Hostname: page-component-8448b6f56d-42gr6 Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-04-24T09:59:51.533Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

Long-term effects of breast-feeding in a national birth cohort: educational attainment and midlife cognitive function

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  22 December 2006

Marcus Richards*
Affiliation:
MRC National Survey of Health and Development, Royal Free & University College Medical School, University College London, Department of Epidemiology and Public Health, 1–19 Torrington Place, London WC1E 6BT, UK
Rebecca Hardy
Affiliation:
MRC National Survey of Health and Development, Royal Free & University College Medical School, University College London, Department of Epidemiology and Public Health, 1–19 Torrington Place, London WC1E 6BT, UK
Michael EJ Wadsworth
Affiliation:
MRC National Survey of Health and Development, Royal Free & University College Medical School, University College London, Department of Epidemiology and Public Health, 1–19 Torrington Place, London WC1E 6BT, UK
*
*Corresponding author: Email m.richards@ucl.ac.uk
Rights & Permissions [Opens in a new window]

Abstract:

Core share and HTML view are not available for this content. However, as you have access to this content, a full PDF is available via the ‘Save PDF’ action button.
Objective:

A recent meta-analysis showed that breast-feeding confers a 3.2 point increment in cognitive function through adolescence. Little is known, however, about possible longer-term effects of breast-feeding. We investigated the effect of breast-feeding on educational attainment, and on a range of cognitive skills in midlife, in the British 1946 birth cohort.

Objective:

Design: Regression analyses were used to test the association between breast-feeding, likelihood of obtaining advanced educational qualifications by age 26 years, and three cognitive test scores at age 53 years: i.e. reading ability (NART), timed visual search and verbal memory. These associations were then adjusted for social confounding variables and for cognitive ability at age 15 years.

Setting and subjects:

One thousand seven hundred and thirty-nine male and female participants in the MRC National Survey of Health and Development, also known as the British 1946 birth cohort, distributed throughout England, Wales and Scotland.

Results:

Breast-feeding was significantly and positively associated with educational attainment, an effect that was independent of early social background, but largely accounted for by cognitive ability at age 15 years. Breast-feeding was significantly and positively associated with the NART at 53 years, an effect that was independent of early social background, educational attainment and adult social class, but, again, largely accounted for by cognitive ability at 15 years. There was no independent effect of breast-feeding on timed visual search or verbal memory at 53 years.

Conclusion:

The benefit of breast-feeding has long-term potential impact across the life course through its influence on childhood cognition and educational attainment.

Type
Research Article
Copyright
Copyright © CABI Publishing 2002

References

1Anderson, JA, Johnstone, BM, Remley, DT. Breast-feeding and cognitive development: a meta-analysis. Am. J. Clin. Nutr. 1999;70:525–35.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
2Rodgers, B. Feeding in infancy and later ability and attainment: a longitudinal study. Develop. Med. Child Neurol. 1978;20:421–6.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
3Wadsworth, MEJ. The Imprint of Time: Childhood, History and Adult Life. Oxford: Clarendon Press, 1991.Google Scholar
4Wadsworth, MEJ, Mann, SL, Rodgers, B, Kuh, DL, Hilder, WS, Yusuf, EJ. Loss and representativeness in a 43 year follow-up of a national birth cohort. J. Epidemiol. Community Health 1992;46:300–4.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
5Department of Education and Science. Burnham Further Education Committee Grading Courses. London: HMSO, 1972.Google Scholar
6Nelson, HE, Willison, JR. National Adult Reading Test (NART), 2nd ed. Windsor: NFER-Nelson, 1991.Google Scholar
7Crawford, JR, Stewart, LE, Parker, DM, Besson, JAO, De Lacey, G. Prediction of WAIS IQ with the National Adult Reading Test: cross-validation and extension. Br. J. Psychol. 1989;28:267–73.Google Scholar
8Rabbitt, P. Does it all go together when it goes? The Nineteenth Bartlett Memorial Lecture. Quart. J. Exp. Psychol. 1993;46A:385434.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
9Masur, DM, Sliwinski, M, Lipton, RB, Blau, AD, Crystal, HA. Neuropsychological prediction of dementia and the absence of dementia in healthy elderly persons. Neurology 1994;44:1427–32.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
10Jacobs, DM, Sano, M, Dooneief, G, Marder, K, Bell, KL, Stern, Y. Neuropsychological detection and characterization of preclinical Alzheimer's disease. Neurology 1995;45:957–62.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
11Pigeon, DA. Details of the fifteen years tests. In: Douglas, JWB, Ross, JM, Simpson, HR, eds. All Our Future. London: Davies, 1968; Appendix 1.Google Scholar
12Office of Population Censuses and Surveys. Classification of Occupations. London: HMSO, 1970.Google Scholar
13Douglas, JWB. The Home and The School. London: Macgibbon & Kee, 1964.Google Scholar
14Crawford, MA. The role of essential fatty acids in neural development: implications for perinatal nutrition. Am. J. Clin. Nutr. 1993;75(Suppl.):703S–10S.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
15Stern, Y, Gurland, B, Tatemichi, TK, Tang, MX, Wilder, D, Mayeux, R. Influence of education and occupation on the incidence of Alzheimer's disease. J. Am. Med. Assoc. 1994;271:1004–10.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
16Horn, JL, Cattell, RB. Age differences in fluid and crystallized intelligence. Acta Psychologica 1967;26:107–29.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed