate the total number of relevant examples.

The postverbal position of I-topic subjects in V1 sentences is not compatible with the hypothesis that Old French sentences are dominated by TP, with the verb under T and the postverbal field expressing the comment and containing the I-focus portion of the clause. The facts are explained if we assume, following Labelle and Hirschbühler (Reference Labelle, Hirschbuhler, Batllori, Hernanz, Picallo and Roca2005), that, in V1 constructions, the emphasis is on the verb, which moves to a discourse-related position within the left periphery. Labelle and Hirschbühler propose that the verb in V1 clauses first moves to Fin, then, in a second step, to a higher discourse-related head, which they label ‘Z’, and which could correspond to focus in the articulated structure of the left periphery proposed by Benincà and Poletto (Reference Benincà, Poletto and Rizzi2004), shown in (8).

  1. (8) [ HT [ Scene setting [ Force [ topic [ focus [ WH [ Fin ]]]]]]]

Under Labelle and Hirschbühler's analysis, postverbal subjects could occupy either [Spec,vP] or [Spec,TP], and one could explore the hypothesis that I-focus subjects occupy [Spec,vP] while I-topic subjects occupy [Spec,TP], as proposed by Rinke and Meisel.

  1. (9) a.  [FocP Cunuit [FinP cunuit [TP cunuit [vP Brandans cunuit …] (=(6))

    b.  [FocP Cunuit [FinP cunuit [TP Brandans cunuit [vP Brandans cunuit …]

But to account for the fact that postverbal subjects could be I-topics, we need to assume a derivation of V1 clauses that involves the left periphery in a manner similar to what happens in V2 sentences. Such a structure is compatible with the V2 nature of Old French if we assume, with Ledgeway (Reference Ledgeway2008: 439), that V2 is not a surface linear constraint, but a syntactic requirement that the verb raise to the C-domain in general, though not necessarily, with fronting of a pragmatically salient constituent to its left.

5.2 Verbs introducing direct discourse

Let us now turn to verbs introducing direct discourse. As schematized in (10)–(12), with these verbs, V1 declaratives (10) alternate with V2 declaratives, some of the SV(X) type (11), and some of the XVS type (12), where the preverbal element is either a cataphoric object or an adverb.

  1. (10) Dit Roland: “ … ”     = VS

    ‘Says Roland: ….’

  2. (11) Roland dit: “ … ”      = SV

    ‘Roland says: ….’

  3. (12) a. Ço dit Roland: “ … ”  = Cataphoric object VS

    ‘This says Roland: ….’

  4. b. Donc dit Roland: “ … ”  = Adverb VS

    ‘Thus says Roland: ….’

We did not consider verbs of saying in parentheticals, nor cases where the verb of saying follows the direct discourse, because there is no word order variation in these cases: the subject is always postverbal.

As far as we could see, the informational role of the subject is the same in examples (10)–(12). If this is correct, the preverbal or postverbal position of the subject is not dictated exclusively by its informational role. The following analysis attempts to verify this by focusing on the following question: with the disappearance of VS declaratives at the beginning of the 13th century, which construction replaces VS declaratives? If the postverbal position of the subject in VS declaratives was dictated by Information Structure, VS sentences are expected to be replaced by XVS sentences, to keep the subject postverbal. However, Figure 2 shows that, when the rate of VS sentences dropped dramatically (between 1177 and 1225), the proportion of SV(X) sentences increased, but not that of XVS sentences.

Figure 2: Verbs introducing direct discourse: Rates of SVX sentences as compared to VS and XVS sentences. The numbers at the bottom correspond to the number of relevant examples.

The fact that SV sentences are replacing VS sentences casts serious doubts on the idea that the subject's informational role dictates its position. Moreover, there is no support for the hypothesis that I-topic subjects could not remain postverbal. The vast majority of the subjects are definite and discourse-old both in SV and in VS sentences. In 99% (573/580) of the examples containing a verb of saying, the subject is definite.Footnote 6 As definite subjects correspond to known information, they tend to represent the I-topic, particularly in the discourse context of verbs of saying.

Figure 2 suggests that VS order in declaratives resulted from a desire to place the verb first, and not from the need to leave the subject in a postverbal I-focus position. When speakers started to avoid placing emphasis on the verb in declaratives, a V2 construction was used instead, with the verb remaining under Fin and some constituent filling [Spec,FinP]. The fact that I-topic subjects were largely preferred over either objects or adverbs to fill the preverbal position is coherent with the distribution of preverbal elements in V2 sentences (see section 7, table 4).

To summarize this section, the hypothesis that subjects need to move to the preverbal position to avoid being interpreted as I-focus is refuted. Postverbal subjects in V1 clauses are often the I-topic of the clause. It could very well be that subjects move to [Spec,TP] in order to be interpreted as I-topics. However, we must conclude that the Old French clause structure projects higher up than [Spec,TP] and that the Old French verb moves to the left periphery in V1 main clauses. This movement to the left periphery is in line with the standard V2 analysis of Old French.

6. V2 Clauses

Let us now turn to V2 clauses. Two questions are addressed in this section: First, what is the informational role of the postverbal subjects in V2 clauses? Second, what is the informational role of the preverbal constituents?

6.1 Subjects

Because definite subjects tend to be I-topics, whereas indefinite subjects make bad I-topics and are often found in thetic sentences, we asked ourselves whether, in V2 sentences, there is a marked tendency to find definite subjects preverbally and indefinite subjects postverbally. To answer this question, we automatically coded the nature of subjects in the 4397 non-coordinated V2 sentences. Subjects coded as “definite” include those introduced by a definite, possessive or demonstrative determiner, DPs headed by a demonstrative or other referential pronoun, and proper nouns; “indefinite” subjects include DPs headed by a partitive or indefinite determiner. Bare NPs and DPs headed by a quantifier (like tous ‘all’, plusieurs ‘many’), which could be definite or indefinite, were counted separately. Figures 3a and 3b show that subjects are overwhelmingly definite, both preverbally and postverbally. Given that definite subjects mostly introduce known or retrievable information, and rarely new information, the large number of postverbal definite subjects casts serious doubt on the idea that postverbal subjects could not be I-topics.

Figure 3a: Preverbal subject types: Rates of subject types in preverbal position.

Figure 3b: Postverbal subject types: Rates of subject types in postverbal position.

Preverbal definite subjects are expected to be I-topics. Because this would be compatible with Rinke and Meisel's claims, we did not study them further, but rather focused our attention on the 192 indefinite subjects. We found a small tendency to place indefinite subjects postverbally, but clearly no exclusion of indefinite subjects from the preverbal position: 44.7% of indefinite subjects are preverbal in verse texts (12th century) and 43% in prose texts (1170 and 13th century). Examples (13) and (14) show that these indefinite preverbal subjects may be the I-focus of the clause; the contexts should make clear that the indefinite subjects carry brand new information. We conclude from this that the preverbal position may host informationally new, I-focus, elements.Footnote 7, Footnote 8

  1. (13) (Eliduc is looking for a burial place for his love. He remembers that near his domain there is a large forest. A holy hermit lived there, and there was a chapel there. He had talked to him many times. He will bring his love to him, for him to bury her in his chapel.)

    Un  seinz  hermites  i  maneit  et  une  chapele i  aveit

    A  holy  hermit  loc  lived  and  a  chapel  loc  had

    ‘A holy hermit lived there and there was a chapel there.’

    (1180; Marie de France, Eliduc 182;3715)

  2. (14) (While Eliduc's wife is crying next to a dead girl, a weasel comes running out from under the altar and passes over the corpse. The valet kills it and throws its corpse in a corner.)

    Une  musteile  vint  curant,  desuz  l’  auter  esteit  eissue

    A  weasel  came  running,  under  the  altar  was  come.out

    ‘A weasel came running out from under the altar.’

    (1180; Marie de France, Eliduc 187;3815)

Considering only texts containing more than ten indefinite subjects, we observe a small increase in the tendency to place indefinite subjects in postverbal position (Figure 4), but the percentage of preverbal indefinite subjects at the end of the period is still 35%. Thus, there does not seem to be any constraint against preverbal I-focus subjects during the period under investigation.

Figure 4: Postverbal indefinite subjects over total indefinite subjects (in text with more than 10 indefinite subjects): Rates of postverbal indefinite subjects. The numbers at the bottom correspond to the total number of relevant examples.

To summarize this subsection, the distribution of definite vs indefinite subjects in V2 clauses does not support the hypothesis that subjects move to the preverbal position to avoid being interpreted as I-focus. This conclusion will be supported in Section 6.2.3, which presents a quantitative analysis of the informational role of postverbal subjects in sentences containing a preverbal adverbial.

6.2 Preverbal nonsubject constituents

Rinke and Meisel (Reference Rinke, Meisel, Kaiser and Remberger2009) claim that, while in German the preverbal constituent may be a topic, an I-focus, or an element that is neither topic nor focus, the preverbal constituent in Old French is always a topic (an I-topic, or an adverb with topical properties). In this section, we consider XVS sentences with the aim of determining whether the preverbal position is a topic position, and whether postverbal subjects are interpreted as being part of the I-focus portion of the clause.

Since clause-initial contrastive elements could result from movement to a dedicated contrastive focus projection within the left periphery (Rizzi's Reference Rizzi and Haegeman1997 FocP), we will set aside examples like (15) containing an overtly contrastive preverbal constituent.

  1. (15) Meïsmes  a  l' empereour  sont  les  lermes  venues  aus  yex,

    even  to  the  emperor  are  the  tears  come-ptcp  to-the  eyes

    ‘Even to the emperor, the tears came to the eyes.’ (1267; Cassidorus, 664;4447)

6.2.1 Preverbal objects.

The OVS word order in 13th century texts has been studied by Rickard (Reference Rickard1962), who shows that, in addition to establishing a link with the previous discourse, the preverbal position may also be a prominence position serving to convey a brand-new and affectively important idea. Marchello-Nizia (Reference Marchello-Nizia1995, Reference Marchello-Nizia1999), who studied the role of preverbal direct objects in two texts, Roland (1100) and La Queste del Saint Graal (1225), noticed a change in the informational role of preverbal objects. In Roland, there is no restriction on the informational role of preverbal objects, but they tend to be, in her terms, more often rhematic (I-focus) than thematic (I-topic) (Marchello-Nizia Reference Marchello-Nizia1995: 99–100). By contrast, the OV(S) order is more restricted in the Queste, where, according to Marchello-Nizia, it serves mainly to thematize the object. When the object is rhematic, the OVS order is marked and is found mainly in idioms of type mander saluz ‘send greetings’, as in (16), or when O is either strongly focused or cataphorically linked to the discourse.Footnote 9

  1. (16) Saluz  vos  mande  li  bons  chevaliers

    Greetings  you  send  the  good  knight

    ‘The good knight sends you his greetings’   (Queste, p. 30, cited in Marchello-Nizia Reference Marchello-Nizia1999: 44)

Our corpus study confirms the change in the informational role of preverbal objects. Taking context into account, we coded all preverbal objects in V2 sentences as I-topic, I-focus, or unclear. As shown in Figure 5, a preverbal object tended to be the I-focus of the clause before 1205, but the I-topic of the clause, after 1225.

Figure 5: Informational status of preverbal objects: Rates of preverbal objects that are I-focus, I-topic, or Unclear. The numbers at the bottom correspond to the total number of relevant examples.

The examples in (17)–(20) contain preverbal objects analyzed as part of the I-focus:

  1. (17) (Marsile comes through a valley with the great army he had assembled)

    .XX.  escheles  ad  li  reis  anumbrees.

    twenty  columns  has  the  king  counted

    ‘The King has organized them in twenty divisions.’Footnote 10 (1100; Roland, 112;1459)

  2. (18) (After the storm, a large quantity of birds assembled on the pine tree, they all sang with perfect harmony.)

    et  divers  chanz  chantoit  chascuns;

    and  various  songs  sang  each.one’

    ‘and they all sang a different song’ (1177; Yvain, 15;453)

  3. (19) (Lunete asks the lady to swear that she will help the knight)

    La  main  destre  leva  adonques  la  dame,

    The  hand  right  raised  then  the  lady

    ‘Then the lady raised the right hand’ (1177; Yvain, 202;7065)

  4. (20) (While the king fortified Sayete, merchants arrived and told us that the king of the Tartars had taken the city of Baudas.)

    La  maniere  comment  il  pristrent  la  cité  de  Baudas et  le  calife  nous  conterent

    The  manner  how  they  took.pl  the  city  of  Baudas and  the  caliph  us  told

    les  marcheans;

    the  merchants

    ‘The merchants told us how they took the city of Baudas and the caliph’

    (1309; de Joinville 289; 3370.Footnote 11)

It can be seen that the postverbal definite subjects are not part of the comment or I-focus of the clause, but constitute the old information of the clause and its I-topic (the entity under which the comment must be stored in the common ground).

A change in the informational ordering of constituents seems to have happened at the beginning of the 13th century. This change led to a tendency to leave I-focus objects in postverbal position. This trend could suggest that the language changed from having a preverbal position unrestricted with respect to information role, towards a situation where the preverbal position tends to be restricted to I-topics. However, since the OVS order with a preverbal I-focus object counts for twenty to forty percent of preverbal objects until the end of the period, one cannot say that the language becomes a topic-initial language at the beginning of the 13th century. The detailed analysis of the OV order carried out by de Andrade (this issue) confirms this finding. De Andrade shows, among other things, that it is only in the 15th century (that is, in Middle French) that informational focus objects become rare in preverbal position.Footnote 12 We will come back to the change that affected preverbal objects in section 7 below.

We conclude that, in Old French, the preverbal position may be occupied by an I-focus, in which case a postverbal subject may be the I-topic.

6.2.2 Preverbal predicates and quantifiers

In Old French, the preverbal position may be filled by constituents that are not good candidates for topic-hood. These include quantifiers, non-finite verbs, adjectives and other predicates.

In (21), the preverbal predicate is known information. It establishes a link with the previous chapter and introduces a contrast with the second clause. It is neither the I-topic nor the I-focus of the discourse.

  1. (21) (Beginning of the section on rebellion, after a section on the sin of grumbling [murmuring against an authority].)

    Male  chose  est  murmure,  mes  trop  vaut  pis  rebellion.

    bad  thing  is  grumble  but  too.much  be.worth  worse  rebellion

    ‘Grumbling is a bad thing, but rebellion is much worse.’ (1279; Somme, 1,64; 1758)

However, in general, preverbal predicates and quantifiers are interpreted as part of the I-focus. When that is the case, subject may appear postverbally and be construed as I-topics. Examples (22)–(24) illustrate this situation.

  1. (22) (Lanval leaves his hosts, rides towards the town. He often looks behind him.)

    Mut  est  Lanval  en  grant  esfrei!

    much  is  Lanval  in  great  fear

    ‘Lanval is greatly afraid.’ (1180; Marie de France, Lanval, 196; 78.1592)

  2. (23) (Count Roland says to Marsile “…today you'll learn my sword's name”. He goes to strike him.)

    Trenchet  li  ad  li  quens  le  destre  poign

    cut-ptcp  him  has  the  count  the  right  hand

    ‘The count has cut his right hand.’ (1100; Roland, 142; 1926)

  3. (24) (The girl, hiding in a shelter, disinherited and disconsolate, becomes full of joy upon hearing that the Lion Knight is coming (to defend her). She thinks that, now, her sister will give her part of her inheritance.)

    Malade  ot  geü  longuemant  la  pucele

    sick  has  laid  a.long.time  the  girl

    ‘The girl had been sick for a long time.’ (1177; Yvain, 177; 6235)

Preverbal predicates and quantifiers were always rare in preverbal position compared to adverbials and PPs. Table 2 shows that their percentage is higher in verse texts (12th century) than in prose texts (mainly 13th century). It could be that versification constraints favored the use of an atypical word order. It might also be that after the 12th century, speakers were less likely to place I-focus elements in preverbal position, echoing what was found with objects.

Table 2: Distribution of adjectives, predicative NPs and non finite verbs in preverbal position.

6.2.3 Adverbials

We coded the informational status of postverbal subjects in V2 sentences introduced by an adverbial, or a scene-setting temporal DP like le lendemain ‘the next day’. There were about 9% of cases that we were unable to code (86 cases out of 975); these were removed from the analysis. The majority of the postverbal subjects (71%) were coded as I-topic. Only 29% of the postverbal subjects were coded as new information, that is, as I-focus. Figure 6, illustrating only the texts for which we had at least nine relevant examples, demonstrates that no observable change is apparent within the period considered.

Figure 6: Information function of postverbal subjets in Adv-V-S clauses: Percentage of postverbal subjects that are coded as I-focus or I-topic in texts containing more than nine relevant examples.

In the majority of cases, the preverbal adverb is a short temporal adverbial like lors, puis, après, donc, adont, or the particle si. In these cases, the preverbal element is neither a topic nor an I-focus, but a cohesive element, linking to the previous discourse. When such an element is preverbal, the I-topic appears postverbally. The postverbal placement of I-topics is expected in a V2 system, and it is observed in German.

  1. (25) (Ydoines asks Helcanor if he prayed God to give him his hands back; Helcanor answers “Yes I did.”)

    Dont  sot  Ydoines  vraiement  que  Diex  l’  amoit,

    Then  knew  Ydoine  truly  that  God  her  loved

    ‘Then Ydoine truly knew that God loved her.’ (1267; Cassidorus, 351; 3031)

  2. (26) (Perceval wants to leave immediately, but his aunt refuses to give him leave, saying that he should stay overnight and leave after hearing mass in the morning. Perceval stays. The servants install the table. Perceval and his aunt eat the meal prepared for them.)

    Si  demora  laienz  Perceval  avec  s’  antain.

    thus  stayed  therein  Perceval  with  his  aunt

    ‘Thus, Perceval stayed there with his aunt.’ (1225; Queste, 107; 2806)

A second category of preverbial adverbials includes locative or temporal scene-setting elements. Here again, the postverbal subject is usually interpreted as the I-topic of the clause. The examples in (27) and (28) illustrate this type of situation. These examples show that in Old French, scene-setting adverbs do not necessarily occupy the preverbal position when no I-topic is available to fill it. On the contrary, Old French appears to function like German, where, according to Speyer (Reference Speyer, Benz and Kühnlein2008), when a clause contains both a scene-setting element and an I-topic, the scene-setting element has priority for filling the Vorfeld.

  1. (27) (Cassidorus takes his spear and runs towards Lapsus to fight.)

    La  fist  Cassidorus  plus   d’  armes

    There  made  Cassidorus  more  (feats) of  arms

    que  onques  Achilles  ne   fist  devant  Troies.

    than  ever  Achilles  neg  made  in.front.of  Troy

    ‘There, Cassidorus made more feats of arms than Achilles ever made in front of Troy.’

    (1267; Cassidorus, 26; 627)

  2. (28) (During Lent, the king had his boats prepared to go back to France. On the eve of Saint Mark's day, after Easter, the king and the queen prayed on their boats; they set sail with fair winds.)

    Le  jour  de  la  saint  Marc  me  dit  le  roy que  a  celi  jour  il  avoit  esté  né;

    The  day  of  the  Saint  Marc  me  told  the  king  that  at  that  day  he  had  been  born

    ‘On Saint Marc's day, the king told me that he was born that day;’

    (1309; de Joinville, 305; 3574)

Our results can be summarized as follows. In the preverbal position of V2 sentences, we observed not only elements treated as topics by Rinke and Meisel (I-topics and scene-setting adverbials), but also elements that were the I-focus of the clause as well as adverbs that are neither topics nor focus. In postverbal position, we counted a large number of definite subjects interpreted as the I-topic of their clause. We conclude that Old French was not a topic-initial language and that subjects could remain postverbal without being interpreted as part of the I-focus. Therefore, from the informational point of view, Old French resembles German. The standard analysis whereby the verb moves to the left periphery in main clauses accounts adequately, not only for the V2 examples, but also for the V1 examples.

7. Comparison with Germanic Languages

In this section, we compare the distribution of preverbal constituents in Old French and in Germanic languages. If the information role of the preverbal constituent in Old French is similar to what is observed in Germanic V2 languages, we expect the distributions to be similar. If, however, Old French is not V2 but is a topic-initial language, we expect this fact to be reflected in the distribution. We base our comparison on the study by Bohnacker and Rosén (Reference Bohnacker, Rosén, Anderssen and Westergaard2007).

Bohnacker and Rosén (Reference Bohnacker, Rosén, Anderssen and Westergaard2007) compare the prefields in German and Swedish, and they arrive at the figures in Table 3 (which combines their Tables 1–3, pages 34 and 36). Table 3 can be compared with our results for Old French in Table 4. The percentages of the various types of preverbal constituents are of the same order of magnitude in Old French and in the two Germanic languages. This comparison reinforces our claim that during the 12th and 13th centuries, Old French was not a topic-initial language, but, rather, a V2 language similar in the relevant respects to German and Swedish.

Table 3: Constituents in the prefield in German and Swedish (from Bohnacker and Rosén Reference Bohnacker, Rosén, Anderssen and Westergaard2007).

Table 4: Constituents in the prefield in Old French.

Commenting on the figures in Table 3, Bohnacker and Rosén explain the difference in the percentages of fronted objects in German (about 7%) as compared to Swedish (about 3%) as follows: both Swedish and German tend to start declaratives with a subject coinciding with the theme and topic, and to place the theme before the rheme. However, Swedish has a stronger tendency to place the rheme after the verb, to start with a light element or one of low informational value (e.g., an expletive, det, så), and to use few fronted objects. Although the constructions with a heavy rhematic (i.e., I-focus) object found in German are considered grammatical in Swedish, they are dispreferred. Swedish typically fronts objects that are themes (i.e., I-topics).

Turning to Old French, Table 4 shows that the percentage of preverbal objects in verse texts (12th century) is similar to what is found in German, while the corresponding percentage in prose texts (mainly13th century) is similar to what is found in Swedish. If we add to this observation the decrease of I-focus preverbal objects in prose texts (section 6.2.1), the lower percentage of preverbal predicates and non-finite verbs in prose texts (section 6.2.2), and the small increase in the proportion of postverbal indefinite subjects (section 6.1), we are tempted to conclude (if the differences are not exclusively due to text genre) that the use of the prefield during the period considered evolved from one similar to that of contemporary German to one similar to that of contemporary Swedish. These changes might indicate a change towards a preference for keeping I-focus constituents within the VP. It is clear, nonetheless, that I-topics are not restricted to the preverbal position during the period considered, contrary to what was argued by Rinke and Meisel (Reference Rinke, Meisel, Kaiser and Remberger2009). Moreover, as German and Swedish are both V2 languages, the shift observed in Old French cannot be taken as an argument against the analysis of Old French as a V2 language.

8. Conclusion

We have argued that, from an information-structure viewpoint, Old French was essentially a V2 language of the Germanic type until the end of the 13th century. Just as in German, the preverbal position of an Old French sentence could be filled by an I-focus, by an I-topic, by a scene-setting topic, or by an adverb that was neither a topic nor part of the I-focus. Moreover, postverbal subjects were frequently interpreted as I-topics; therefore, subjects did not need to move to the preverbal position to avoid being interpreted as part of the I-focus. This is what is expected of a language in which the verb moves to a position within the left periphery (FinP or higher) with some constituent merged to its left in V2 sentences. If the verb occupies a position within the left periphery, postverbal subjects in SpecTP may be construed as I-topics.

Our research allowed us to advance in the description of the evolution of Old French, in that we noticed a slight change in the use of the left periphery. The 12th-century grammar is statistically closer to that of German, while the 13th-century grammar is similar to that of Swedish. This shift appears to reflect a change in progress towards a grammar where new information preferentially occurs in postverbal position. Specifically, noncontrastive elements carrying the I-focus of the clause are less frequently preverbal at the end of the period, compared to what was found at the beginning. If the differences are not exclusively due to text genre, they reflect one of the first steps towards the loss of the V2 grammar and the emergence of an SVO grammar, as argued by Marchello-Nizia (Reference Marchello-Nizia1995). It would be interesting to study Middle French V2 sentences to better understand this evolution.

List of Old French texts studied, by date

Footnotes

A previous version of this paper was presented at the 13th Diachronic Generative Syntax conference in Philadelphia, June 2–5, 2011. We thank the audience for their helpful comments.

1 The following abbreviations are used: gen: genitive clitic; loc: locative clitic; MCVF: Modéliser le changement: les Voies du français (corpus); neg: negation; pl: plural; ptcp: participle; OVS: object-verb-subject; refl: reflexive clitic; SV: subject-verb; V1: verb-initial; V2: verb-second; VS: verb-subject.

2 Some verse texts from later than the 12th century have subsequently been added to the corpus (after the research reported here was completed) in order to address this concern.

3 References to Old French examples from the MCVF corpus contain first the assigned date of the text, then the abbreviated name of the text, followed by a page, verse, or section number (according to the text), and the phrase structure number in the corpus.

4 In the present article, as in Rinke and Meisel (Reference Rinke, Meisel, Kaiser and Remberger2009), postverbal subjects include subjects described as of the Germanic as well as of the Romance inversion type.

5 The Unclear category functions as an elsewhere condition, and includes unclear cases as well as elements constituting background information. See Cook and Bildhauer (Reference Cook, Bildhauer, Dipper and Zinsmeister2011) for a discussion of the difficulties facing the coding of information structure.

6 This figure includes subjects headed by a definite, possessive or demonstrative determiner or pronoun, proper nouns, and 33 bare NPs hand-coded as definite given the context (all the bare NPs from Roland 1100).

7 Under the present definitions, an indefinite carrying the clause's new information is coded as I-focus. In the first clause of (13), for instance, un seinz hermites is the brand-new information predicated of the discourse-old forest represented by the clitic i; it is therefore the I-focus of the clause. Once introduced in the discourse, the hermit becomes discourse-old and it could become the aboutness topic of the following discourse. See Frascarelli and Hinterhölzl (Reference Frascarelli, Hinterhölzl, Schwabe and Winkler2007) for the notion of ‘new aboutness topic’.

8 Concerning example (13) and others to follow, note that at no time during the history of French did an object clitic pronoun count for determining the V2 position of the verb (Labelle and Hirschbühler Reference Labelle, Hirschbuhler, Batllori, Hernanz, Picallo and Roca2005).

9 Zaring (Reference Zaring2010) studied the OV order with (non-initial) non-finite verbs, in two different texts (Le Roman de Perceval ou le Conte du Graal by Chrétien de Troyes (c. 1185) and La Conqueste de Constantinople by Geoffroy de Villehardouin (c. 1205)), and found the same evolution. It thus seems that what changes is not the nature of the clause-initial position, but more generally what allows an object to precede the verb.

10 Translation from Brault (Reference Brault1978:91); Moignet (Reference Moignet1985) translates the sentence as The King counted 20 divisions.

11 This example is also discussed in Rickard (Reference Rickard1962: 29).

12 See Ledgeway (Reference Ledgeway2008: 441) for a similar observation regarding informationally new (rhematic, I-focus) preverbal objects in Old Neapolitan and in early Romance varieties in general.

References

Adams, Marianne P. 1987. From Old French to the theory of pro-drop. Natural Language and Linguistic Theory 5(1): 132.Google Scholar
Adams, Marianne P. 1988. V2 effects in Old and Middle French. Revue québécoise de linguistique théorique et appliquée 7(3): 1339.Google Scholar
de Andrade, Aroldo. This issue. Aboutness topics in Old and Middle French: A corpus-based study on the fate of V2.Google Scholar
Andréasson, Maia. 2007. The architecture of I-structure. In Proceedings of LFG07, ed. Butt, Miriam and King, Tracy Holloway, 2643. Stanford: CSLI Publications. <http://csli-publications.stanford.edu.>Google Scholar
Batllori, Montserrat, and Hernanz, M. Lluïsa, eds. 2011. Generative diachronic syntax: Word order and information structure. Catalan Journal of Linguistics 10: 915.Google Scholar
Belletti, Adriana. 2004. Aspects of the low IP area. In The cartography of syntactic structures, volume 2: The structure of CP and IP, ed. Rizzi, Luigi, 1651. Oxford: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Benincà, Paola. 1999. The position of topic and focus in the left periphery. In Current studies in Italian syntax: Essays offered to Lorenzo Renzi, ed. Cinque, Guglielmo and Salvi, Giampaolo, 3964. Amsterdam: Elsevier-North Holland.Google Scholar
Benincà, Paola. 2006. A detailed map of the left periphery of medieval Romance. In Cross-linguistic research in syntax and semantics: Negation, tense and clausal architecture, ed. Zanuttini, Raffaella, Campos, Héctor, Herberger, Elena, and Portner, Paul, 5386. Washington, D.C.: Georgetown University Press.Google Scholar
Benincà, Paola, and Munaro, Nicola. 2010. The cartography of syntactic structures, volume 5: Mapping the left periphery. Oxford: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Benincà, Paola, and Poletto, Cecilia. 2004. Topic, focus, and V2. In The cartography of syntactic structures, volume 2: The structure of CP and IP, ed. Rizzi, Luigi, 5275. Oxford: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Bohnacker, Ute, and Rosén, Christina. 2007. How to start a V2 declarative clause: Transfer of syntax vs. information structure in L2 German. In Nordlyd 34(3): Papers from the Language Acquisition Workshop SCL 2006, ed. Anderssen, Merete and Westergaard, Marit, 2956.Google Scholar
Brault, Gerard J. 1978. The song of Roland: Oxford text and English translation. Pennsylvania State University Press.Google Scholar
Büring, Daniel. 2007. Semantics, intonation, and information structure. In The Oxford handbook of linguistic interfaces, ed. Ramchand, Gillian and Reiss, Charles, 445474. Oxford: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Charolles, Michel. 1978. Introduction aux problèmes de la cohérence des textes. Langue française 38(1): 741.Google Scholar
Cinque, Guglielmo. 2002. The cartography of syntactic structures, volume 1: Functional structure in DP and IP. Oxford: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Cinque, Guglielmo. 2006. The cartography of syntactic structures, volume 4: Restructuring and functional heads. Oxford: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Cinque, Guglielmo, and Rizzi, Luigi. 2008. The cartography of syntactic structures. CISCL Working Papers 2: 4258.Google Scholar
Cook, Philippa, and Bildhauer, Felix. 2011. Annotating information structure: The case of topic. In Beyond semantics: Corpus-based investigations of pragmatic and discourse phenomena, ed. Dipper, Stefanie and Zinsmeister, Heike. Bochumer Linguistische Arbeitsberichte 3:4556.Google Scholar
van Craenenbroeck, Jeroen. 2009. Alternatives to cartography: An introduction. In Alternatives to cartography, ed. van Craenenbroeck, Jeroen, 114. Berlin: Mouton de Gruyter.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Elsig, Martin. 2009. Verb-second effects in Old French and Middle High German: Considerations from the language border. University of Hamburg: Collaborative Research Center on Multilingualism.Google Scholar
Ferraresi, Gisella, and Goldbach, Maria. 2002. V2 syntax and topicalisation in Old French. Linguistische Berichte 189: 325.Google Scholar
Foulet, Lucien. 1928. Petite syntaxe de l'ancien français. 3rd revised edition. Paris: Honoré Champion.Google Scholar
Frascarelli, Mara, and Hinterhölzl, Roland. 2007. Types of topics in German and Italian. In On information structure, meaning and form: Generalizations across languages, ed. Schwabe, Kerstin and Winkler, Susanne, 87116. Amsterdam: John Benjamins.Google Scholar
Gundel, Jeanette K., and Fretheim, Thorstein. 2004. Topic and focus. In The handbook of pragmatics, ed. Horn, Laurence R. and Ward, Gregory, 175196. Malden, MA: Blackwell Publishing.Google Scholar
Hinterhölzl, Roland. 2009. The role of information structure in word order variation and word order change. In Information structure and language change: New approaches to word order variation in Germanic, ed. Hinterhölzl, Roland and Petrova, Svetlana, 4566. Berlin: Mouton de Gruyter.Google Scholar
Hinterhölzl, Roland, and Petrova, Svetlana. 2009. Information structure and language change: New approaches to word order variation in Germanic. Berlin: Mouton de Gruyter.Google Scholar
Hinterhölzl, Roland, and Petrova, Svetlana. 2010. From V1 to V2 in West Germanic. Lingua 120(2): 315328.Google Scholar
Hinterhölzl, Roland, and Petrova, Svetlana. 2011. Rhetorical relations and verb placement in Old High German. In Salience: Multidisciplinary perspectives on its function in discourse, ed. Chiarcos, Christian, Claus, Berry, and Grabski, Michael, 173202. Berlin: Mouton De Gruyter.Google Scholar
Holmberg, Anders. 2015. Verb second. In Syntax—theory and analysis: An international handbook, volume I, ed. Kiss, Tibor and Alexiadou, Artemis, 342383. Berlin: Mouton De Gruyter.Google Scholar
Kaiser, Georg A. 2000. Dialect contact and language change: A case study on word order change in French. Working Papers in Multilingualism, Sonderforschungsbereich 538. Hamburg: Universität Hamburg.Google Scholar
Kaiser, Georg A. 2002. Verbstellung und Verbstellungswandel in den Romanischen Sprachen. Tübingen: Niemeyer.Google Scholar
Kaiser, Georg A., and Zimmermann, Michael. 2011. On the decrease in subject-verb inversion in French declaratives. In The development of grammar: Language acquisition and diachronic change, in honour of Jürgen M. Meisel, ed. Rinke, Esther and Kupisch, Tanya, 355381. Amsterdam: John Benjamins.Google Scholar
Krifka, Manfred. 2008. Basic notions of information structure. Acta Linguistica Hungarica 55(3–4): 243276.Google Scholar
Kroch, Anthony S., and Santorini, Beatrice. 2009. The comparative evolution of word order in French and English. Paper presented at the 11th Diachronic Generative Syntax Conference, University of Campinas, Brazil. <www.ling.upenn.edu/~kroch/handouts/digs11.pdf>>Google Scholar
Kroch, Anthony S., and Santorini, Beatrice. 2012. Penn supplement to the MCVF corpus of historical French, University of Pennsylvania. <http://www.ling.upenn.edu/hist-corpora>>Google Scholar
Labelle, Marie, and Hirschbuhler, Paul. 2005. Changes in clausal organization and the position of clitics in Old French. In Grammaticalization and parametric variation, ed. Batllori, Montserrat, Hernanz, Maria-Lluïsa, Picallo, Carme, and Roca, Francesc, 6071. New York: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Labelle, Marie, and Hirschbühler, Paul. 2017. Leftward stylistic displacement (LSD) in medieval French. In Micro-change and macro-change in diachronic syntax, ed. Mathieu, Éric and Truswell, Robert, 145176. Oxford: Oxford University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Laenzlinger, Christopher. 2006. Le rôle de l'interface syntaxe–structure informationnelle dans la variation de l'ordre des constituants dans la phrase. Nouveaux cahiers de linguistique française 27: 5381.Google Scholar
Ledgeway, Adam. 2008. Satisfying V2 in early Romance: Merge vs. move. Journal of Linguistics 44(2): 437470.Google Scholar
Marchello-Nizia, Christiane. 1995. L’évolution du français : Ordre des mots, démonstratifs, accent tonique. Paris: Armand Colin.Google Scholar
Marchello-Nizia, Christiane. 1999. Le français en diachronie : douze siècles d’évolution. Paris: Ophrys.Google Scholar
Martineau, France, Hirschbühler, Paul, Kroch, Anthony S., and Morin, Yves-Charles. 2010. Corpus MCVF. Modéliser le changement: les Voies du français. Département de français, Université d'Ottawa. CD-ROM, 1st edition.Google Scholar
Moignet, Gérard. 1985. La chanson de Roland. Paris: Bordas.Google Scholar
Nikolaeva, Irina. 2001. Secondary topic as a relation in information structure. Linguistics 39(1): 149.Google Scholar
Platzack, Christer. 1995. The loss of verb second in English and French. In Clause structure and language change, ed. Battye, Adrian and Roberts, Ian, 200226. New York: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Randall, Beth. 2005. CorpusSearch 2 <http://corpussearch.sourceforge.net>..>Google Scholar
Reinhart, Tanya. 1981. Pragmatics and linguistics: An analysis of sentence topics. Philosophica 27: 5394.Google Scholar
Rickard, Peter. 1962. The Word-order object–verb–subject in medieval French. Transactions of the Philological Society 61(1): 139.Google Scholar
Rinke, Esther, and Meisel, Jürgen M.. 2009. Subject inversion in Old French: Syntax and information structure. In Proceedings of the workshop “Null Subjects, Expletives, and Locatives in Romance”, ed. Kaiser, Georg A. and Remberger, Eva-Maria, 93130. Arbeitspapier Nr. 123. Fachbereich Sprachwissenschaft, Universität Konstanz.Google Scholar
Rizzi, Luigi. 1997. The fine structure of the left periphery. In Elements of grammar: A handbook of generative syntax, ed. Haegeman, Liliane, 281337. Dordrecht: Kluwer.Google Scholar
Rizzi, Luigi. 2004. The cartography of syntactic structures, volume 2: The structure of CP and IP. Oxford: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Roberts, Ian G. 1993. Verbs and diachronic syntax: A comparative history of English and French. Dordrecht: Kluwer.Google Scholar
Rouveret, Alain. 2004. Pronominal clitics and the left periphery in Old French. Bulletin de la Société de linguistique de Paris 99(1): 181237.Google Scholar
Skårup, Povl. 1975. Les premières zones de la proposition en ancien français: essai de syntaxe de position. Études romanes de l'université de Copenhague 6. Copenhague: Akademisk Forlag.Google Scholar
Speyer, Augustin. 2004. Competing constraints on Vorfeldbesetzung in German. In ZAS Papers in Linguistics 35: Proceedings of the Dislocated Elements Workshop, ed. Shaer, Benjamin, Frey, Werner, and Maienborn, Claudia, 519541. Berlin: Zentrum für Allgemeine Sprachwissenschaft.Google Scholar
Speyer, Augustin. 2008. German Vorfeld-filling as constraint interaction. In Constraints in discourse, ed. Benz, Anton and Kühnlein, Peter, 255278. Amsterdam: John Benjamins.Google Scholar
Speyer, Augustin. 2010. Filling the Vorfeld in written and spoken discourse. In Discourses in interaction, ed. Tanskanen, Sanna-Kaisa, Helasvuo, Marja-Liisa, Johansson, Marjut, and Raitaniemi, Mia, 263290. Amsterdam: John Benjamins.Google Scholar
Thurneysen, Rudolf. 1892. Zur Stellung des Verbums im Altfranzösischen. Zeitschrift für Romanische Philologie 16: 289307.Google Scholar
Vallduví, Enric. 1993. The informational component. Technical report IRCS 93–38, University of Pennsylvania.Google Scholar
Vance, Barbara. 1995. On the decline of verb movement to Comp in Old and Middle French. In Clause structure and language change, ed. Battye, Adrian and Roberts, Ian G., 173199. Oxford: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Zaring, Laurie. 2010. Changing from OV to VO: More evidence from Old French. Ianua. Revista Philologica Romanica 10: 118.Google Scholar
Zaring, Laurie. This issue. The nature of V2 in Old French: Evidence from subject inversion in embedded clauses.Google Scholar
Figure 0

Table 1: Informational role of postverbal subjects in V1 declaratives.

Figure 1

Figure 1: V1 declaratives–Informational nature of subjects: Number of postverbal subjects that are I-topic, I-focus or Unclear. The numbers at the bottom indicate the total number of relevant examples.

Figure 2

Figure 2: Verbs introducing direct discourse: Rates of SVX sentences as compared to VS and XVS sentences. The numbers at the bottom correspond to the number of relevant examples.

Figure 3

Figure 3a: Preverbal subject types: Rates of subject types in preverbal position.

Figure 4

Figure 3b: Postverbal subject types: Rates of subject types in postverbal position.

Figure 5

Figure 4: Postverbal indefinite subjects over total indefinite subjects (in text with more than 10 indefinite subjects): Rates of postverbal indefinite subjects. The numbers at the bottom correspond to the total number of relevant examples.

Figure 6

Figure 5: Informational status of preverbal objects: Rates of preverbal objects that are I-focus, I-topic, or Unclear. The numbers at the bottom correspond to the total number of relevant examples.

Figure 7

Table 2: Distribution of adjectives, predicative NPs and non finite verbs in preverbal position.

Figure 8

Figure 6: Information function of postverbal subjets in Adv-V-S clauses: Percentage of postverbal subjects that are coded as I-focus or I-topic in texts containing more than nine relevant examples.

Figure 9

Table 3: Constituents in the prefield in German and Swedish (from Bohnacker and Rosén 2007).

Figure 10

Table 4: Constituents in the prefield in Old French.