Skip to main content Accessibility help
×
Home

Visual-somatosensory integration in aging: Does stimulus location really matter?

  • JEANNETTE R. MAHONEY (a1), CUILING WANG (a1) (a2), KRISTINA DUMAS (a3) and ROEE HOLTZER (a1) (a3)

Abstract

Individuals are constantly bombarded by sensory stimuli across multiple modalities that must be integrated efficiently. Multisensory integration (MSI) is said to be governed by stimulus properties including space, time, and magnitude. While there is a paucity of research detailing MSI in aging, we have demonstrated that older adults reveal the greatest reaction time (RT) benefit when presented with simultaneous visual-somatosensory (VS) stimuli. To our knowledge, the differential RT benefit of visual and somatosensory stimuli presented within and across spatial hemifields has not been investigated in aging. Eighteen older adults (Mean = 74 years; 11 female), who were determined to be non-demented and without medical or psychiatric conditions that may affect their performance, participated in this study. Participants received eight randomly presented stimulus conditions (four unisensory and four multisensory) and were instructed to make speeded foot-pedal responses as soon as they detected any stimulation, regardless of stimulus type and location of unisensory inputs. Results from a linear mixed effect model, adjusted for speed of processing and other covariates, revealed that RTs to all multisensory pairings were significantly faster than those elicited to averaged constituent unisensory conditions (p < 0.01). Similarly, race model violation did not differ based on unisensory spatial location (p = 0.41). In summary, older adults demonstrate significant VS multisensory RT effects to stimuli both within and across spatial hemifields.

Copyright

Corresponding author

Address correspondence to: Jeannette R. Mahoney, Albert Einstein College of Medicine, Yeshiva University, Bronx, NY 10461. E-mail: Jeannette.Mahoney@einstein.yu.edu

References

Hide All
Beck, A.T., Epstein, N., Brown, G. & Steer, R.A. (1988). An inventory for measuring clinical anxiety: Psychometric properties. Journal of Consulting & Clinical Psychology 56, 893897.
Buschke, H., Kuslansky, G., Katz, M., Stewart, W.F., Sliwinski, M.J., Eckholdt, H.M. & Lipton, R.B. (1999). Screening for dementia with the memory impairment screen. Neurology 52, 231238.
Colonius, H. & Diederich, A. (2006). The race model inequality: Interpreting a geometric measure of the amount of violation. Psychological Review 113, 148154.
Diederich, A. & Colonius, H. (2004). Bimodal and trimodal multisensory enhancement: Effects of stimulus onset and intensity on reaction time. Perception and Psychophysics 66, 13881404.
Eriksen, C.W., Goettl, B., St James, J.D. & Fournier, L.R. (1989). Processing redundant signals: Coactivation, divided attention, or what?. Perception and Psychophysics 45, 356370.
Fort, A., Delpuech, C., Pernier, J. & Giard, M.H. (2002). Early auditory-visual interactions in human cortex during nonredundant target identification. Brain Research Cognitive Brain Research 14, 2030.
Foxe, J.J., Wylie, G.R., Martinez, A., Schroeder, C.E., Javitt, D.C., Guilfoyle, D., Ritter, W. & Murray, M.M. (2002). Auditory-somatosensory multisensory processing in auditory association cortex: An fMRI study. Journal of Neurophysiology 88, 540543.
Freiherr, J., Lundström, J.N., Habel, U. & Reetz, K. (2013). Multisensory integration mechanisms during aging. Frontiers in Human Neuroscience 7, 863.
Galvin, J.E., Roe, C.M., Powlishta, K.K., Coats, M.A., Muich, S.J., Grant, E., Miller, J.P., Storandt, M. & Morris, J.C. (2005). The AD8: A brief informant interview to detect dementia. Neurology 65, 559564.
Galvin, J.E., Roe, C.M., Xiong, C. & Morris, J.C. (2006). Validity and reliability of the AD8 informant interview in dementia. Neurology 67, 19421948.
Giard, M.H. & Peronnet, F. (1999). Auditory-visual integration during multimodal object recognition in humans: A behavioral and electrophysiological study. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience 11, 473490.
Gondan, M., Lange, K., Rosler, F. & Roder, B. (2004). The redundant target effect is affected by modality switch costs. Psychonomic Bulletin and Review 11, 307313.
Harrington, L.K. & Peck, C.K. (1998). Spatial disparity affects visual-auditory interactions in human sensorimotor processing. Experimental Brain Research 122, 247252.
Holmes, N.P. (2007). The law of inverse effectiveness in neurons and behaviour: Multisensory integration versus normal variability. Neuropsychologia 45, 33403345.
Holtzer, R., Friedman, R., Lipton, R.B., Katz, M., Xue, X. & Verghese, J. (2007). The relationship between specific cognitive functions and falls in aging. Neuropsychology 21, 540548.
Holtzer, R., Verghese, J., Wang, C., Hall, C.B. & Lipton, R.B. (2008). Within-person across-neuropsychological test variability and incident dementia. The Journal of the American Medical Association 300, 823830.
Holtzer, R., Verghese, J., Xue, X. & Lipton, R.B. (2006). Cognitive processes related to gait velocity: Results from the Einstein Aging study. Neuropsychology 20, 215223.
Holtzer, R., Wang, C. & Verghese, J. (2014). Performance variance on walking while talking tasks: Theory, findings, and clinical implications. Age (Dordrecht) 36, 373–81.
Hugenschmidt, C.E., Mozolic, J.L. & Laurienti, P.J. (2009). Suppression of multisensory integration by modality-specific attention in aging. Neuroreport 20, 349353.
Kinchla, R. (1974). Detecting target elements in multielement arrays: A confusability model. Perception and Psychophysics 15, 149158.
Laird, N.M. & Ware, J.H. (1982). Random-effects models for longitudinal data. Biometrics 38, 963974.
Laurienti, P.J., Burdette, J.H., Maldjian, J.A. & Wallace, M.T. (2006). Enhanced multisensory integration in older adults. Neurobiology of Aging 27, 11551163.
Mahoney, J.R., Li, P.C., Oh-Park, M., Verghese, J. & Holtzer, R. (2011). Multisensory integration across the senses in young and old adults. Brain Research 1426, 4353.
Mahoney, J.R., Verghese, J., Dumas, K., Wang, C. & Holtzer, R. (2012). The effect of multisensory cues on attention in aging. Brain Research 1472, 6373.
Mahoney, J.R., Verghese, J., Goldin, Y., Lipton, R. & Holtzer, R. (2010). Alerting, orienting, and executive attention in older adults. Journal of the International Neuropsychological Society 16, 877889.
Masur, D.M., Sliwinski, M., Lipton, R.B., Blau, A.D. & Crystal, H.A. (1994). Neuropsychological prediction of dementia and the absence of dementia in healthy elderly persons. Neurology 44, 14271432.
Meredith, M.A., Nemitz, J.W. & Stein, B.E. (1987). Determinants of multisensory integration in superior colliculus neurons. I. Temporal factors. The Journal of Neuroscience 7, 32153229.
Meredith, M.A. & Stein, B.E. (1986). Visual, auditory, and somatosensory convergence on cells in superior colliculus results in multisensory integration. Journal of Neurophysiology 56, 640662.
Meredith, M.A. & Stein, B.E. (1996). Spatial determinants of multisensory integration in cat superior colliculus neurons. Journal of Neurophysiology 75, 18431857.
Miller, J. (1982). Divided attention: Evidence for coactivation with redundant signals. Cognitive Psychology 14, 247279.
Miller, J. (1986). Timecourse of coactivation in bimodal divided attention. Perception and Psychophysics 40, 331343.
Molholm, S., Ritter, W., Murray, M.M., Javitt, D.C., Schroeder, C.E. & Foxe, J.J. (2002). Multisensory auditory-visual interactions during early sensory processing in humans: A high-density electrical mapping study. Brain Research Cognitive Brain Research 14, 115128.
Mordkoff, J.T. & Yantis, S. (1991). An interactive race model of divided attention. Journal of Experimental Psychology Human Perception and Performance 17, 520538.
Murray, M.M., Molholm, S., Michel, C.M., Heslenfeld, D.J., Ritter, W., Javitt, D.C., Schroeder, C.E. & Foxe, J.J. (2005). Grabbing your ear: rapid auditory-somatosensory multisensory interactions in low-level sensory cortices are not constrained by stimulus alignment. Cerebral Cortex 15, 963974.
Oldfield, R.C. (1971). The assessment and analysis of handedness: The Edinburgh inventory. Neuropsychologia 9, 97113.
Otto, T.U., Dassy, B. & Mamassian, P. (2013). Principles of multisensory behavior. Journal of Neuroscience, 33, 7463–74.
Otto, T.U. & Mamassian, P. (2012). Noise and correlations in parallel perceptual decision making. Current Biology 22, 13911396.
Pavani, F., Spence, C. & Driver, J. (2000). Visual capture of touch: Out-of-the-body experiences with rubber gloves. Psychological Science 11, 353359.
Peiffer, A.M., Mozolic, J.L., Hugenschmidt, C.E. & Laurienti, P.J. (2007). Age-related multisensory enhancement in a simple audiovisual detection task. Neuroreport 18, 10771081.
Ross, L.A., Saint-Amour, D., Leavitt, V.M., Javitt, D.C. & Foxe, J.J. (2007). Do you see what I am saying? exploring visual enhancement of speech comprehension in noisy environments. Cerebral Cortex 17, 11471153.
Salthouse, T.A. (1985). Speed of Behavior and its Implication for Cognition in Handbook of the Psychology of Aging, ed. Birren, J.E. & Schaie, K.W., pp. 400426. New York: Van Nostrand Reinhold Co.
Schurmann, M., Kolev, V., Menzel, K. & Yordanova, J. (2002). Spatial coincidence modulates interaction between visual and somatosensory evoked potentials. Neuroreport 13, 779783.
Sliwinski, M., Buschke, H., Stewart, W.F., Masur, D. & Lipton, R.B. (1997). The effect of dementia risk factors on comparative and diagnostic selective reminding norms. Journal of the International Neuropsychological Society 3, 317326.
Stein, B.E., Huneycutt, W.S. & Meredith, M.A. (1988). Neurons and behavior: The same rules of multisensory integration apply. Brain Research 448, 355358.
Stein, B.E., Magalhaes-Castro, B. & Kruger, L. (1975). Superior colliculus: Visuotopic-somatotopic overlap. Science 189, 224226.
Stein, B.E. & Meredith, M.A. (1990). Multisensory integration. Neural and behavioral solutions for dealing with stimuli from different sensory modalities. Annals of the New York Academy of Sciences 608, 5165; discussion, 65–70.
Stein, B.E., Meredith, M.A. (1993). The Merging of the Senses. Cambridge, Massachusetts: MIT Press.
Stein, B.E. & Wallace, M.T. (1996). Comparisons of cross-modality integration in midbrain and cortex. Progress in Brain Research, 112, 289–99.
Stephen, J.M., Knoefel, J.E., Adair, J., Hart, B. & Aine, C.J. (2010). Aging-related changes in auditory and visual integration measured with MEG. Neuroscience Letters 484, 7680.
Stern, Y., Habeck, C., Moeller, J., Scarmeas, N., Anderson, K.E., Hilton, H.J., Flynn, J., Sackeim, H. & van Heertum, R. (2005). Brain networks associated with cognitive reserve in healthy young and old adults. Cerebral Cortex 15, 394402.
Teder-Sälejärvi, W.A., Di Russo, F., McDonald, J.J. & Hillyard, S.A. (2005). Effects of spatial congruity on audio-visual multimodal integration. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience 17, 13961409.
Verghese, J., Wang, C., Lipton, R.B., Holtzer, R. & Xue, X. (2007). Quantitative gait dysfunction and risk of cognitive decline and dementia. Journal of Neurology Neurosurgery and Psychiatry 78, 929935.
Wallace, M.T., Wilkinson, L.K. & Stein, B.E. (1996). Representation and integration of multiple sensory inputs in primate superior colliculus. Journal of Neurophysiology 76, 12461266.
Yesavage, J.A., Brink, T.L., Rose, T.L., Lum, O., Huang, V., Adey, M. & Leirer, V.O. (1982). Development and validation of a geriatric depression screening scale: A preliminary report. Journal of Psychiatric Research 17, 3749.

Keywords

Metrics

Full text views

Total number of HTML views: 0
Total number of PDF views: 0 *
Loading metrics...

Abstract views

Total abstract views: 0 *
Loading metrics...

* Views captured on Cambridge Core between <date>. This data will be updated every 24 hours.

Usage data cannot currently be displayed