Hostname: page-component-848d4c4894-cjp7w Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-06-22T19:19:41.087Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

Transitions between color categories mapped with a reverse Stroop task

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  06 September 2006

HANNAH E. SMITHSON
Affiliation:
Department of Psychology, Durham University, United Kingdom Institute of Ophthalmology, UCL, United Kingdom
SABAH S. KHAN
Affiliation:
Institute of Ophthalmology, UCL, United Kingdom
LINDSAY T. SHARPE
Affiliation:
Institute of Ophthalmology, UCL, United Kingdom
ANDREW STOCKMAN
Affiliation:
Institute of Ophthalmology, UCL, United Kingdom

Abstract

In the reverse Stroop task, observers are instructed to ignore the ink color in which a color word is printed (the distractor color) and to respond to the meaning of the color word (the target). Reaction times (RTs) are faster with congruent combinations when the ink color matches the word than with incongruent combinations when the ink color does not match the word. We manipulated the distracting ink color from congruent to incongruent and measured the transition from facilitation to interference. In Experiment 1, we confirmed that this transition could be assessed independently from the contextual influence of particular sets of stimuli and responses, implying that the color space in which interference and facilitation occurs is generalizable. In Experiment 2, we obtained reverse Stroop data for transitions between red and yellow, yellow and green, green and blue, and blue and red, and compared them with independent estimates of color appearance obtained by hue scaling for the same chromaticity samples. We find that the magnitude of the reverse Stroop effect can provide a reliable index of the similarity of color appearance between the distracting chromaticity and the color category represented by the target color word. Moreover, it will allow us to quantify the mapping between the chromaticity space defined at the cone photoreceptors and a cognitive color space defined at an advanced level of neural processing.

Type
CHROMATIC CODING
Copyright
© 2006 Cambridge University Press

Access options

Get access to the full version of this content by using one of the access options below. (Log in options will check for institutional or personal access. Content may require purchase if you do not have access.)

References

REFERENCES

Abramov, I., Gordon, J., & Chan, H. (1990). Using hue scaling to specify color appearance and to derive color differences. Proceedings of SPIE 1250, 4051.Google Scholar
Berlin, B. & Kay, P. (1969). Basic Color Terms: Their Universality and Evolution. University of California Press.
Boynton, R.M. & Gordon, J. (1965). Bezold-Brucke hue shift measured by color-naming technique. Journal of the Optical Society of America 55, 7886.Google Scholar
Boynton, R.M. & Olson, C.X. (1990). Salience of chromatic basic color terms confirmed by 3 measures. Vision Research 30, 13111317.Google Scholar
Buckelmuller, I., Cardinal, K., & Kiper, D.C. (2002). The categorization of colors measured with the Stroop effect [Abstract]. Journal of Vision 2, 153a.Google Scholar
Cardinal, K.S., Buckelmuller, I., & Kiper, D.C. (2003). The chromatic tuning of the Stroop effect. Unpublished manuscript. Institution: Institute of Neuroinformatics, University of Zurich, Switzerland.
de Weert, C.M.M., Snoeren, P.R., Puts, M.J.H., & te Pas, S.F. (1999). Reverse Stroop interference effect [Abstract]. Perception 28(S).Google Scholar
Derrington, A.M., Krauskopf, J., & Lennie, P. (1984). Chromatic mechanisms in lateral geniculate-nucleus of macaque. Journal of Physiology (London) 357, 241265.Google Scholar
De Valois, R.L., De Valois, K.K., Switkes, E., & Mahon, L. (1997). Hue scaling of isoluminant and cone-specific lights. Vision Research 37, 885897.Google Scholar
Durgin, F.H. (2000). The reverse Stroop effect. Psychonomic Bulletin & Review 7, 121125.Google Scholar
Durgin, F.H. (2003). Translation and competition among internal representations in a reverse Stroop effect. Perception & Psychophysics 65, 367378.Google Scholar
Dyer, F.N. (1973). Stroop phenomenon and its use in study of perceptual, cognitive, and response processes. Memory and Cognition 1, 106120.Google Scholar
Dyer, F.N. & Severance, L.J. (1972). Effects of irrelevant colors on reading of color names: A controlled replication of the reversed Stroop effect. Psychonomic Science 28, 336338.Google Scholar
Gegenfurtner, K.R. & Kiper, D.C. (2003). Color vision. Annual Review of Neuroscience 26, 181206.Google Scholar
Gumenik, W.E. & Glass, R. (1970). Effects of reducing readability of words in Stroop color-word test. Psychonomic Science 20, 247248.Google Scholar
Hering, E. (1920). Grundzuge der Lehre vom Lichtsinn [Outlines of a theory of the light sense]. Berlin: Springer.
Hurvich, L.M. & Jameson, D. (1957). An opponent-process theory of color-vision. Psychological Review 64, 384404.Google Scholar
Kiper, D.C., Buckelmuller, I., & Cardinal, K. (2002). The categorisation of colours measured with the Stroop effect [Abstract]. Perception 31(S).Google Scholar
Klopfer, D.S. (1996). Stroop interference and color-word similarity. Psychological Science 7, 150157.Google Scholar
Komatsu, H., Ideura, Y., Kaji, S., & Yamane, S. (1992). Color selectivity of neurons in the inferior temporal cortex of the awake macaque monkey. Journal of Neuroscience 12, 408424.Google Scholar
Krauskopf, J. (1999). Higher order color mechanisms. In Color Vision, eds. Gegenfurtner, K.R. & Sharpe, L.T., pp. 303316. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.
Lindsay, D.S. & Jacoby, L.L. (1994). Stroop process dissociations—the relationship between facilitation and interference. Journal of Experimental Psychology 20, 219234.Google Scholar
MacLeod, C.M. (1991). Half a century of research on the Stroop effect—an integrative review. Psychology Bulletin 109, 163203.Google Scholar
McKeefry, D.J., Parry, N.R., & Murray, I.J. (2003). Simple reaction times in color space: The influence of chromaticity, contrast, and cone opponency. Investigative Ophthalmology and Visual Science 44, 22672276.Google Scholar
Melara, R.D. & Mounts, J.R.W. (1993). Selective attention to Stroop dimensions—effects of base-line discriminability, response-mode, and practice. Memory and Cognition 21, 627645.Google Scholar
Seymour, P.H. (1977). Conceptual encoding and locus of the Stroop effect. Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology 29, 245265.Google Scholar
Smithson, H.E. & Mollon, J.D. (2004). Is the S-opponent chromatic sub-system sluggish? Vision Research 44, 29192929.Google Scholar
Stroop, J.R. (1935). Studies of interference in serial verbal reactions. Journal of Experimental Psychology 18, 643662.Google Scholar
Sugg, M.J. & McDonald, J.E. (1994). Time-course of inhibition in color-response and word-response versions of the Stroop task. Journal of Experimental Psychology 20, 647675.Google Scholar
Virzi, R.A. & Egeth, H.E. (1985). Toward a translational model of Stroop interference. Memory and Cognition 13, 304319.Google Scholar
Webster, M.A., Malkoc, G., Bilson, A.C., & Webster, S.M. (2002). Color contrast and contextual influences on color appearance. Journal of Vision 2, 505519.Google Scholar