Hostname: page-component-8448b6f56d-jr42d Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-04-24T17:32:14.945Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

The Frequency Attenuation Effect in Identity and Associative Priming

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  10 January 2013

Francisco Nievas*
Affiliation:
Universidad de Almería (Spain)
*
Correspondence concerning this article should be addressed to Francisco Nievas Cazorla. Departmento de Psicología Evolutiva y de la Educación. Universidad de Almería. La Cañada de San Urbano. 04120 Almería. (Spain). Phone: +34-950015375. Fax: +34-950015083. E-mail: fnievas@ual.es

Abstract

Three lexical decision experiments were carried out, where the masked priming paradigm is used to study the role of the frequency attenuation effect (more priming in low-frequency target words than in high-frequency target words) in repetition and associative priming, manipulating Prime Duration (PD) and Stimulus Onset Asynchrony (SOA). A new concept was introduced, Minimum Time Threshold (MTT), this is, the minimum time interval of exposure to the masked word in order to become aware of it. Results support the notion that MTT is a key to the appearance of the frequency attenuation effect when enough word processing time is allowed. Results do not support the unified explanation of masked priming and long-term priming as proposed by Bodner and Masson (2001). Moreover, information feedback from the semantic level was not the reason for the frequency attenuation effect in repetition priming.

Se han realizado tres experimentos de decisión léxica, en donde se utiliza el paradigma de facilitación enmascarada, para estudiar el efecto de atenuación de la frecuencia (más facilitación para las palabras objetivo de baja frecuencia que para las de alta frecuencia) para la facilitación por repetición y asociativa, manipulando la duración de la palabra preparatoria (PD) y la asincronía entre los comienzos de los estímulos preparatorio y objetivo (SOA). Un nuevo concepto se ha introducido, el umbral de tiempo mínimo (MTT), que es el intervalo mínimo de exposición necesario para que la palabra enmascarada sea percibida conscientemente. Los resultados apoyan la noción de que el MTT es la clave para que aparezca el efecto de atenuación de la frecuencia cuando se da suficiente tiempo de procesamiento de la palabra. Los resultados refutan la explicación unificada de la facilitación enmascarada y facilitación a largo plazo como ha sido propuesta por Bodner y Masson (2001). Además, la retroalimentación de la información desde el nivel semántico no es la razón de la aparición del efecto de atenuación de la frecuencia en la facilitación por repetición.

Type
Research Article
Copyright
Copyright © Cambridge University Press 2010

Access options

Get access to the full version of this content by using one of the access options below. (Log in options will check for institutional or personal access. Content may require purchase if you do not have access.)

References

Algarabel, S., Sanmartín, J., García, J., & Espert, R. (1985). Normas de asociación libre para investigación experimental. [Free association norms for experimental research]. Informes del Laboratorio de Aprendizaje y Memoria: Informe no. 1. Department of Experimental Psychology, University of Valencia.Google Scholar
Anaki, D., & Henik, A. (2003). Is there a “strength effect” in automatic semantic priming? Memory & Cognition, 31(2), 262272.Google Scholar
Anderson, J. R. (1976). Language, memory, and thought. Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.Google Scholar
Anderson, J. R. (1983). The architecture of cognition. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press.Google Scholar
Angwin, A. J., Chenery, H.J., Copland, D.A., Arnott, W.L., Murdoch, B.E., & Silburn, P.A. (2004). Dopamine and semantic activation: An investigation of masked direct and indirect priming. Journal of the International Neuropsychological Society, 10, 1525.Google Scholar
Balota, D. A. (1983). Automatic semantic activation and episodic memory encoding. Journal of Verbal Learning and Verbal Behavior, 22, 88104.Google Scholar
Becker, S., Moscovitch, M., Behrmann, M., & Joordens, S. (1997). Long-term semantic priming: A computational account and empirical evidence. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 23, 10591082.Google Scholar
Bodner, G. E.& Dypvik, A.T. (2005). Masked priming of number judgments depends on prime validity and task. Memory & Cognition, 33(1), 2947.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Bodner, G. E.& Masson, M.E.J. (1997). Masked repetition priming of words and nonwords: Evidence for a nonlexical basis for priming. Journal of Memory and Language, 37, 268293.Google Scholar
Bodner, G. E.& Masson, M.E.J. (2001). Prime validity affects masked repetition priming: Evidence for an episodic resource account of priming. Journal of Memory and Language, 45, 616647.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Bodner, G. E.& Masson, M.E.J. (2003). Beyond spreading activation: An influence of relatedness proportion on masked semantic priming. Psychonomic Bulletin & Review, 10, 645652.Google Scholar
Bodner, G. E.& Masson, M.E.J. (2004). Beyond binary judgments: Prime validity modulates masked repetition priming in the naming task. Memory & Cognition, 32, 111.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Borowsky, R.& Besner, D. (1991). Visual word recognition across orthographies: On the interaction between context and degradation. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 17, 272276.Google Scholar
Borowsky, R.& Besner, D. (1993). Visual word recognition: A multistage activation model. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 19, 813840.Google Scholar
Bowers, J. S.& Turner, E. L. (2005). Masked priming is abstract in the left and right visual fields. Brain and Language, 95, 414422.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Brown, C.& Hagoort, P. (1993). The processing nature of the N400: Evidence from masked priming. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 5, 3444.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Brown, M.& Besner, D. (2002). Semantic priming: On the role of awareness in visual word recognition in the absence of an expectancy. Consciousness and Cognition, 11, 402422.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Butler, L. T.& Berry, D. C. (2004). Understanding the relationship between repetition priming and mere exposure. British Journal of Psychology, 95, 467487.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Carr, T. H., McCauley, C., Sperber, R. D.& Parmelee, C. M. (1982). Words, pictures, and priming: On semantic activation, conscious identification, and the automaticity of information processing. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 8, 757777.Google ScholarPubMed
Carreiras, M., Ferrand, L., Grainger, J.& Perea, M. (2005). Sequential effects of phonological priming in visual word recognition. Psychological Science, 18(8), 585589.Google Scholar
Castles, A., Davis, C.& Letcher, T. (1999). Neighbourhood effects on masked form priming in developing readers. Language and Cognitive Processes, 14(2), 201224.Google Scholar
Cheesman, J.& Merikle, P. M. (1984). Priming with and without awareness. Perception & Psychophysics, 36, 387395.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Cheesman, J.& Merikle, P. M. (1986). Distinguishing conscious from unconscious perceptual processes. Canadian Journal of Psychology, 40, 343367.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Collins, A. M.& Loftus, E. F. (1975). A spreading-activation theory of semantic processing. Psychological Review, 82, 407428.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Collins, A. M.& Quillian, M. R. (1969). Retrieval time from semantic memory. Journal of Verbal Learning and Verbal Behaviour, 8, 240247.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Colombo, L. (1986). Activation and inhibition with orthographically similar words. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 12, 226234.Google Scholar
Dagenbach, D., Carr, T. H.& Wilhelmsen, A. (1989). Task-induced strategies and near-threshold priming: Conscious influences on unconscious perception. Journal of Memory and Language, 28, 412443.Google Scholar
Davis, C. J.& Lupker, S. J. (2006). Masked inhibitory priming in English: Evidence for lexical inhibition. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 32(3), 668687.Google Scholar
de Groot, A. M. B. (1984). Primed lexical decision: Combined effects of the proportion of related prime-target pairs and the stimulus-onset asynchrony of prime and target. Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 36A, 253280.Google Scholar
de Groot, A. M. B. (1990). The locus of the associative-priming effect in the mental lexicon. In Balota, D.A. & d'Arcais, G.B. Flores (Eds.), Comprehension processes in reading (pp. 101123). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.Google Scholar
Dehaene, S., Jovert, A., Naccache, L., Ciuciu, P., Poline, J. B., Bihan, D.& Cohen, L. (2004). Letter binding and invariant recognition of masked words. Psychological Science, 15(5), 307313.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Dehaene, S., Naccache, L., Cohen, L., Le Bihan, D., Mangin, J., Poline, J.& Rivière, D. (2001). Cerebral mechanisms of word masking and unconscious repetition priming. Nature Neuroscience, 4, 17.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
den Heyer, K., Briand, K.& Dannenbring, G. L. (1983). Strategic factors in a lexical-decision task: Evidence for automatic and attention-driven processes. Memory & Cognition, 11, 374381.Google Scholar
den Heyer, K., Briand, K.& Smith, L. (1985). Automatic and strategic effects in semantic priming: An examination of Becker's verification model. Memory & Cognition, 13, 228232.Google Scholar
den Heyer, K., Goring, A.& Dannenbring, G. L. (1985). Semantic priming and word repetition: The two effects are additive. Journal of Memory and Language, 24, 699716.Google Scholar
Dennis, I.& Schmidt, K. (2003). Associative processes in repetition priming. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 29(4), 532538.Google Scholar
Dlhopolsky, J. G. (1989). Synchronizing stimulus displays with millisecond timer software for IBM PC. Behavior Research Methods, Instruments and Computers, 21(4). 441446.Google Scholar
Dosher, B. A.& Rosedale, G. (1989). Integrated retrieval cues as a mechanism for priming in retrieval from memory. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 118, 191211.Google Scholar
Duchek, J. T.& Neely, J. H. (1989). A dissociative word-frequency x levels-of-processing interaction in episodic recognition and lexical decision tasks. Memory & Cognition, 17, 148162.Google Scholar
Durgunoglu, A. Y. (1988). Repetition, semantic priming, and stimulus quality: Implications for the interactive-compensatory reading model. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 14, 590603.Google Scholar
Durgunoglu, A. Y.& Neely, J. H. (1987). On obtaining episodic priming in a lexical decision task following paired-associate learning. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 13, 206222.Google Scholar
Duyck, W. (2005). Translation and associative priming with cross-lingual pseudohomophones: Evidence for nonselective phonological activation in bilinguals. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory and Cognition, 31(6), 13401359.Google Scholar
Ferrand, L. (1996). The masked repetition priming effect dissipates when increasing the inter-stimulus interval: Evidence from word naming. Acta Psychologica, 91, 1525.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Ferrand, L., Grainger, J.& Segui, J. (1994). A study of masked form priming in picture and word naming. Memory & Cognition, 22, 431441.Google Scholar
Feustel, T. C., Shiffrin, R. M.& Salasoo, A. (1983). Episodic and lexical contributions to the repetition effect in word identification. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 112, 309346.Google Scholar
Finkbeiner, M., Forster, K., Nicol, J.& Nakamura, K. (2004). The role of polysemy in masked semantic and translation priming. Journal of Memory and Language, 51, 122.Google Scholar
Fischler, I.& Goodman, G. O. (1978). Latency of associative activation in memory. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 4, 455470.Google Scholar
Fleischman, D. A.& Gabrieli, J. D. E. (1998). Repetition priming in normal aging and Alzheimer's disease: A review of findings and theories. Psychology and Aging, 13(1), 88119.Google Scholar
Forster, K. I. (1987). Form-priming with masked primes: The best-match hypothesis. In Coltheart, M. (Ed.), Attention & performance XII (pp. 127146). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.Google Scholar
Forster, K. I. (1998). The pros and cons of masked priming. Journal of Psycholinguistic Research: Special Issue, 27(2), 203233.Google Scholar
Forster, K. I. (1999). The microgenesis of priming effects in lexical access. Brain & Language Special Issue: Mental lexicon, 68(1-2), 515.Google ScholarPubMed
Forster, K. I. (2003). Category size effects revisited: Frequency and masked priming effects in semantic categorization. Brain and Language, 90, 276286.Google Scholar
Forster, K. I., Booker, J., Schacter, D. L.& Davis, C. (1990). Masked repetition priming: Lexical activation or novel memory trace? Bulletin of the Psychonomic Society, 28, 341345.Google Scholar
Forster, K. I.& Davis, C. (1984). Repetition priming and frequency attenuation in lexical access. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 10, 680698.Google Scholar
Forster, K. I.& Davis, C. (1991). The density constraint on form-priming in the naming task: Interference effects from a masked prime. Journal of Memory and Language, 30, 125.Google Scholar
Forster, K. I., Davis, C., Schoknecht, C.& Carter, R. (1987). Masked priming with graphemically related forms: Repetition or partial activation? Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 39, 211251.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Forster, K. I., Mohan, K.& Hector, J. (2003). The mechanics of masked priming. In Kinoshita, S. & Lupker, S.J. (Eds.), Masked priming: The state of the art (pp. 337). New York: Psychology Press.Google Scholar
Fowler, C. A., Wolford, G., Slade, R.& Tassinary, L. (1981). Lexical access with and without awareness. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 110, 341362.Google Scholar
Frenck-Mestre, C.& Bueno, S. (1999). Semantic features and semantic categories: Differences in rapid activation of the lexicon. Brain & Language Special Issue: Mental Lexicon, 68(1-2), 199204.Google Scholar
Frings, C.& Neubauer, A. (2005). Are masked-stimuli-discrimination-tests in masked priming studies measures of intelligence? An alternative task for measuring inspection time. Personality and Individual Differences, 39, 11811191.Google Scholar
Graf, P.& Mandler, G. (1984). Activation makes words more accessible, but not necessarily more retrievable. Journal of Verbal Learning and Verbal Behaviour, 23, 553568.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Hines, D., Czerwinski, M., Sawyer, P. K.& Dwyer, M. (1986). Automatic semantic priming: Effect of category exemplar level and word association level. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 12, 370379.Google Scholar
Hino, Y., Lupker, S. J., Ogawa, T., Sears, C. R. (2003). Masked repetition priming and word frequency effects across different types of Japanese scripts: An examination of the lexical activation account. Journal of Memory and Language, 48, 3366.Google Scholar
Holcomb, P. J., Reder, L., Misra, M.& Grainger, J. (2005). The effects of prime visibility on ERP measures of masked priming. Cognitive Brain Research, 24, 155172.Google Scholar
Holender, D. (1986). Semantic activation without conscious identification in dichotic listening, parafoveal vision, and visual masking: A survey and appraisal. Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 9, 166.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Holender, D.& Duscherer, K. (2004). Unconscious perception: the need for a paradigm shift. Perception & Psychophysics, 66(5), 872881.Google Scholar
Humphreys, G. W., Besner, D.& Quinlan, P. T. (1988). Event perception and the word repetition effect. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 117, 5167.Google Scholar
Humphreys, G.W., Evett, L.J.& Quinlan, P.T. (1990). Orthographic processing in visual word identification. Cognitive Psychology, 22, 517560.Google Scholar
Jacoby, L. L. (1983). Perceptual enhancement: Persistent effects of an experience. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 9, 2138.Google Scholar
Jacoby, L. L., Baker, J. G.& Brooks, L. R. (1989). Episodic effects on picture identification: Implications for theories of concept learning and theories of memory. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 15, 275281.Google Scholar
Jacoby, L. L.& Dallas, M. (1981). On the relationship between autobiographical memory and perceptual learning. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 110, 306340.Google Scholar
Jacoby, L. L.& Hayman, C. A. G. (1987). Specific visual transfer in word identification. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 13, 456463.Google Scholar
Joordens, S.& Becker, S. (1997). The long and short of semantic priming effects in lexical decision. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 23, 10831105.Google Scholar
Juilland, R.& Chang-Rodríguez, E. (1964). Frequency dictionary of Spanish words. The Hague: Mouton.Google Scholar
Kahneman, D. (1973). Attention and Effort. Englewood Cliffs. N.J.: Prentice-Hall.Google Scholar
Kinoshita, S. (2006). Additive and interactive effects of word frequency and masked repetition in the lexical decision task. Psychonomic Bulletin & Review, 13(4), 668673.Google Scholar
Koivisto, M.& Revonsuo, A. (2004). Preconscious analysis of global structure: Evidence from masked priming. Visual Cognition, 11(1), 105127.Google Scholar
Kolers, P. A.& Roediger, H. L. (1984). Procedures of mind. Journal of Verbal Learning and Verbal Behavior, 23, 425449.Google Scholar
Kouider, S.& Dupoux, E. (2001). A functional disconnection between spoken and visual word recognition: Evidence from unconscious priming. Cognition, 82, 3549.Google Scholar
Kunde, W., Kiesel, A.& Hoffmann, J. (2005). On the masking and disclosure of unconscious elaborate processing. A reply to Van Opstal, Reynvoet, and Verguts (2005). Cognition, 97, 99105.Google Scholar
Lingnau, A.& Vorberg, D. (2005). The time course of response inhibition in masked priming. Perception & Psychophysics, 67(3), 545557.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Lleras, A.& Enns, J. T. (2005). Updating a cautionary tale of masked priming: reply to Klapp (2005). Journal of Experimental Psychology: General. 134(3), 436440.Google Scholar
Locker, L., Simpson, G. B.& Yates, M. (2003). Semantic neighbourhood effects on the recognition of ambiguous words. Memory & Cognition, 31(4), 505515.Google Scholar
Logan, J. M.& Balota, D. A. (2003). Conscious and unconscious lexical retrieval blocking in younger and older adults. Psychology and Aging, 18(3), 537550.Google Scholar
Logan, G. D. (1990). Repetition priming and automaticity: Common underlying mechanisms? Cognitive Psychology, 22, 135.Google Scholar
MacKay, D. G. (1987). The organization of perception and action: A theory for language and other cognitive skills. New York: Springer-Verlag.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
MacKay, D. G. (1990). Perception, action and awareness: A three-body problem. In Newmann, O. & Prinz, W. (Eds.), Relationships between perception and action. Berlin: Springer-Verlag.Google Scholar
MacLeod, C. M.& Masson, M. E. J. (2000). Repetition priming in speeded word reading: Contributions of perceptual and conceptual processing episodes. Journal of Memory and Language, 42, 208228.Google Scholar
Marcel, A. J. (1983). Conscious and unconscious perception: Experiments on visual masking and word recognition. Cognitive Psychology, 15, 197237.Google Scholar
Martin, R. C.& Jensen, C. R. (1988). Phonological priming in the lexical decision task: A failure to replicate. Memory & Cognition, 16, 505521.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Masson, M. E. J.& Bodner, G. E. (2003). A retrospective view of masked priming: toward a unified account of masked and long-term repetition priming. In Kinoshita, S. & Lupker, S.J. (Eds), Masked Priming: The State of the Art (pp. 5796). Hove: Psychology Press.Google Scholar
Masson, J. E. J.& Isaak, M. I. (1999). Masked priming of words and nonwords in a naming task: Further evidence for a nonlexical basis for priming. Memory & Cognition, 27, 399412.Google Scholar
McCauley, C., Parrmelee, C. M., Sperber, R. D.& Carr, T. H. (1980). Early extraction of meaning from pictures and its relation to conscious identification. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 6, 265276.Google Scholar
McClelland, J. L.& Rumelhart, D. E. (1981). An interactive activation model of context effects in letter perception: Part 1. An account of basic findings. Psychological Review, 88, 375407Google Scholar
McKone, E. (1995). Short-term implicit memory for words and nonwords. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory and Cognition, 21, 11081126.Google Scholar
McNamara, T. P. (2005). Semantic priming: perspectives from memory and word recognition. New York: Psychology Press.Google Scholar
Merikle, P. M., Smilek, D.& Eastwood, J. D. (2001). Perception without awareness: perspectives from cognitive psychology. Cognition, 79, 115134.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Monsell, S. (1991). The nature and locus of word frequency effects in reading. In Besner, D. & Humphreys, G.W. (Eds), Basic processes in reading: Visual word recognition (pp. 148197). Hove: Erlbaum.Google Scholar
Morton, J. (1969). Interaction of information in word recognition. Psychological Review, 76, 165178.Google Scholar
Neely, J. H. (1977). Semantic priming and retrieval from lexical memory: Roles of inhibitionless spreading activation and limited-capacity attention. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 106, 226254.Google Scholar
Neill, W. T., Valdes, L. A., Terry, K. M.& Gorfein, D. S. (1992). Persistence of negative priming: II. Evidence for episodic trace retrieval. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 18, 9931000.Google Scholar
Nevers, B.& Versace, R. (1998). Knowledge acquisition in long-term memory: Activation and construction or traces. In Ritschard, G., Berchtold, A., Duc, F., & Zighed, A.D. (Eds), Apprentissage: Des principes naturels aux méthodes artificielles. [Learning: From natural principles to artificial methods.] (pp. 125138). Paris: Hermès.Google Scholar
Norris, D. (1984). The effects of frequency, repetition and stimulus quality in visual word recognition. Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 36A, 507518.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Perea, M.& Gotor, A. (1997). Associative and semantic priming effects occur at very short stimulus-onset asynchronies in lexical decision and naming. Cognition, 62, 223240.Google Scholar
Perea, M.& Lupker, S. (2003b). Does jugde activate COURT? Transposed-letter similarity effects in masked associative priming. Memory & Cognition, 31(6), 829841.Google Scholar
Perea, M.& Rosa, E. (2002). Does the proportion of associatively related pairs modulate the associative priming effect at very brief stimulus-onset asynchronies? Acta Psychologica, 110, 103124.Google Scholar
Perea, M.& Rosa, E. (2002b). The effects of associative and semantic priming in the lexical decision task. Psychological Research, 66, 180194.Google Scholar
Plaut, D. C.& Booth, J. R. (2000). Individual and developmental differences in semantic priming: Empirical and computational support for a single-mechanism account of lexical processing. Psychological Review, 107(4), 786823.Google Scholar
Pollatsek, A.& Well, A. D. (1995). On the use of counterbalanced designs in cognitive research: A suggestion for a better and more powerful analysis. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 21, 785794.Google Scholar
Posner, M. I.& Snyder, C. R. R. (1975). Attention and cognitive control. In Solso, R.L. (Ed.), Information processing and cognition: The Loyola symposium (pp. 5585). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.Google Scholar
Raaijmakers, J. G. W. (2003). A further look at the “Language-as-Fixed-Effects Fallacy”. Canadian Journal of Experimental Psychology, 57, 141151.Google Scholar
Raaijmakers, J. G. W., Schrijnemakers, J.M.C.& Gremmen, F. (1999). How to deal with “The Language-as-Fixed-Effect Fallacy”: Common misconceptions and alternative solutions. Journal of Memory and Language, 41, 416426.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Rajaram, S.& Neely, J. H. (1992). Dissociative masked repetition priming and word frequency effects in lexical decision and episodic recognition tasks. Journal of Memory and Language, 31, 152182.Google Scholar
Rastle, K., Davis, M. H., Marslen-Wilson, W. D.& Tyler, L. K. (2000). Morphological and semantic effects in visual word recognition: A time-course study. Language & Cognitive Processes, 15, 507537.Google Scholar
Ratcliff, R., Hockley, W.& McKoon, G. (1985). Components of activation: Repetition and priming effects in lexical decision and recognition. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 114, 435450.Google Scholar
Ratcliff, R.& McKoon, G. (1988). A retrieval theory of priming in memory. Psychological Review, 95, 385408.Google Scholar
Ratcliff, R.& McKoon, G. (1994). Retrieving information from memory: Spreading-activation theories versus compound-cue theories. Psychological Review, 101, 177184.Google Scholar
Reynvoet, B., Gevers, W.& Caessens, B. (2005). Unconscious primes activate motor codes through semantics. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory and Cognition, 31(5), 9911000.Google Scholar
Richardson-Klavehn, A.& Bjork, R. A. (1988). Measures of memory. Annual Review of Psychology, 39, 475543.Google Scholar
Roediger, H.L.& Blaxton, T. A. (1987). Effects of varying modality, surface features and retention interval on priming in word-fragment completion. Memory, and Cognition, 15, 379388.Google Scholar
Salasoo, A., Shiffrin, R. M.& Feustel, T. C. (1985). Building permanent memory codes: Codification and repetition effect in word identification. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 114, 5077.Google Scholar
Scarborough, D. L., Cortese, C., & Scarborough, H. S. (1977). Frequency and repetition effects in lexical memory. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 3, 117.Google Scholar
Schacter, D. L. (1987). Implicit memory: History and current status. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 13, 501518.Google Scholar
Schacter, D. L., Chiu, C. Y. P.& Ochsner, K. N. (1993). Implicit memory: A selective review. Annual Review of Neuroscience, 16, 159182.Google Scholar
Schacter, D. L.& Graf, P. (1986). Preserved learning in amnesic patients: Perspectives from research on direct priming. Journal of Clinical and Experimental Neuropsychology, 8, 727743.Google Scholar
Schneider, W.& Shiffrin, R. M. (1977). Controlled and automatic human information processing: I. Detection, search, and attention. Psychological Review, 84, 166.Google Scholar
Sears, C. R., Campbell, C. R.& Lupker, S. J. (2006). Is there a neighbourhood frequency effect in English? Evidence from reading and lexical decision. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 32(4), 10401062.Google Scholar
Segui, J., & Grainger, J. (1990). Priming word recognition with orthographic neighbors: Effects of relative prime-target frequency. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 16, 6576.Google Scholar
Sereno, J. A. (1991). Graphemic, associative, and syntactic priming effects at a brief stimulus onset asynchrony in lexical decision and naming. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 17, 459477.Google Scholar
Shiffrin, R. M.& Schneider, W. (1977). Controlled and automatic human information processing: II. Perceptual learning, automatic attending, and a general theory. Psychological Review, 84, 127190.Google Scholar
Squire, L. R. (1987). Memory and brain. New York: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Stolz, J. A.& Merikle, P. M. (2000). Conscious and unconscious influences of memory: temporal dynamics. Memory, 8(5), 333343.Google Scholar
Stolz, J. A.& Neely, J. H. (1995). When target degradation does and does not enhance semantic context effects in word recognition. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory and Cognition, 21, 596611.Google Scholar
Tenpenny, P. L. (1995). Abstractionist versus episodic theories of repetition priming and word identification. Psychonomic Bulletin & Review, 2, 339363.Google Scholar
Toth, J. P., Reingold, E. M.& Jacoby, L. L. (1994). Toward a redefinition of implicit memory: Process dissociation following elaborative processing and self-generation. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, & Cognition, 20, 290303.Google Scholar
Tulving, E. (1984). Précis of elements of episodic memory. Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 7, 223238.Google Scholar
Tulving, E., Schacter, D. L.& Stark, H. A. (1982). Priming effects in word-fragment completion are independent of recognition memory. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, & Cognition, 8, 336342.Google Scholar
Tweedy, J. R., Lapinski, R. H.& Schvaneveldt, R. W. (1977). Semantic-context effects on word recognition: Influence of varying the proportion of items presented in an appropriate context. Memory & Cognition, 5, 8489.Google Scholar
Van Assche, E.& Grainger, J. (2006). A study of relative-position priming with superset primes. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 32(2), 399415.Google Scholar
Van Opstal, F., Reynvoet, B.& Verguts, T. (2005). Unconscious semantic categorization and mask interactions: An elaborate response to Kunde et al. (2005). Cognition, 97, 107113.Google Scholar
Van Opstal, F., Reynvoet, B.& Verguts, T. (2005b). How to trigger elaborate processing? A comment on Kunde, Kiesel, and Hoffmann (2003). Cognition, 97, 8997.Google Scholar
Versace, R. (1998). Frequency and prime duration effects on repetition priming and orthographic priming with words and nonwords. Cahiers de Psychologie Cognitive [Current Psychology of Cognition], 17, 535556.Google Scholar
Versace, R.& Nevers, B. (2003). Word frequency effect on repetition priming as a function of prime duration and delay between the prime and the target. British Journal of Psychology, 94, 389408.Google Scholar
Visser, T. A. W., Merikle, P. M.& Di Lollo, V. (2005). Priming in the attentional blink: perception without awareness? Visual Cognition, 12(7), 13621372.Google Scholar
Whitlow, J. W. (1990). Differential sensitivity of perceptual identification for words and nonwords to test expectations: Implications for the locus of word frequency effects. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 16, 837851.Google Scholar
Whitlow, J. W.& Cebollero, A. (1989). The nature of word frequency effects on perceptual identification. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 15, 643656.Google Scholar
Wilding, J. M. (1986). Joint effects of semantic priming and repetition in a lexical decision task: Implications for a model of lexical access. Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 38A, 213228.Google Scholar
Woltz, D. J. (1990). Repetition of semantic comparisons: Temporary and persistent priming effects. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 16, 392403.Google Scholar
Zeelenberg, R., Wagenmakers, E. J., & Shiffrin, R. M. (2004). Nonword repetition priming in lexical decision reverses as a function of study task and speed stress. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 30(1), 270277.Google Scholar