Hostname: page-component-76fb5796d-5g6vh Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-04-25T15:18:59.807Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

Has the Genetic Contribution to the Propensity to Gamble Increased? Evidence From National Twin Studies Conducted in 1962 and 2002

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  12 March 2018

Wendy S. Slutske*
Affiliation:
Department of Psychological Sciences, University of Missouri, Columbia, MO, USA
*
address for correspondence: Professor Wendy S. Slutske, Department of Psychological Sciences, University of Missouri, Columbia, MO 65211, USA. E-mail: slutskew@missouri.edu

Abstract

Social changes, such as the expansion of legal forms of gambling, can influence not only the prevalence of gambling, but can also shape the relative importance of genetic and environmental contributions to individual differences in the propensity to gamble. In the present study, I examined differences in the prevalence and in the relative contribution of genetic and environmental factors to gambling involvement in the United States in 1962 versus 2002. The data came from two sources: (1) a survey of 839 17-year-old same-sex twin pairs from the National Merit Scholarship Qualifying Test twin study, and (2) an interview of 477 18- to 26-year-old same-sex twin pairs from Wave III of the National Longitudinal Study of Adolescent to Adult Health. Similar measures of gambling participation were included in the two studies. Evidence for a genotype-by-time interaction was evaluated by testing whether the contribution of genetic influences was greater in the more recently born cohort of twins. Despite the major changes in the gambling landscape over the intervening 40 years, there was no evidence for such an interaction. The contribution of genetic factors and environmental factors did not significantly differ and there was no evidence for genetic influences at either time point. Instead, the variation in the propensity to gamble was explained nearly equally by common and unique environmental factors. Explanations for this surprising finding are discussed.

Type
Articles
Copyright
Copyright © The Author(s) 2018 

There have been massive changes in the availability of legal gambling opportunities in the United States over the last half century (Tidwell et al., Reference Tidwell, Welte, Barnes and Dayanim2015). Coinciding with this increased availability have been increases in the prevalence of gambling involvement and disorder (Gerstein et al., Reference Gerstein, Volberg, Toce, Harwood, Christiansen, Hoffman and Hill1999; Kessler et al., Reference Kessler, Hwang, LaBrie, Petukhova, Sampson, Winters and Shaffer2008; Shaffer & Hall, Reference Shaffer and Hall2001; Welte et al., Reference Welte, Barnes, Tidwell and Hoffman2011), and a decrease in the average age of gambling initiation (Richmond-Rakerd et al., Reference Richmond-Rakerd, Slutske and Piasecki2013).

Social changes, such as the expansion of legal forms of gambling, can influence not only the prevalence of a behavior, but can also shape the relative importance of genetic and environmental contributions to individual differences in the behavior. Cigarette smoking provides the best example. Cigarettes became much more widely available early in the 20th century with the invention of automatic cigarette rolling machines and safety matches, and a rise in product marketing (Cummings & Proctor, Reference Cummings and Proctor2014). This led to an increase in the prevalence of cigarette smoking in the United States from essentially zero in 1900 to 42% by 1963 (Burns et al., Reference Burns, Lee, Shen, Gilpin, Tolley, Vaughn and Shanks1997). In the latter half of the 20th century, a series of public health events, including the pivotal Surgeon General's report on smoking and health in 1964 (Burns et al., Reference Burns, Lee, Shen, Gilpin, Tolley, Vaughn and Shanks1997; U.S. Department of Health and Human Services, 2014), marked the beginning of a decline in the prevalence of smoking to 15% in 2015 (King et al., Reference King, Neff, Whitmill, Babb and Graffunder2016).

Just as changes in availability and public health policies affected the prevalence of smoking, they also appear to have affected the extent to which individual differences were due to genetic or environmental influences. For example, an examination of birth cohort differences in the heritability of smoking in a national U.S. twin sample yielded striking differences in the heritability of smoking among those who came of age prior to 1964 when cigarettes were cheap, plentiful, socially acceptable, and not yet considered a health risk (h 2 = ~60%) compared to those who came of age around the time that the Surgeon General's report was released (h 2 = ~0; Boardman et al., Reference Boardman, Blalock and Pampel2010). A similar phenomenon was observed in Swedish and Spanish twin studies that showed that as the prevalence of smoking among women in more recently born birth cohorts increased, the heritability also increased, both presumably due to increasing social acceptability of smoking in women (Kendler et al., Reference Kendler, Thornton and Pedersen2000; Mezquita et al., Reference Mezquita, Sánchez-Romera, Ibáñez, Morosoli, Colodro-Conde, Ortet and Ordoñana2018). These changes in heritability observed in different birth cohorts are examples of a phenomenon that might be termed genotype-by-time interaction—a genotype-by-environment interaction (Shanahan & Hofer, Reference Shanahan and Hofer2005) where the relevant environment is change occurring across historical periods.

Several theories of the mechanisms underlying the genotype-by-time interaction have been proposed (Boardman et al., Reference Boardman, Blalock and Pampel2010). The social control model posits that genetic differences are dampened over time by social forces (Shanahan & Hofer, Reference Shanahan and Hofer2005), whereas the social trigger model suggests that genetic differences are amplified over time by social forces. For example, the rise in the heritability of smoking prior to the Surgeon General's report in the United States (and among women in Sweden and Spain) could be explained by a social trigger model, and the declining heritability of smoking after the report is consistent with the social control model. A third model, a social push model, asserts that genetic differences may be hidden or highlighted by changing social forces. This model provides an explanation for the increasing heritability of smoking in the decades since the Surgeon General's report (Boardman et al., Reference Boardman, Blalock and Pampel2010). The population of smokers that remained may have shifted to those who were more genetically vulnerable to nicotine addiction (Boardman et al., Reference Boardman, Blalock, Pampel, Hatemi, Heath and Eaves2011; Emery et al., Reference Emery, Gilpin, Ake, Farkas and Pierce2000; Hughes, Reference Hughes2011).

There are some parallels in the rising popularity and prevalence of gambling in the latter half of the 20th century (Volberg et al, Reference Volberg, Toce and Gerstein1999) to smoking in the first half (Brandt, Reference Brandt2007) in the United States. This raises the question of whether there may also be parallels in the contribution of genetic factors to individual differences among cohorts born during different historical periods. This question was the focus of the present study. The genetic and environmental contributions to gambling were examined in two distinct birth cohorts born around 1945 and 1980. Gambling involvement in the two cohorts was assessed around 1962 and 2002. In 1962, the United States was still going through a period of gambling prohibition, there were no legal lotteries in the United States, and the only casinos were in Nevada. In 2002, there was legal gambling in nearly every state, including lotteries in 41 states and casinos in 30. I hypothesized that, based on predictions of the social trigger model, the prevalence and heritability of gambling would be higher in the 2002 than in the 1962 cohort.

Methods

Participants

Secondary analyses were conducted of data from two national samples of young people, the National Merit Scholarship Qualifying Test twin study and the National Longitudinal Study of Adolescent to Adult Health.

National Merit Scholarship Qualifying Test twin study (National Merit)

This is a sample of same-sex twin pairs who participated in the 1962 National Merit Scholarship Qualifying Test (Loehlin & Nichols, Reference Loehlin and Nichols1976). Twin pairs were identified from 596,241 high school juniors who completed the test; 1,507 same-sex twin pairs were recruited to respond to further questionnaires about personality and behavior. Participation rates were very good (79%). Among the respondents were 509 monozygotic (MZ) and 330 same-sex dizygotic (DZ) twin pairs. Participants were approximately 17-years old at the time of the test administration and the sample was overwhelmingly (98%) Caucasian, with females being slightly over-represented (58%). A previous paper (Slutske et al., Reference Slutske, Meier, Zhu, Statham, Blaszczynski and Martin2009) included summary statistics of the individual gambling behaviors in this cohort that were extracted from the original source (Loehlin & Nichols, Reference Loehlin and Nichols1976). The raw data were subsequently obtained, which made this new set of analyses possible.

National Longitudinal Study of Adolescent to Adult Health (Add Health)

This sample is from the Wave III in-home interview of the National Longitudinal Study of Adolescent to Adult Health, which took place in 2001–2002 and included an assessment of gambling. The Add Health study included a genetically informative subsample of twins and siblings (Harris et al., Reference Harris, Halpern, Smolen and Haberstick2006). To be consistent with the National Merit twin study, only data from the 220 complete (and 35 partial) MZ and 182 complete (and 40 partial) same-sex DZ twin pairs were used in this analysis. The twin pairs in this national sample came from 45 different states and 87% of the pairs lived in the same state. Participant response rates at Wave III for the genetic sample were excellent (92% for MZ twins and 89% for DZ twins). All participants gave informed consent and the study was approved by the Institutional Review Board of the University of North Carolina at Chapel Hill. These data have previously been published (Slutske & Richmond-Rakerd, Reference Slutske and Richmond-Rakerd2014), but not in the context of an investigation of a cross-cohort genotype-by-time interaction.

Measures

Both studies included assessments of the most common gambling activities of the time. Included in the National Merit survey were four questions about the frequency of participation in gambling (frequently, occasionally, or not at all) in the past year. The first two response options were combined to harmonize the assessments between the two samples (and because the endorsement of ‘frequently’ was rare). Included in the Add Health interview were eight questions about gambling, with the first three questions about ever participating in three different types of gambling activities (Slutske & Richmond-Rakerd, Reference Slutske and Richmond-Rakerd2014). With the exception of playing the lottery, which was not yet legal in the United States at the time of the National Merit study, there was considerable overlap between the two assessments (see Table 1). The gambling items were combined into a single ‘any gambling’ outcome in the two studies.

TABLE 1 Assessment and Prevalence of Gambling Behaviors in the Two Cohorts

M = men, W = women; samples sizes for the National Merit study are 701 men and 976 women; for the Add Health study are 437 men and 442 women.

Data Analysis

The frequencies of any gambling in the two samples and among men and women were compared using survey data analysis procedures in SAS version 9.4 (SAS Institute Inc., Cary, NC) that take into account the non-independence of twin pair observations.

Biometric models were fitted using robust weighted least squares directly to the raw twin data using Mplus Version 7 (Muthén and Muthén, Reference Muthén and Muthén2012). To account for the age differences in the two cohorts, models included age as a covariate. Univariate biometric model fitting was conducted to partition the variation in the propensity to gamble into additive genetic (A), shared environmental (C), and unique environmental (E) influences, and to test for cohort differences in the genetic and environmental contributions to gambling liability (Prescott, Reference Prescott2004; Rijsdijk & Sham, Reference Rijsdijk and Sham2002). Evidence for cohort differences was tested by comparing the fits of models that allowed parameter estimates for the National Merit 1962 and Add Health 2002 cohorts to vary with the fits of models that constrained the estimates to be the same. Evidence for sex differences was similarly tested by comparing the fits of models that allowed parameter estimates for the men and women to vary with the fits of models that constrained the estimates to be the same.

Results

Prevalences

The prevalence of gambling in 2002 was significantly higher than in 1962 in the full sample (75% vs. 49%; odds ratio [OR] = 2.80; χ 2 = 103.35, df = 1, p < .0001), among men (OR = 1.75; χ 2 = 12.91, df = 1, p = .0003) and women (OR = 3.76; χ 2 = 97.90, df = 1, p < .0001). In both the 1962 (OR = 3.07; χ 2 = 90.78, df = 1, p < .0001) and 2002 (OR = 1.43; χ 2 = 4.26, df = 1, p = .039) cohorts, the prevalence of gambling was higher among men than among women (see Table 1). When the 2002 cohort was restricted to a sample closer in age (18–19 years of age) to the 1962 cohort, the prevalences of gambling among men (68%) and women (55%) decreased and the differences between the two cohorts were attenuated. This suggests that the differences in prevalences were due to age as well as birth cohort effects.

Biometric Model Fitting

A model that allowed the contribution of genetic and environmental influences to the propensity to gamble to differ in the two cohorts did not fit significantly better than a model that constrained the contributions to be equivalent (Δχ 2 = 1.01, df = 2, p = .60). In both the 1962 (Δχ 2 = 0.71, df = 2, p = .70) and the 2002 cohorts (Δχ 2 = 0.05, df = 2, p = .97), the contribution of genetic and environmental influences to the propensity to gamble did not significantly differ in men and women. In the models that constrained the parameter estimates in men and women, there was evidence that the shared and unique environment, but not genetic factors, significantly contributed to the propensity to gamble in both cohorts (see Table 2).

TABLE 2 Proportion of Variation in Any Gambling Explained by Additive (A), Shared Environmental (C), and Unique Environmental (E) Influences Among Young People in 1962 Versus 2002

A = additive genetic influences, C = shared environmental influences, E = unique environmental influences, CI = confidence interval. Parameters in bold are significantly greater than zero at p < .05.

Discussion

Based on compelling findings from the smoking literature, I examined the prevalence and genetic and environmental contributions to the propensity to gamble in two distinct birth cohorts born around 1945 and 1980. As expected, the prevalence of any gambling increased, from 49% among those born around 1945 (in 1962) to 75% among those born around 1980 (in 2002). Interestingly, the first U.S. national gambling prevalence survey was conducted 13 years after the National Merit twin study, at which point there were still casinos in only one state, but legal lotteries in 12; the prevalence of any gambling had reached 73%Footnote 1 among 18–24 year olds (Kallick et al., Reference Kallick, Suits, Dielman and Hybels1979).

Contrary to predictions based on the social trigger model, there were no differences in the genetic contribution to gambling propensity across these two historical periods. In fact, in both the 1962 National Merit sample and the 2002 Add Health sample, all of the variation in the propensity to gamble was due to environmental factors, including those environmental factors that are shared between members of the same family and those that are unique to the individual. What makes these consistent findings so remarkable is that the gambling participation in 1962 was primarily non-commercial or illegal, whereas in 2002 it was primarily commercial and legal.

Another puzzling aspect of these results is that they are not in line with the two previous twin studies that focused on gambling among young people. In a study of 279 13-year-old Quebec twin pairs, gambling involvement was completely explained by genetic and unique environmental factors, with no contribution from the shared family environment (Vitaro et al., Reference Vitaro, Hartl, Brendgen, Laursen, Dionne and Boivin2014). A study of 756 Minnesota twin pairs assessed at ages 18 and 25 years reported results for several measures of gambling, including one ‘typical frequency’ similar to the measure used in the Quebec study (King et al., Reference King, Keyes, Winters, McGue and Iacono2017).Footnote 2 In the Minnesota study, there were significant genetic influences on variation in typical frequency at ages 18 and 25 years. There was a substantial and significant contribution of the shared environment only at age 18 years (King et al., Reference King, Keyes, Winters, McGue and Iacono2017).

One potential explanation for the discrepant findings of the present and the two prior studies might be that the measures focused on different stages of gambling. The present study focused on whether the participant had gambled or not (either in the past year or ever), which is similar to the concept of initiation used in the substance use literature. Initiation is the milestone in the gambling career when one transitions from being a non-gambler to a gambler. Once gambling has been initiated, gamblers can be differentiated on a number of characteristics, such as how frequently they gamble or whether they develop problems as a consequence of their gambling. The previous studies focused on a composite indicator of gambling that included initiation and the average frequency of participating in different types of gambling. Based on findings from the substance use literature demonstrating that the contribution of genetic influences increase and the contribution of shared environmental influences decrease with the stage of substance use involvement (Fowler et al., Reference Fowler, Lifford, Shelton, Rice, Thapar, Neale and van den Bree2007; Pagan et al., Reference Pagan, Rose, Viken, Pulkken, Kaprio and Dick2006; Rhee et al., Reference Rhee, Hewitt, Young, Corley, Crowley and Stallings2003), it has been suggested that genetic factors may play an increasing role and shared environmental factors a decreasing role at later stages of gambling involvement progression (i.e., from initiation to frequency of use to disordered gambling; Slutske, in press; Slutske & Richmond-Rakerd, Reference Slutske and Richmond-Rakerd2014). In other words, whether one gambles at all is primarily due to the environment; how or how much one gambles is also due to genes.

Another explanation might reside with the composition of the samples. The present study was based on national twin samples, whereas the two previous studies were based on regional twin samples. Although the shared environment is often assumed to be a feature specific to the family, especially in national samples such as the National Merit and Add Health twin samples, ‘place’ is an important aspect of the shared environment. (Of course, place would also potentially be an important aspect of the unique environment in adult twin samples—that is, twins might move apart once they reach adulthood.) This is particularly true for gambling in the United States because it is regulated at the state level. Thus, observing substantial contributions of the shared environment in the National Merit and Add Health twin studies, but weaker contributions in the Minnesota and Quebec twin studies, may be explained by geographic differences contributing to differences between families (that are shared by the members of twins residing in the same state). This is consistent with evidence from national studies documenting associations between the number of legal gambling activities in one's state and the prevalence of gambling, gambling frequently, or of having a gambling disorder (Kallick et al., Reference Kallick, Suits, Dielman and Hybels1979; Welte et al., Reference Welte, Tidwell, Barnes, Hoffman and Wieczorek2016); the number of legal gambling activities even increased the prevalence of participating in illegal gambling activities in the 1975 national survey (Kallick et al., Reference Kallick, Suits, Dielman and Hybels1979). In sum, the state where one lives is a strong contributor to individual differences in gambling involvement and it is shared by adolescent twin pairs.

The fundamental issue is that estimates of genetic and environmental influences are a function of the overall variation in a trait (in a specific population at a given time). The overall variation is composed of genetic and environmental portions. Greater environmental variability will lead to a proportionately greater contribution to the overall variability in a population, and changes in the amount of environmental variability will change the estimates of both genetic and environmental influences. The thesis here is that there will be greater environmental variability in a national U.S. twin sample than a regional twin sample, and that this will be reflected in a greater contribution of environmental influences in the former than the latter.

Although it is not possible to adjudicate between the ‘stage’ and the ‘place’ explanations of the different study findings, it appears that they are at least as important as ‘time’ when trying to understand contributions to individual differences in gambling. With respect to the issue of ‘place’, it is likely that in a national U.S. sample of adolescent twins all living together in the same state, the profound influence of environmental variation will mask any genetic contributions to the propensity to gamble. When one hones in on contexts with less environmental variation, the influence of genetic factors may ‘shine through’ (Boardman et al., Reference Boardman, Blalock and Pampel2010; Raine, Reference Raine2002).

Limitations

The main limitation of this study was that the ages of the samples differed—the participants in the National Merit sample were all approximately 17 years old, the participants in the Add Health sample were 18–26 years of age. Thus, the differences in prevalences may have been due to age as well as birth cohort effects. However, there is no plausible explanation of how failure to find cohort differences in the proportions of variation in the propensity to gamble attributable to genetic and environmental factors could be due to age differences between the two samples. In other words, the main finding of this study is unaffected by the age differences between the two cohorts.

Conclusions

There was no evidence to support the social trigger model of gene-by-time interaction in the propensity to gamble among young people. Rather, given the similar findings in 1962 and 2002, it is possible that the genetic contributions to gambling propensity were being masked by environmental cross-state differences in the United States that existed in 1962 and that still existed in 2002.Footnote 3 Especially when interpreting findings of behavioral genetic studies of gambling (or other activities for which regulations vary across jurisdictions, such as marijuana use, e.g., Azofeifa et al., Reference Azofeifa, Sherman, Mattson and Pacula2018), the varied and changing landscape of gambling over time and place must always be considered. The increasing popularity of online gambling (Welte et al., Reference Welte, Barnes, Tidwell, Hoffman and Wieczorek2015), delivered directly to nearly anywhere in the world via mobile technologies, may eventually level the gambling landscape in the coming years. It is hypothesized that such a leveling of the gambling landscape would alter the relative contributions of genetic and environmental factors to gambling involvement in national U.S. twin studies.

Acknowledgments

Preparation of this manuscript was supported in part by a Center of Excellence grant from the National Center for Responsible Gaming. The research uses data from Add Health, a program project directed by Kathleen Mullan Harris and designed by J. Richard Udry, Peter S. Bearman, and Kathleen Mullan Harris at the University of North Carolina at Chapel Hill, and funded by grant P01-HD31921 from the Eunice Kennedy Shriver National Institute of Child Health and Human Development, with cooperative funding from 23 other federal agencies and foundations. Special acknowledgment is due to Ronald R. Rindfuss and Barbara Entwisle for assistance in the original design. Information on how to obtain the Add Health data files is available on the Add Health website (http://www.cpc.unc.edu/addhealth). No direct support was received from grant P01-HD31921 for this analysis. The author also expresses gratitude to John Loehlin and Robert Nichols for having the foresight to assess gambling behavior in their landmark study. None of the authors have any conflicts of interest to report.

Footnotes

1 Based on a more comprehensive assessment of gambling than used in the present study.

2 This entailed averaging the frequency of participation in 10 (King et al., Reference King, Keyes, Winters, McGue and Iacono2017) or 12 (Vitaro et al., Reference Vitaro, Hartl, Brendgen, Laursen, Dionne and Boivin2014) different gambling activities over the past 12 months, with past-year non-gamblers coded as zero. This measure taps into several components—gambling versatility, frequency, and initiation.

3 A misconception held by some is that only problematic outcomes such as ‘gambling addiction’ can be influenced by genes, and that leisure activities such as gambling participation cannot. This runs contrary to a vast behavioral genetic literature documenting heritable influences on nearly every human behavioral trait studied (Plomin et al., Reference Plomin, DeFries, Knopik and Neiderhiser2016; Turkheimer, Reference Turkheimer2000).

References

Azofeifa, A., Sherman, L. J., Mattson, M. E., & Pacula, R. L. (2018). Marijuana buyers in the United States, 2010–2014. Drug and Alcohol Dependence, 183, 3442.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Boardman, J. D., Blalock, C. L., & Pampel, F. C. (2010). Trends in the genetic influences on smoking. Journal of Health and Social Behavior, 51, 108123.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Boardman, J. D., Blalock, C. L., Pampel, F. C., Hatemi, P. K., Heath, A. C., & Eaves, L. J. (2011). Population composition, public policy, and the genetics of smoking. Demography, 48, 15171533.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Brandt, A. M. (2007). The cigarette century: The rise, fall, and deadly persistence of the product that defined America. New York, NY: Basic Books.Google Scholar
Burns, D. M., Lee, L., Shen, L. Z., Gilpin, E., Tolley, H. D., Vaughn, J., & Shanks, T. G. (1997). Cigarette smoking behavior in the United States. Changes in cigarette-related disease risks and their implication for prevention and control. Smoking and Tobacco Control Monograph, 8, 1342.Google Scholar
Cummings, K. M., & Proctor, R. N. (2014). The changing public image of smoking in the United States: 1964–2014. Cancer Epidemiology and Prevention Biomarkers, 23, 3236.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Emery, S., Gilpin, E. A., Ake, C., Farkas, A. J., & Pierce, J. P. (2000). Characterizing and identifying ‘hard-core’ smokers: Implications for further reducing smoking prevalence. American Journal of Public Health, 90, 387394.Google ScholarPubMed
Fowler, T., Lifford, K., Shelton, K., Rice, F., Thapar, A., Neale, M. C., . . . van den Bree, M. B. (2007). Exploring the relationship between genetic and environmental influences on initiation and progression of substance use. Addiction, 102, 413422.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Gerstein, D. R., Volberg, R. A., Toce, M. T., Harwood, H., Christiansen, E. M., Hoffman, J., . . . Hill, M. A. (1999). Gambling impact and behavior study: Report to the national gambling impact study commission. Chicago, IL: National Opinion Research Center, University of Chicago.Google Scholar
Harris, K. M., Halpern, C. T., Smolen, A., & Haberstick, B. C. (2006). The national longitudinal study of adolescent health (Add Health) twin data. Twin Research and Human Genetics, 9, 988997.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Hughes, J. R. (2011). The hardening hypothesis: Is the ability to quit decreasing due to increasing nicotine dependence? A review and commentary. Drug and Alcohol Dependence, 117, 111117.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Kallick, M. D., Suits, T., Dielman, T., & Hybels, J. (1979). A survey of American gambling attitudes and behavior (Research Report Series). Ann Arbor: Survey Research Center, Institute for Social Research, University of Michigan Press.Google Scholar
Kendler, K. S., Thornton, L. M., & Pedersen, N. L. (2000). Tobacco consumption in Swedish twins reared apart and reared together. Archives of General Psychiatry, 57, 886892.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Kessler, R. C., Hwang, I., LaBrie, R., Petukhova, M., Sampson, N. A., Winters, K. C., & Shaffer, H. J. (2008). The prevalence and correlates of DSM-IV pathological gambling in the national comorbidity survey replication. Psychological Medicine, 38, 13511360.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
King, J. A., Neff, L. J., Whitmill, J., Babb, S. D., & Graffunder, C. M. (2016). Current cigarette smoking among adults—United States, 2005–2015. Morbidity and Mortality Weekly Report, 65, 12051211.Google Scholar
King, S. M., Keyes, M., Winters, K. C., McGue, M., & Iacono, W. G. (2017). Genetic and environmental origins of gambling behaviors from ages 18 to 25: A longitudinal twin family study. Psychology of Addictive Behaviors, 31, 367374.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Loehlin, J. C., & Nichols, R. C. (1976). Heredity, environment, and personality: A study of 850 sets of twins. Austin, TX: University of Texas Press.Google Scholar
Mezquita, L., Sánchez-Romera, J. F., Ibáñez, M. I., Morosoli, J. J., Colodro-Conde, L., Ortet, G., & Ordoñana, J. R. (2018). Effects of social attitude change on smoking heritability. Behavior Genetics, 48, 1221.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Muthén, L. K., & Muthén, B. O. (2012). Mplus user's guide (7th ed.). Los Angeles, CA: Author.Google Scholar
Pagan, J. L., Rose, R. J., Viken, R. J., Pulkken, L., Kaprio, J., & Dick, D. M. (2006). Genetic and environmental influences on stages of alcohol use across adolescence and into young adulthood. Behavior Genetics, 36, 483497.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Plomin, R., DeFries, J. C., Knopik, V. S., & Neiderhiser, J. M. (2016). Top 10 replicated findings from behavioral genetics. Perspectives on Psychological Science, 11, 323.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Prescott, C. A. (2004). Using the Mplus computer program to estimate models for continuous and categorical data from twins. Behavior Genetics, 34, 1740.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Raine, A. (2002). Biosocial studies of antisocial and violent behavior in children and adults: A review. Journal of Abnormal Child Psychology, 30, 311326.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Rhee, S. H., Hewitt, J. K., Young, S. E., Corley, R. P., Crowley, T. J., & Stallings, M. C. (2003). Genetic and environmental influences on substance initiation, use, and problem use in adolescents. Archives of General Psychiatry, 60, 12561264.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Richmond-Rakerd, L. S., Slutske, W. S., & Piasecki, T. M. (2013). Birth cohort and sex differences in the age of gambling initiation in the United States: Evidence from the national comorbidity survey replication. International Gambling Studies, 13, 417429.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Rijsdijk, F. V., & Sham, P. C. (2002). Analytic approaches to twin data using structural equation models. Briefings in Bioinformatics, 3, 119133.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Shaffer, H. J., & Hall, M. N. (2001). Updating and refining prevalence estimates of disordered gambling behaviour in the United States and Canada. Canadian Journal of Public Health, 92, 168172.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Shanahan, M. J., & Hofer, S. M. (2005). Social context in gene–environment interactions: Retrospect and prospect. The Journals of Gerontology Series B: Psychological Sciences and Social Sciences, 60, 6576.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Slutske, W. S. (in press). Genetic and environmental contributions to risk for disordered gambling. In Heinz, A., Romanczuk-Seiferth, N., & Potenza, M. (Eds.), Gambling disorder. Springer.Google Scholar
Slutske, W. S., & Richmond-Rakerd, L. S. (2014). A closer look at the evidence for sex differences in the genetic and environmental influences on gambling in the national longitudinal study of adolescent health: From disordered to ordered gambling. Addiction, 109, 120127.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Slutske, W. S., Meier, M. H., Zhu, G., Statham, D. J., Blaszczynski, A., & Martin, N. G. (2009). The Australian twin study of gambling (OZ-GAM): Rationale, sample description, predictors of participation, and a first look at sources of individual differences in gambling involvement. Twin Research and Human Genetics, 12, 6378.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Tidwell, M. C. O., Welte, J. W., Barnes, G. M., & Dayanim, B. (2015). Gambling modes and state gambling laws: Changes from 1999 to 2011 and beyond. Gaming Law Review and Economics, 19, 1326.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Turkheimer, E. (2000). Three laws of behavior genetics and what they mean. Current Directions in Psychological Science, 9, 160164.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
U.S. Department of Health and Human Services. (2014). The health consequences of smoking: 50 years of progress. A report of the Surgeon General. Atlanta, GA: U.S. Department of Health and Human Services, Centers for Disease Control and Prevention, National Center for Chronic Disease Prevention and Health Promotion, Office on Smoking and Health.Google Scholar
Vitaro, F., Hartl, A. C., Brendgen, M., Laursen, B., Dionne, G., & Boivin, M. (2014). Genetic and environmental influences on gambling and substance use in early adolescence. Behavior Genetics, 44, 347355.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Volberg, R. A., Toce, M. T., & Gerstein, D. R. (1999). From back room to living room: Changing attitudes toward gambling. Public Perspective, 10, 813.Google Scholar
Welte, J. W., Barnes, G. M., Tidwell, M. C. O., & Hoffman, J. H. (2011). Gambling and problem gambling across the lifespan. Journal of Gambling Studies, 27, 4961.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Welte, J. W., Barnes, G. M., Tidwell, M. C. O., Hoffman, J. H., & Wieczorek, W. F. (2015). Gambling and problem gambling in the United States: Changes between 1999 and 2013. Journal of Gambling Studies, 31, 695715.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Welte, J. W., Tidwell, M. C. O., Barnes, G. M., Hoffman, J. H., & Wieczorek, W. F. (2016). The relationship between the number of types of legal gambling and the rates of gambling behaviors and problems across US states. Journal of Gambling Studies, 32, 379390.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Figure 0

TABLE 1 Assessment and Prevalence of Gambling Behaviors in the Two Cohorts

Figure 1

TABLE 2 Proportion of Variation in Any Gambling Explained by Additive (A), Shared Environmental (C), and Unique Environmental (E) Influences Among Young People in 1962 Versus 2002