Hostname: page-component-8448b6f56d-c47g7 Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-04-16T21:22:43.762Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

INTRODUCTION

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  02 August 2019

Andrea Révész*
Affiliation:
University College London
Marije Michel
Affiliation:
University of Groningen and Lancaster University
*
*Correspondence concerning this article should be addressed to Andrea Révész, UCL Institute of Education, University College London, 20 Bedford Way, London, WC1H 0AL, UK. E-mail: a.revesz@ucl.ac.uk
Rights & Permissions [Opens in a new window]

Abstract

Type
State of the Scholarship
Creative Commons
Creative Common License - CCCreative Common License - BY
This is an Open Access article, distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution licence (http://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/4.0/), which permits unrestricted re-use, distribution, and reproduction in any medium, provided the original work is properly cited.
Copyright
Copyright © Cambridge University Press 2019

The last three decades have seen a significant advancement in describing and understanding the processes involved in second language (L2) writing (for recent reviews, see Cumming, Reference Cumming, Manchón and Matsuda2016; Polio, Reference Polio, Gass and Mackey2012; Roca de Larios, Nicolás-Conesa, & Coyle, Reference Roca de Larios, Nicolás-Conesa, Coyle, Manchón and Matsuda2016). Much of the existing research has been cognitive in orientation and concerned with capturing the online behaviors of L2 writers (i.e., directly observable characteristics of the writing process such as pausing) and the cognitive operations that underlie L2 written production (e.g., planning, linguistic encoding). In the cognitive paradigm, the motivation for studying L2 writing processes comes from two main sources. First, research into L2 writing processes is important for theory-building purposes. When constructing and assessing theoretical models of L2 writing, it is not sufficient to look at the linguistic product of the composing process. To avoid the risk of construct underrepresentation (Norris & Ortega, Reference Norris, Ortega, Doughty and Long2003), it is also crucial to provide evidence about the behaviors of L2 writers and the cognitive activities underlying those behaviors. Second, it is believed that information about what L2 writers do when they compose a text can yield useful insights for L2 instruction and assessment. There is increasing evidence, for example, that understanding the processes in which good L2 writers engage may help identify what strategies may be beneficial to teach (e.g., Olson & Land, Reference Olson and Land2007; van Gelderen, Ootsdam, & van Schooten, Reference van Gelderen, Oostdam and van Schooten2011). Knowledge about how observable writing behaviors relate to underlying cognitive processes could also help diagnose sources of writing difficulty, and thereby enable educators to adjust instruction to better meet their students’ needs. Last but not least, research into L2 writing processes can also provide instructors with insights into the language learning potential of writing, that is, how the act of written production may foster cognitive processes, which are assumed to facilitate L2 development (Manchón & Roca de Larios, Reference Manchón, Roca de Larios, Alcón Soler and Safont Jordà2007a). In short, L2 writing process research may inform L2 instruction and assessment by advancing our understanding of both the learning-to-write and writing-to-learn dimensions of L2 writing (Manchón, Reference Manchón2011).

Over the past decades, a variety of methods have been employed to explore L2 writing processes, with a view to informing cognitive models of writing and generating insights for L2 instruction. The aim of this special issue is to highlight methodological innovations in the field. We also intend to demonstrate how adopting new data-collection technologies and approaches to data analysis and combining these in innovative ways can generate new and more valid information about the L2 writing process and open up new avenues for research. This focus on methodological issues appears timely not only in the context of L2 writing research but also against the backdrop of increasing interest in and growing awareness of methodological considerations in the larger field of L2 research (Marsden & Plonsky, Reference Marsden, Plonsky, Gudmestad and Edmonds2018). This introduction provides a brief review of previous theoretical and related empirical work on L2 writing processes; discusses the potential of various methods, both more traditional and novel, to gain insights about the cognitive activities of L2 writers; and finally introduces the five empirical studies included in the special issue with a focus on their methodological contribution.

THEORETICAL PERSPECTIVES AND EMPIRICAL INSIGHTS ON THE L2 WRITING PROCESS

Much of the research into L2 writing processes has been inspired by cognitive models originally developed to explain the process of first language (L1) writing. Initially, Hayes and Flower’s (Reference Hayes, Flower, Gregg and Steinberg1980) model was mainly selected as the theoretical basis for L2 investigations. Based on research in cognitive psychology, this model describes writing as a recursive, nonlinear process of problem solving, including three subprocesses: planning, which entails the generation of ideas, organization, and setting of goals; translation, involving the conversion of plans and thoughts into linguistic form; and reviewing, which comprises editing and reading the evolving text. These processes interact with the task environment, defined by the task instructions and the text written so far; and draw on information retrieved from the writer’s long-term memory, such as stored plans for writing and knowledge of the topic, audience, grammar, and writing conventions. This complex set of operations is coordinated and controlled by the monitor. Informed by this model, a principal focus of L1 and L2 writing research was to identify differences in the behaviors and cognitive processes of expert and novice writers. Researchers studying L2 users (e.g., Cumming, Reference Cumming1989; Raimes, Reference Raimes1987; Victori, Reference Victori1999) revealed that, similar to L1 users, experienced L2 writers plan longer and make more elaborate plans than their less experienced counterparts. Also, experts revise more, and focus on both global and local issues, whereas novices tend to be more concerned with local problems.

The distinction between skilled and less skilled writers is also central to Bereiter and Scardamalia’s (1987) model, a theoretical framework that was adopted by some ensuing studies into L2 writing processes. Bereiter and Scardamalia distinguish a basic knowledge-telling approach, in which writers share their knowledge about a topic through direct retrieval of content from long-term memory, from an expert knowledge-transforming approach, in which writers do not only access ready-made information from memory to tell knowledge but also shape and revise it to correspond to their rhetorical functions in the emerging text. The few L2 studies that were conceptualized in terms of Bereiter and Scardamalia’s model confirmed that, depending on their level of expertise, L2 writers indeed demonstrate knowledge-telling and knowledge-transforming behaviors to a differential degree (e.g., Cumming, Reference Cumming, Belcher and Braine1995; Danzak, Reference Danzak2011).

A shared feature of Flower and Hayes’ (Reference Hayes, Flower, Gregg and Steinberg1980) and Bereiter and Scardamalia’s (1987) models is an emphasis on higher-level reflective thinking processes, with the implication that L1 and L2 writing processes are similar in terms of setting goals and generating content (Galbraith, Reference Galbraith2009). Neither of the models, however, addresses the difficulties that may arise in the process of converting ideas into language, and how these might influence the content-generation process. Subsequent cognitive models of writing have placed greater emphasis on the cognitive operations involved in translating ideas into text, in light of empirical evidence demonstrating that text production is often an effortful and nonautomatic process, especially for L2 writers (Roca de Larios, Murphy, & Manchón, Reference Roca de Larios, Murphy and Manchón1999). In their later work, Hayes and colleagues (Chenoweth & Hayes, Reference Chenoweth and Hayes2003; Hayes, Reference Hayes, Levy and Ransdell1996, Reference Hayes, Beard, Myhill, Riley and Nystrand2009) include more elaborate descriptions of linguistic encoding processes and assume more active interaction between translation and planning.

Likewise, Kellogg’s (Reference Kellogg, Levy and Ransdell1996) cognitive model, the theoretical basis of several recent L2 investigations (e.g., Kormos, Reference Kormos2011; Révész, Kourtali, & Mazgutova, Reference Révész, Kourtali and Mazgutova2017; Révész, Michel, & Lee, Reference Révész, Michel and Lee2017), provides detailed predictions about linguistic encoding processes, including lexical retrieval, syntactic encoding, and expression of cohesion. These are assumed to interact in a recursive manner with the processes of planning content and organization, execution (i.e., production of text through motor movements during handwriting or typing), and monitoring of the developing text. There is increasing empirical research indicating that L2 cognitive writing processes are well aligned with the writing stages and substages posited by Kellogg (Révész, Kourtali, et al., Reference Révész, Kourtali and Mazgutova2017; Révész, Michel, et al., Reference Révész, Michel and Lee2017). However, less empirical evidence is available about when the stages and substages proposed occur, and how long they last during the writing process. In general, studies of writing processes have tended to examine the overall frequency and proportion of cognitive activities without considering when the cognitive activities occurred (Rijlaarsdam & van den Bergh, Reference Rijlaarsdam, Van Den Bergh, Levy and Ransdell1996), even though writing is assumed to be a dynamic and recursive process both in Kellogg’s and earlier models of writing. The few L2 investigation that have addressed the temporal dimension of writing indicate that, indeed, L2 writers engage in specific writing processes and subprocesses to a differential extent at various writing stages (e.g., beginning, middle, end), and the distribution of cognitive activities over time depends on the writers’ level of proficiency (Barkaoui, Reference Barkaoui2016; Manchón & Roca de Larios, Reference Manchón and Roca de Larios2007b; Roca de Larios, Manchón, Murphy, & Marín, Reference Roca de Larios, Manchón, Murphy and Marín2008; Tillema, Reference Tillema2012) and the type of writing task (Barkaoui, Reference Barkaoui2016).

The dynamic nature of writing is also captured in Galbraith’s (Reference Galbraith2009) model of writing. This model challenges classical writing models (Bereiter & Scardamalia, 1987; Flower & Hayes, Reference Hayes, Flower, Gregg and Steinberg1980) on the grounds that they treat writing exclusively as a top-down, explicit process, where text production is assigned the passive role of translating previously formed ideas and manipulating existing content (Galbraith, Reference Galbraith2009). Instead, inspired by connectionist models and empirical evidence that writing can affect thought, he proposed a dual-process model, which entails two types of processes: knowledge retrieval and knowledge constituting. As in the classical models, the knowledge-retrieval processes are explicit and involve retrieving existing knowledge from long-term memory, which may be reorganized in working memory to meet rhetorical aims. Knowledge-constituting processes, however, are implicit and can produce new content and ideas through synthesis, potentially leading to novel understandings during text production. According to Galbraith, effective writers rely on and can switch between these two conflicting processes. L2 writers, however, are expected to rely less on the knowledge-constituting system, given that they can produce shorter bursts of language due to their limited L2 proficiency (Chenoweth & Hayes Reference Chenoweth and Hayes2003), and bursts are the basis for developing new ideas through implicit memory. Galbraith’s model received some empirical confirmation in the context of L1 writing research (Galbraith, Reference Galbraith, Torrance and Galbraith1999; Galbraith, Torrance, & Hallam, Reference Galbraith, Torrance and Hallam2006), but little is known about its applicability to L2 writing.

Several more recent writing models (e.g., Galbraith, Reference Galbraith2009; Hayes, Reference Hayes, Levy and Ransdell1996; Kellogg, Reference Kellogg, Levy and Ransdell1996, Reference Kellogg2001) posit a key role for working memory. Drawing on Baddeley’s (Reference Baddeley1986) multicomponent framework, Kellogg made explicit predictions regarding how the various components may affect the writing process. According to Baddeley’s original model, working memory comprises three main components: a central executive and two domain-specific subsystems: the phonological loop and the visual-spatial sketchpad. In Kellogg’s view, the central executive or supervisory system, is involved at all stages of the writing process, as multiple operations are happening in parallel. The phonological loop, which is responsible for the temporary retention and manipulation of verbal information, is called upon during translation and monitoring as these stages involve the processing of verbal material. The visual-spatial sketchpad, specialized for storing and handling visual and spatial information, is required when writers engage in planning and monitoring. Planning content involves generating prelinguistic ideas that often entail images, whereas monitoring necessitates maintaining an image of where different parts of the text are located. The small number of studies that have examined whether working memory is implicated in L2 writing confirmed a role for various aspects of working memory. Kormos and Sáfár (Reference Kormos and Sáfár2008) and Adams and Guillot (Reference Adams and Guillot2008) observed a positive link between phonological short-term memory and text quality, and Révész, Michel, and Lee (Reference Révész, Michel and Lee2017) found that phonological short-term memory, the visual spatial sketchpad, and executive functioning were all related to several aspects of text quality or type of online writing behavior in the direction predicted by Kellogg (Reference Kellogg, Levy and Ransdell1996, Reference Kellogg2001).

Working memory, a construct interrelated with the processes of attention and awareness, is also implicated in theoretical perspectives that have been adopted as a basis for research exploring the language learning potential of writing, the consideration that L2 writing can serve as a venue for language learning (Manchón & Roca de Larios, Reference Manchón, Roca de Larios, Alcón Soler and Safont Jordà2007a). This line of research has mainly been conceptualized drawing on Schmidt’s (Reference Schmidt1994, Reference Schmidt and Robinson2001) Noticing Hypothesis and Swain’s (Reference Swain, Gass and Madden1985, Reference Swain, Cook and Seidlhofer1995) Output Hypothesis. Schmidt claims that attention is a necessary condition for L2 development; and noticing, defined as focal attention accompanied with awareness, further enhances the chance of new L2 features being encoded and then learned. Building on Schmidt’s work, one tenet of the Output Hypothesis is that producing output induces learners to notice gaps in their interlanguage knowledge. In Swain’s view, output production additionally provides a platform for testing hypotheses about the L2, and can serve as a point of departure for metalinguistic reflection. Reflecting these theoretical ideas, writing has been proposed as an optimal platform for developing knowledge of and gaining control over the use of L2 form-meaning mappings (Cumming, Reference Cumming1990; Manchón & Roca de Larios, Reference Manchón, Roca de Larios, Alcón Soler and Safont Jordà2007a). By now, the presumed language learning benefits of writing have received much empirical confirmation (Manchón, Reference Manchón2011), with most studies employing the concept of language-related episode (LRE; Swain & Lapkin, Reference Swain and Lapkin1995) as a unit of analysis to operationalize awareness. Previous studies in this area, however, have predominantly focused on collaborative writing (e.g., Storch, Reference Storch2005); less research is available about the extent to which language learning opportunities arise during the process of individual writing.

INVESTIGATING L2 WRITING PROCESSES: METHODOLOGICAL ISSUES AND POSSIBILITIES

With the aim of describing L2 writing processes and thereby informing L2 writing models and instruction, researchers have employed various methods, ranging from traditional techniques such as verbal reports, questionnaires, and interviews to the more recent use of state-of-the-art tools such as keystroke logging and eye-tracking. In reflection of current methodological practices and new developments in the field, the remainder of this section focuses on three techniques: verbal reports, keystroke logging, and eye-tracking.

To date, the majority of cognitively oriented research into L2 writing processes has relied on verbal reports, such as the think-aloud and stimulated recall procedures. Think-alouds, or concurrent verbal reports, involve participants in describing their thoughts while they are in the process of composing. In the area of L2 writing, this technique has been employed to examine a number of topics, including the cognitive processes in which writers engage (e.g., Manchón, Roca de Larios, & Murphy, 2009), links between the writing process and product (e.g., Van Weijen, Van den Bergh, Rijlaarsdam, & Sanders, Reference Van Weijen, Van den Bergh, Rijlaarsdam and Sanders2008), the use of L1 in L2 writing (e.g., Wang & Wen, 2002), and the strategy use of L2 writers (e.g., Yang & Shi, 2003). Although studies using think-alouds have contributed considerably to our current understanding about L2 writing processes, the technique has been criticized because the act of thinking aloud may lead to reactivity and thus jeopardize construct validity. In other words, concurrent verbalization of one’s thoughts while composing may alter the writing process and/or the resulting writing product, leading to incorrect conclusions. The potential danger posed by reactivity has been scrutinized in several studies of writing (e.g., Yang, Hu, & Zhang, 2014; Yanguas & Lado, 2012), but so far, the findings are inconclusive (Polio & Freedman, Reference Polio and Freedman2017). Another possible threat associated with concurrent verbal reports concerns veridicality, that is, the incapability of verbal reports to capture all of the participants’ thought processes. Indeed, there are individual differences in the ability to think aloud (Barkaoui, Reference Barkaoui2011), and some cognitive processes are not accessible to awareness and thus remain unavailable for verbalization (Polio & Freedman, Reference Polio and Freedman2017).

Like think-alouds, stimulated recall protocols are also widely used to investigate L2 writing processes. This technique intends to tap the thoughts that participants had during writing by employing a posteriori recall sessions (Gass & Mackey, Reference Gass and Mackey2016), using some tangible reminder (e.g., the text produced, video- or screen-recording of the writing process) to stimulate participants’ recall. L2 researchers have utilized this technique, for example, to explore changes in writing processes over time (Sasaki, Reference Sasaki2004), individual differences in writing processes (Bosher, Reference Bosher1998), strategies adopted in L2 writing (DeSilva & Graham, Reference De Silva and Graham2015), and the impact of task complexity manipulations on writing processes (e.g., Révész, Kourtali, & Mazgutova, Reference Révész, Kourtali and Mazgutova2017). As compared to concurrent verbalization, stimulated recall has the advantage of posing reduced threat to reactivity, but it carries a higher risk in terms of veridicality. Due to memory decay, it is likely that participants cannot provide fully accurate and complete reports of the cognitive activities in which they previously engaged. However, a careful implementation of the technique can arguably mitigate the influence of potential issues with veridicality (Gass & Mackey, Reference Gass and Mackey2016).

Probably due to the limitations associated with verbal reports, more recently researchers have begun to use keystroke logging, alone or in combination with verbal reports, to capture real-time L2 writing processes. Keystroke-logging programs record the writers’ keystrokes and mouse movements without interrupting the composing process, and the resulting log files provide detailed information about concurrent writing processes, such as fluency, pausing, and revision behaviors (Lindgren & Sullivan, Reference Lindgren and Sullivan2019; Van Waes, Leijten, Lindgren, & Wengelin, Reference Van Waes, Leijten, Lindgren, Wengelin, MacArthur, Graham and Fitzgerald2015). Although a relatively new technique, a few L2 studies have already employed keystroke logging to examine the effects of proficiency (Barkaoui, Reference Barkaoui2016), task type (Barkaoui, Reference Barkaoui2016; Spelman-Miller, Reference Spelman Miller2000), and task complexity (Révész, Kourtali, & Mazgutova, Reference Révész, Kourtali and Mazgutova2017) on L2 writing behaviors; to compare the behaviors of L1 and L2 writers (Stevenson, Schoonen, & De Glopper, Reference Stevenson, Schoonen and De Glopper2006; Thorson, Reference Thorson2000; Tillema, Reference Tillema2012; Van Waes & Leijten, Reference Van Waes and Leijten2015); and to assess longitudinal changes in L2 writing behaviors (Spelman Miller, Lindgren, & Sullivan, Reference Spelman Miller, Lindgren and Sullivan2008). Being unobtrusive and providing real-time data, keystroke logging appears to offer several benefits over verbal protocols. Data obtained with the help of keystroke logging, however, has the disadvantage of affording no direct evidence about the cognitive operations of L2 writers. In addition, keystroke logs, unlike verbal protocols, provide no information about the reading activities in which L2 writers are involved.

A way to address this limitation is to combine keystroke logging with eye-tracking methodology. This technique can be used to record the writer’s moment-to-moment eye gaze or eye fixations they make while they are engaged in composing the text. The assumption underlying eye-tracking methodology is that the focus, order, and length of eye fixations reflect individuals’ attentional allocation in processing visual information (Reichle, Reference Reichle2006). Thus, the joint use of eye-tracking and keystroke logging allows for recording not only physical text production processes but also viewing behaviors during writing. While eye-tracking has been employed in several L1 writing studies (e.g., de Smet, Leijten, & Van Waes, Reference de Smet, Leijten and Van Waes2018; Johansson, Wengelin, Johansson, & Holmqvist, Reference Johansson, Wengelin, Johansson and Holmqvist2010), this technique is just beginning to be applied to investigate L2 writing processes. In one of the first studies to use this technique, Révész, Michel, and Lee (Reference Révész, Michel and Lee2017) examined L2 writers’ viewing behaviors while pausing, with a focus on whether their eye fixations during pauses remained within the word, clause, sentence, or paragraph preceding the point of inscription. The researchers also triangulated the eye-tracking data with recordings of keystroke logs and stimulated recall comments. Similarly, Gánem-Gutiérrez and Gilmore (Reference Gánem-Gutiérrez and Gilmore2018) used eye-tracking methodology together with digital screen capture and stimulated recall protocols to study L2 writing processes. In both studies, the combination of methods allowed for obtaining a more complete picture of the writing process than employing any of the methods alone would have afforded. Indeed, it would appear that a promising approach to overcoming the limitations of the various methods is to use multiple data-collection techniques and triangulate the data obtained, thereby increasing the likelihood that valid inferences are made. This is also the approach that has been employed by several contributions to this special issue, as explained in the following section.

THE SPECIAL ISSUE

The articles in this special issue either adopt new technologies, employ novel analytical approaches, and/or combine new and more traditional methods in innovative ways to examine the L2 writing process. The articles included demonstrate how the use of novel techniques and analytical procedures, alone and/or together, can foster and lead to a more in-depth, detailed, and valid understanding of the L2 writing process.

The first empirical study by López-Serrano, Roca de Larios, and Manchón employed concurrent verbal protocols to explore the language learning potential of individual argumentative writing tasks. The methodological contribution of this research lies in the development of a theoretically motivated and empirically based coding system to categorize language-related reflections of L2 writers. Drawing on the idea that L2 writing is an important site for language development (Manchón, Reference Manchón2011), the specific aim of López-Serrano, Roca de Larios, and Manchón was to identify and characterize the processes in which individual writers engage when composing L2 texts. The dataset comprised transcriptions of 21 EFL writers’ think-aloud protocols, which was coded for LREs (Swain & Lapkin, Reference Swain and Lapkin1995). Then, the LREs were further reconceptualized and categorized according to five dimensions: linguistic focus, resolution, strategy use, depth of processing, and orientation. López-Serrano, Roca de Larios, and Manchón also provide examples for the various coding categories based on the dataset, but the primary outcome of the study is the coding scheme, which can be used in future research to conduct in-depth analyses of the LREs that emerge during individual L2 writing and to classify them in terms of the potential for language development they may involve.

The next contribution by Barkaoui examines L2 writers’ pausing behaviors as a window into writers’ cognitive processes. Barkaoui begins with a comprehensive literature review of previous work on pausing during writing, which is followed by an extensive introduction to keystroke logging methodology, highlighting both the advantages and challenges associated with the technique. In the second part of the article, Barkaoui illustrates how pausing data can be collected, analyzed, and interpreted. In doing so, he relies on keystroke-logging data of 68 L2 writers of English, who performed a version of the integrated and independent TOEFL iBT writing task. The study demonstrates the ways in which keystroke-logging data can be utilized to investigate the influence of factors such as proficiency, keyboarding skills, task type, and stage of writing (beginning, middle, end) on pause frequency, duration, and location. Barkaoui found that high-proficiency students tended to pause less often than low-proficiency writers. Interestingly, pauses were also found to be longer initially, probably to allow writers to plan their text in greater detail. Finally, the initial stage elicited fewer but longer pauses on the integrated task, possibly because writers were rereading the input text at the beginning of this task type.

Another empirical study by Leijten, Van Waes, Schrijver, Bernolet, and Vangehuchten also used keystroke logging to tap L2 writing processes. The researchers compared the writing performance of 280 university students, who engaged in source-based writing in both their L1 Dutch and L2 English. The participants were asked to write texts drawing on three different sources: a report, a web text, and a newspaper article. Using the software Inputlog (Leijten & van Waes, 2013), the resulting keystroke logs were analyzed for frequency and duration of pausing and source use at five different stages of the writing process. Confirmative factor analysis showed three components to be relevant to describe source use in L1 and L2 writing: (a) initial reading time, (b) interaction with sources, and (c) the degree of variance in source use throughout the writing process. Regardless of whether the students wrote in their L1 and L2, those who produced high-quality texts spent most of the first interval consulting the source texts. Then, this initial stage was followed by an intensive writing time, during which more skillful writers frequently switched between the text and sources for short periods. In the final stage, hardly any source use was logged for successful writers, as they focused on revising their texts.

The next empirical contribution is among the first studies to employ eye-tracking methodology in combination with keystroke logging in L2 writing research. The study by Chukharev-Hudilainen, Saricaoglu, Torrance, and Feng demonstrates a sophisticated method of investigating writing disfluencies and the nature of associated cognitive processes, using a tool they had developed to record time-aligned logs of eye gazes and keystrokes. The participants were 24 Turkish L1 writers, who completed two writing tasks, one in Turkish and one in L2 English. The study utilized largely automated analyses to examine interkey intervals or pauses at different locations (e.g., within word) and their relationships to viewing patterns (e.g., lookback distance). The results confirmed that the participating writers were significantly slower in their L2 than L1. In line with previous research (e.g., Révész, Michel, et al., Reference Révész, Michel and Lee2017), the researchers also found that, irrespective of the language used, pauses at smaller linguistic units were associated with shorter lookback distances, and pausing at larger linguistic units was related to longer lookback distances. Unexpectedly, however, writers proved faster at starting a new clause in the middle of a sentence in their L2 than L1, followed by longer pauses within the new clause. Writers also consulted the previous sentences more often when writing in their L2. Chukharev-Hudilainen, Saricaoglu, Torrance, and Feng interpreted this behavior as a potential threat to text quality, given that the co-occurrence of a clause-initial pause and a lookback beyond the current sentence likely indicates that the L2 writer’s attention was diverted away from the emerging linguistic unit.

The last empirical study in the special issue by Révész, Michel, and Lee used eye tracking together with keystroke logging and retrospective verbal reports to examine the revision and pausing behaviors of L2 writers and the cognitive processes underlying them. Thirty advanced-level Chinese writers of English completed a version of the IELTS Academic Writing task 2 while their keystrokes were logged and viewing behavior was recorded. In addition, a subset of the participants took part in stimulated recall interviews immediately after task performance. The study found that pausing at larger textual units was more likely to be related to lookbacks further in the text and higher-order writing processes. By contrast, pauses at lower textual units were linked to shorter lookbacks and lower-order writing processes. The results also revealed that, before revising their text, participants had most often viewed the text that they later altered or their eye gazes had remained off the screen. More revisions focused on language- than content-related issues, but when the revisions involved larger textual units, the difference in the number of language- and content-focused stimulated recall comments was smaller. In general, Révész, Michel, and Lee found that the triangulation of the three data sources enabled them to obtain a fuller and more valid picture of the writing process than using a single data source would make possible.

The epilogue by Galbraith and Vedder first considers the theoretical frameworks within which previous work on L2 writing processes has been situated. Then, the researchers provide a synthesis of the empirical studies included in the special issue, highlighting promising methodological advances and associated challenges. Galbraith and Vedder conclude that the methods described in the special issue promise to equip researchers with tools that will enable a deeper understanding of the complex cognitive processes involved in L2 writing.

References

REFERENCES

Adams, A. M., & Guillot, K. (2008). Working memory and writing in bilingual students. International Journal of Applied Linguistics, 156, 1328.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Baddeley, A. D. (1986). Working memory. London, UK: Oxford University Press.Google ScholarPubMed
Barkaoui, K. (2011). Think-aloud protocols in research on essay rating: An empirical study of their veridicality and reactively. Language Testing, 28, 5175.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Barkaoui, K. (2016). What and when second-language learners revise when responding to timed writing tasks on the computer: The roles of task type, second language proficiency, and keyboarding skills. The Modern Language Journal, 100, 320–240.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Bereiter, C., & Scardamalia, M. (1987). The psychology of written composition. Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates.Google Scholar
Bosher, S. (1998). The composing process of three Southeast Asian writers at the postsecondary level: An exploratory study. Journal of Second Language Writing, 7, 205241.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Chenoweth, A., & Hayes, J. (2003). The inner voice in writing. Written Communication, 20, 99118.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Cumming, A. (1989). Writing expertise and second-language proficiency. Language Learning: A Journal of Applied Linguistics, 39, 81141.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Cumming, A. (1990). Metalinguistic and ideational thinking in second language composing. Written Communication, 7, 482511.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Cumming, A. (1995). Fostering writing expertise in ESL composition instruction: Modeling and evaluation. In Belcher, D. & Braine, G. (Eds.), Academic writing in a second language: Essays on research and pedagogy (pp. 375397). Norwood, NJ: Ablex.Google Scholar
Cumming, A. (2016). Theoretical orientations to L2 writing. In Manchón, R. M. & Matsuda, P. K. (Eds.), Handbook of second and foreign language writing (pp. 6588). Boston, MA, and Berlin, Germany: De Gruyter Mouton.Google Scholar
Danzak, R. L. (2011). The integration of lexical, syntactic, and discourse features in bilingual adolescents’ writing: An exploratory approach. Language, Speech, and Hearing Services in Schools, 42, 491505.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
De Silva, R., & Graham, S. (2015). The effects of strategy instruction on writing strategy use for students of different proficiency levels. System, 53, 4759.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
de Smet, M. J. R., Leijten, M., & Van Waes, L. (2018). Exploring the process of reading during writing using eye tracking and keystroke logging. Written Communication, 35, 411447.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Galbraith, D. (1999). Writing as a knowledge-constituting process. In Torrance, M. & Galbraith, D. (Eds.), Knowing what to write (pp. 139160). Amsterdam, The Netherlands: Amsterdam University Press.Google Scholar
Galbraith, D. (2009). Cognitive models of writing. German as a Foreign Language, 2–3, 722.Google Scholar
Galbraith, D., Torrance, M., & Hallam, J. (2006). Effects of writing on conceptual coherence. In Proceedings of the 28th Annual Conference of the Cognitive Science Society (pp. 13401345). Mahway, NJ: Erlbaum.Google Scholar
Gánem-Gutiérrez, G. A., & Gilmore, A. (2018). Tracking the real-time evolution of a writing event: Second language writers at different proficiency levels. Language Learning, 68, 469506.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Gass, S., & Mackey, A. (2016). Stimulated recall in second language research (2nd ed.). New York, NY: Routledge.Google Scholar
Hayes, J. R. (1996). A new framework for understanding cognitive and affect in writing. In Levy, C. M. & Ransdell, S. (Eds.), The science of writing: Theories, methods, individual differences, and applications (pp. 128). Mahwah, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates.Google Scholar
Hayes, J. R. (2009). From idea to text. In Beard, R., Myhill, D., Riley, J., & Nystrand, M. (Eds.), The Sage handbook of writing development (pp. 6579). London, UK: Sage Publications.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Hayes, J. R., & Flower, L. S. (1980). Identifying the organization of writing processes. In Gregg, L. W. & Steinberg, E. R. (Eds.), Cognitive processes in writing (pp. 330). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.Google Scholar
Johansson, R., Wengelin, A., Johansson, V., & Holmqvist, K. (2010). Looking at the keyboard or the monitor: Relationship with text production processes. Reading and Writing, 23, 835851.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Kellogg, R. (1996). A model of working memory in writing. In Levy, M. & Ransdell, S. (Eds.), The science of writing: Theories, methods, individual differences, and applications (pp. 5772). Mahwah, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates.Google Scholar
Kellogg, R. (2001). Competition for working memory among writing processes. American Journal of Psychology, 114, 175191.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Kormos, J. (2011). Task complexity and linguistic and discourse features of narrative writing performance . Journal of Second Language Writing, 20, 148161.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Kormos, J., & Sáfár, A. (2008). Phonological short-term memory and foreign language performance in intensive language learning. Bilingualism: Language and Cognition, 11, 261271.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Lindgren, E., & Sullivan, K. (Eds.). (2019). Observing writing: Insights from keystroke logging and handwriting. Leiden, The Netherlands: Brill.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Manchón, R. M. (Ed.). (2011). Learning-to-write and writing-to-learn in an additional language . Amsterdam, The Netherlands, and Philadelphia, PA: John Benjamins.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Manchón, R. M., & Roca de Larios, J. (2007a). Writing-to learn in instructed language learning contexts. In Alcón Soler, E. & Safont Jordà, M. P. (Eds.), Intercultural language use and learning (pp. 101121). Dordrecht, The Netherlands: Springer.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Manchón, R. M., & Roca de Larios, J. (2007b). On the temporal nature of planning in L1 and L2 composing. Language Learning, 57, 549593.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Manchón, R. M., Roca de Larios, J. & Murphy, L. (2009). The temporal dimension and problem-solving nature of foreign language composing processes. Implications for theory. In Manchón, R. M. (Ed.), Writing in foreign language contexts: Learning, teaching and research (pp. 102124). Clevendon, UK: Multilingual Matters.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Marsden, E., & Plonsky, L. (2018). Data, open science, and methodological reform in second language acquisition research. In Gudmestad, A. & Edmonds, A. (Eds.), Critical reflections on data in second language acquisition (pp. 219228). Philadelphia, PA: John Benjamins.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Norris, J., & Ortega, L. (2003). Defining and measuring L2 acquisition. In Doughty, C. J. & Long, M. H. (Eds.), The handbook of second language acquisition (pp. 717761). Malden, MA: Blackwell.Google Scholar
Olson, C. B., & Land, R. (2007). A cognitive strategies approach to reading and writing instruction for English language learners in secondary school. Research in the Teaching of English, 41, 269303.Google Scholar
Polio, C. (2012). Second language writing. In Gass, S. & Mackey, A. (Eds.), Handbook of second language acquisition (pp. 319334). New York, NY: Routledge.Google Scholar
Polio, C., & Freedman, D. (2017). Understanding, evaluating and conducting second language writing research. New York, NY: Routledge/Taylor & Francis.Google Scholar
Raimes, A. (1987). Language proficiency, writing ability, and composing strategies: A study of ESL college student writers. Language Learning, 37, 439468.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Reichle, E. D. (2006). Theories of the “eye-mind” link: Computational models of eye-movement control during reading. Cognitive Systems Research, 7, 23.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Révész, A., Kourtali, N., & Mazgutova, D. (2017). Effects of task complexity on L2 writing behaviors and linguistic complexity. Language Learning, 67, 208241.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Révész, A, Michel, M., & Lee, M. (2017). Investigating IELTS Academic Writing Task 2: Relationships between cognitive writing processes, text quality, and working memory. IELTS Research Reports Online Series, 2017/3.Google Scholar
Rijlaarsdam, G., & Van Den Bergh, G. (1996). The dynamic of composing—An agenda for research into an interactive compensatory model of writing: Many questions, some answers. In Levy, C. M. & Ransdell, S. (Eds.), The science of writing: Theories, methods, individual differences, and applications (pp. 107126). Mahwah, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates.Google Scholar
Roca de Larios, J., Murphy, L., & Manchón, R. (1999). The use of restructuring strategies in EFL writing: A study of Spanish learners of English as a Foreign Language. Journal of Second Language Writing, 8, 1344.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Roca de Larios, J., Nicolás-Conesa, F., & Coyle, Y. (2016). Focus on writers: Processes and strategies. In Manchón, R. M. & Matsuda, P. K. (Eds.), Handbook of second and foreign language writing (pp. 267286). Boston, MA, and Berlin, Germany: De Gruyter Mouton.Google Scholar
Roca de Larios, J., Manchón, R., Murphy, L., & Marín, J. (2008). The foreign language writer’s strategic behaviour in the allocation of time to writing processes. Journal of Second Language Writing, 17, 3047.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Sasaki, M. (2004). A multiple-data analysis of the 3.5-year development of EFL student writers. Language Learning, 54, 525582.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Schmidt, R. (1994). Deconstructing consciousness in search of useful definitions for applied linguistics. AILA Review, 11, 1126.Google Scholar
Schmidt, R. (2001). Attention. In Robinson, P. (Ed.), Cognition and second language instruction (pp. 332). Cambridge, UK: Cambridge University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Spelman Miller, K. (2000). Academic writers on-line: Investigating pausing in the production of text. Language Teaching Research, 4, 123148.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Spelman Miller, K., Lindgren, E., & Sullivan, K. P. H. (2008). The psycholinguistic dimension in second language writing: Opportunities for research and pedagogy using computer keystroke logging. TESOL Quarterly, 42, 433453.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Stevenson, M., Schoonen, R., & De Glopper, K. (2006). Revising in two languages: A multi-dimensional comparison of online writing revisions in L1 and FL. Journal of Second Language Writing, 15, 201233.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Storch, N. (2005). Collaborative writing: Product, process, and students’ reflections. Journal of Second Language Writing, 14, 153173.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Swain, M. (1985). Communicative competence: Some roles of comprehensible input and comprehensible output in its development. In Gass, S. M. & Madden, C. G. (Eds.), Input in second language acquisition (pp. 235253). Rowley, MA: Newbury House.Google Scholar
Swain, M. (1995). Three functions of output in second language learning. In Cook, G. & Seidlhofer, B. (Eds.), Principle and practice in applied linguistics: Studies in honor of H. G. Widdowson (pp. 125144). Oxford, UK: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Swain, M., & Lapkin, S. (1995). Problems in output and the cognitive processes they generate: A step toward second language learning. Applied Linguistics, 16, 371391.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Thorson, H. (2000). Using the computer to compare foreign- and native-language writing processes: A statistical and case study approach. Modern Language Journal, 84, 5570.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Tillema, M. (2012). Writing in first and second language. Empirical studies on text quality and writing processes (Unpublished doctoral thesis). Utrecht, The Netherlands: Netherlands Graduate School of Linguistics.Google Scholar
van Gelderen, A., Oostdam, R., & van Schooten, E. (2011). Does foreign language writing benefit from increased lexical fluency? Evidence from a classroom experiment. Language Learning, 61, 281321.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Van Waes, L., & Leijten, M. (2015). Fluency in writing: A multidimensional perspective on writing fluency applied to L1 and L2. Computers and Composition, 38, 7995.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Van Waes, L., Leijten, M., Lindgren, E., & Wengelin, Å. (2015). Keystroke logging in writing research: Analyzing online writing processes. In MacArthur, C., Graham, A. S., & Fitzgerald, J. (Eds.), Handbook of writing research (2nd ed.) (Vol. 2, pp. 410426). New York, NY: Guilford Press.Google Scholar
Van Weijen, D., Van den Bergh, H., Rijlaarsdam, G., & Sanders, T. (2008). Differences in process and process-product relations in L2 writing. International Journal of Applied Linguistics, 156, 203226.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Victori, M. (1999). An analysis of writing knowledge in EFL composing: A case study of two effective and two less effective writers. System: An International Journal of Educational Technology and Applied Linguistics, 27, 537555.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Wang, W., & Wen, Q. (2002). L1 use in the L2 composing process: An exploratory study of 16 Chinese EFL writers. Journal of Second Language Writing, 11, 225246.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Yang, C., Hu, G., & Zhang, L.J. (2014). Reactivity of concurrent verbal reporting in second language writing. Journal of Second Language Writing, 24, 5170.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Yang, L., & Shi, L. (2003). Exploring six MBA students' summary writing by introspection. Journal of English for Academic Purposes, 2, 165192.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Yanguas, I., & Lado, B. (2012). Is thinking aloud reactive when writing in the heritage language? A quantitative study. Foreign Language Annals, 45, 380399.CrossRefGoogle Scholar