As is well known, longer life expectancy leads to an increase in the prevalence of dementia. This means that our efforts to alleviate or at least defer the onset of Alzheimer's disease and other dementias must be all the more urgent.
The terms ‘brain health’, ‘brain fitness’ and ‘improvement of lifestyle’ are often used to refer to self-determined areas of cognitive ageing. Geropsychology and geropsychiatry constantly supply us with study results which seem to support the view that dementia is associated with everyday activities and lifestyles that affect our health.
In this paper, we explain the concept of ‘motivational reserve’ that complements the more established concepts of brain reserve and cognitive reserve. Brain reserve is believed to enable the brain to tolerate neuropathological dementia-related changes that do not have clinical manifestations.Reference Valenzuela1 The brain uses untended networks more efficiently and thus compensates for networks that are disrupted, which is an example of neuronal plasticity. Motivational reserve refers to the driving forces or processes that lead to this more efficient or flexible use of brain networks. We hypothesised that people with strong motivational abilities are at a lower risk of developing age-related cognitive decline or dementia.Reference Forstmeier and Maercker2,Reference Forstmeier and Maercker3 Motivational reserve thus equips the individual with resilience against neuropathological damage.
Motivational reserve studies
Delay of gratification studies
Motivational abilities that contribute to motivational reserve are – in everyday language – self-discipline, willpower and diligence. Motivation psychology extensively investigates concepts such as motivation regulation, decision regulation, activation regulation, goal orientation, intentionality and self-efficacy. Impressive accounts of the results of research on the delay of gratification have been presented at almost all developmental psychology meetings all over the world. One example would be studies with children conducted by the eminent psychologist Walter Mischel using the ‘marshmallow test’.Reference Mischel, Ebbesen and Zeiss4 The children were asked to sit at a table on which a marshmallow was placed. They were told that they would be given two marshmallows if they waited for the researcher to arrive. About 75% of children were unable to wait, but 25% managed to, and were rewarded with two marshmallows. Astonishingly, this ability to delay gratification is a relatively stable personality attribute predictive of many later abilities of children and adolescents, such as intelligence, academic and social competence, self-control, self-efficacy, goal orientation, as well as health-related behaviours such as drug misuse.Reference Mischel, Ayduk, Berman, Casey, Gotlib and Jonides5
Role of motivational abilities in guarding against dementia
Which motivational abilities therefore might be responsible for an increased resilience to dementia? No delay of gratification tests for adults or the elderly so far exist, although we are working on adequate complex variants of such a test.Reference Forstmeier, Drobetz and Maercker6 Adults differ with regard to motivational engagement in their work and other activities. Occupational scientists have developed a directory that provides a specific profile of motivational, cognitive and social abilities, among others, for a huge range of occupations (Occupational Information Network, O*NET).Reference Peterson, Mumford, Borman, Jeanneret and Fleishman7 For example, fashion designers show high motivational abilities, but retail salespersons show rather low motivational abilities.
The core assumption of the motivational reserve model is that a high motivational ability leads to a lower incidence of cognitive decline and dementia through a higher brain reserve, and we were able to apply our model to an existing longitudinal study.Reference Forstmeier, Maercker, Maier, van den Bussche, Riedel-Heller and Kaduszkiewicz8 The German AgeCoDe sampleReference Luck, Riedel-Heller, Kaduszkiewicz, Bickel, Jessen and Pentzek9 comprises 3300 individuals aged 75 and above who do not have dementia; so far, they have been followed up twice, after 1.5 and 3 years. About 300 participants have developed mild cognitive impairment and 70 have developed Alzheimer's disease. It was found that high occupation-related motivational abilities in mid-life reduced the risk of mild cognitive impairment by 35%. The effect on the risk of Alzheimer's depended on the presence of an ApoE e4 allele which intensifies the biochemical disturbances characteristic of the disease, including beta-amyloid deposition, tangle formation, neuronal cell death and synaptic plasticity. Mid-life motivational abilities were associated with a reduced risk of Alzheimer's disease in ApoE e4 carriers but not in non-carriers. Confirming previous results from a cross-sectional study,Reference Forstmeier and Maercker2 this was now clear evidence supporting the assumption that high motivational abilities in mid-life have effects that both promote health and prevent cognitive decline in later life. These effects seem to go beyond pure cognitive abilities, such as verbal intelligence, education, and occupation-related cognitive abilities in mid-life.Reference Forstmeier, Maercker, Maier, van den Bussche, Riedel-Heller and Kaduszkiewicz8
Motivational reserve and healthy brain
What, therefore, are the consequences of healthy brain ageing? Similar results from other studies point in the same direction. Many original studies and meta-analyses show that the extent of physical activities (e.g. walking, dancing, swimming) and social activities (e.g. visiting friends, going to the theatre, participating in social groups) an individual engages in influences the incidence of dementia.Reference Forstmeier and Maercker3,Reference Polidori, Nelles and Pientka10 Cognitive activities (e.g. reading books and newspapers, writing, studying, doing crossword puzzles) have long been credited with protective effects and have shown promising results in studies. In our opinion, a mixture of motivational (‘I motivate myself to be active’) and cognitive (‘I remain mentally fit’) factors always underlie these cognitive abilities. However, cognitive abilities have been traditionally considered more important than motivational (and social) factors, which are referred to in the established concept of cognitive reserve as the main resiliency factor against age-related decline. Two independent large-scale studies indicate the importance of motivational abilities.
The first study investigated ‘conscientiousness’, the personality trait that refers to an individual's tendency to control impulses and be goal directed. A cohort of 1000 men and women over 65 years old in religious orders in the USA were followed up 12 years after enrolment.Reference Wilson, Schneider, Arnold, Bienias and Bennett11 A high level of conscientiousness was associated with an 89% reduction in the risk of Alzheimer's disease. The results were not substantially different when other personality traits, depression, activity patterns, vascular conditions, and other risk factors were taken into account. Interestingly, the level of conscientiousness is a subtle marker of clinically manifest Alzheimer's disease because people with clinically diagnosed Alzheimer's disease are described by others as being less conscientious than they were before onset, and further decline is observed as the disease progresses.Reference Pocnet, Rossier, Antonietti and von Gunten12
A purposeful life equals a healthier life
Having a purpose in life also constitutes a motivational ability because it results in intentionality and goal-directed action that guides behaviour. Researchers in ChicagoReference Boyle, Buchman, Barnes and Bennett13 found that a high purpose in life, reported by people over 80 years old, reduced the incidence of mild cognitive impairment in the subsequent 7 years by a factor of 1.4 and the incidence of Alzheimer's disease by a factor of 2.4 (even when controlling for covariates such as education and depression). The authors found no causal explanation for this surprising finding, other than the fact that a purpose in life is generally associated with lower mortality and changes in a range of physical health parameters (e.g. increased high-density lipoprotein cholesterol levels, fewer pathological immune markers). Since purpose in life may translate into an increased sense of intentionality or goal-directed striving, in our view the result of the study contributes to the broader concept of motivational reserve.
Both conscientiousness and purpose in life do not arise late in life. Basic research has shown that these two factors – and motivational abilities in general – gradually develop throughout young adulthood and old age, perhaps partly in response to life experiences and changing circumstances in the workplace and home environment that occur at those periods. Interestingly, in the Chicago study on ‘purpose of life’,Reference Boyle, Buchman, Barnes and Bennett13 no reference was made to the earlier study on conscientiousness,Reference Wilson, Schneider, Arnold, Bienias and Bennett11 although we consider both variables closely related as motivational factors.
Motivational resilience – clinical implications
What can we infer from all this for healthy brain ageing and the prevention of age-related cognitive decline or an increase in the incidence of Alzheimer's disease? Is it possible that there are motivation-related interventions that may influence the functioning of the brain? When answering these questions, we must differentiate between two potential windows of opportunity for prevention or intervention: long-term prevention in mid-life and short-term interventions in patients older than 70. We are convinced that the focus of prevention should be in the mid-life period because these are rather new and important considerations.
Popular enthusiasm for some types of lifestyle interventions has been considerable. In particular, cognitive stimulation enjoys wide media coverage,Reference Wollen14 although the benefits are not strictly evidence based. The ability to learn new things, which is certainly maintained in older age, is still viewed by some as the core factor in resilience against cognitive decline or neuropathology. However, it is conceivable that neurogenesis of dendrites or intensive brain activation might also be stimulated by a sense of purpose or drive, and striving.
We feel it is time to reconsider the role of psychologically inspired lifestyle interventions. The following principles shift the emphasis towards motivational abilities and can be applied as preventative measures at any stage of life.
• Challenges should not be avoided. We should find solutions to problems that at first seem insurmountable.
• Self-commitment (‘Do not give up’) and conscientiousness intensify the experiences we have, and although these are sometimes difficult to sustain, the outcome can be very rewarding and instructive for the future.
• The capacity to regulate ourselves and overcome impulses and drives that tempt us – to overeat, drink too much alcohol, take harmful drugs, say hurtful things to others – should be enhanced. This has a short- and a long-term perspective. Self-regulation may fail now and again; the important thing is to regain the former degree or a better level of control and not to give up (so-called ego-depletionReference Hagger, Wood, Stiff and Chatzisarantis15).
• We should make ourselves repeatedly aware of our purpose in life by active reflection. The self-coherence thus achieved seems to benefit psychological processes and the brain.Reference Freund, Nikitin and Ritter16
Although these principles at present look more like the idealistic hopes of a self-advancement movement, we would like to express our cautious optimism that they may one day be tested empirically for the benefit of patients, in the same way as cognitive variables have been.
eLetters
No eLetters have been published for this article.