Hostname: page-component-8448b6f56d-dnltx Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-04-25T05:15:22.131Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

Body size preference and body weight perception among two migrant groups of non-Western origin

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  01 December 2008

Mary Nicolaou*
Affiliation:
Department of Social Medicine, Academic Medical Centre, University of Amsterdam, PO Box 22660, 1100 DD Amsterdam, The Netherlands Department of Nutrition and Health, Institute for Health Sciences, VU University, Amsterdam, The Netherlands
Colleen Doak
Affiliation:
Department of Nutrition and Health, Institute for Health Sciences, VU University, Amsterdam, The Netherlands
Rob van Dam
Affiliation:
Department of Nutrition and Health, Institute for Health Sciences, VU University, Amsterdam, The Netherlands Department of Nutrition, Harvard School of Public Health, Boston, MA, USA
Karen Hosper
Affiliation:
Department of Social Medicine, Academic Medical Centre, University of Amsterdam, PO Box 22660, 1100 DD Amsterdam, The Netherlands
Jaap Seidell
Affiliation:
Department of Nutrition and Health, Institute for Health Sciences, VU University, Amsterdam, The Netherlands
Karien Stronks
Affiliation:
Department of Social Medicine, Academic Medical Centre, University of Amsterdam, PO Box 22660, 1100 DD Amsterdam, The Netherlands
*
*Corresponding author: Email m.nicolaou@amc.uva.nl
Rights & Permissions [Opens in a new window]

Abstract

Objectives

To evaluate body size preference, body weight perception and their relationship with actual weight in two migrant groups of non-Western origin, Turks and Moroccans; additionally, to study the association between body size preference and acculturation.

Design

Cross-sectional study.

Setting

Amsterdam, The Netherlands.

Subjects and methods

Males and females (18–30 years) were randomly selected from the population registry (n 451); participants, or at least one of their parents, were born in Turkey or Morocco. Body size preference was assessed using seven silhouette drawings and body weight perception was assessed by asking participants’ opinion of own weight. Acculturation variables were generation status and two scale measures, cultural orientation and social contacts.

Results

Participants showed preference for a thin body size. The discrepancy between ideal and current size was significant in women but not men (P < 0·001). Perceived current body size was correlated with BMI (Spearman’s correlation coefficient 0·60, P < 0·001 (men) and 0·73, P < 0·001 (women)). Among overweight participants (BMI = 25·0–29·9 kg/m2), 63–82 % of men and 35 % of women perceived themselves as ‘average’. Paying attention to own body weight was associated with a discrepancy between ideal and current size among women and with perceiving oneself as ‘overweight’ among men. Body size preference was not significantly associated with the three acculturation variables.

Conclusion

We did not observe a preference for large body sizes in these two non-Western migrant groups. Similar to Western populations, most women wished to be thinner than they were. This was not the case among men, the majority of whom were also unaware of being overweight.

Type
Research Paper
Copyright
Copyright © The Authors 2008

Overweight and obesity are a major public health problem and are associated with increased risk for type 2 diabetes, CVD and several types of cancer(Reference Visscher and Seidell1). Their prevalence is reaching epidemic proportions, with an estimated 1 billion overweight and 300 million obese adults worldwide(2). Among non-Western migrants, which represent an increasing proportion of many Western populations(3), the prevalence of overweight and obesity is often higher than that of host populations(Reference Kumar, Meyer, Wandel, Dalen and Holmboe-Ottesen4Reference Wandell, Hjorleifsdottir and Johansson6). In The Netherlands more than half of the general adult population is either overweight (51 % of males, 42 % of females) or obese (10 % of males, 12 % of females)(Reference Schokker, Visscher, Nooyens, van Baak and Seidell7). In two of the main migrant groups of non-Western origin, Turks and Moroccans, it is estimated that 25–32 % of women and 37–44 % of men are overweight while 39–40 % of women and 13–16 % of men are obese(Reference Brussaard, van Erp-Baart, Brants, Hulshof and Lowik8, Reference Dijkshoorn, Uitenbroek and Middelkoop9).

Public health promotion efforts aimed at overweight prevention often proceed from the assumption that most individuals prefer to be thin and that the first step in motivating individuals to lose weight is to raise awareness of weight status among those with overweight(Reference Wammes, Breedveld, Looman and Brug10). This approach is justified for Western populations that value thinness in women and lean, muscular physiques in men(Reference Rodin11Reference Pope, Olivardia, Gruber and Borowiecki13), but where awareness of overweight among those with overweight is often low(Reference Blokstra, Burns and Seidell14, Reference Madrigal, Sanchez-Villegas, Martinez-Gonzalez, Kearney, Gibney, Irala and Martínez15). However, this assumption might not hold for non-Western migrant groups among whom body size preference may differ. For example, in many traditional non-Western cultures large bodies in both males and females are associated with prosperity and health(Reference Benjelloun16Reference Simeon, Rattan, Panchoo, Kungeesingh, Ali and Abdool21). Migrants originating from non-Western societies may adhere to the ideals from their traditional cultures. Although this may protect them from experiencing dissatisfaction with their body size, it may also negatively influence the motivation for weight control or weight loss.

On the other hand, living in a Western society is also likely to influence the preferred body size of migrants through the process of acculturation(Reference Cachelin, Monreal and Juarez22). Acculturation is a concept that ‘comprehends those phenomena which result when groups of individuals having different cultures come into continuous first-hand contact with subsequent changes in the original culture patterns of either or both groups’(Reference Redfield, Linton and Heskovitz23), usually with a greater change taking place in the ‘minority’ group(Reference Berry24). The expectation is that migrants are likely to adopt the body size ideals of their ‘host’ peers. Thus, highly acculturated migrants to Western societies would develop a preference for thinner figures.

That culture influences body size preference has been the rationale of many studies researching this topic among different ethnic groups. However, many of the studies of this topic have either focused on non-Western populations in their country of origin(Reference Al-Sendi, Shetty and Musaiger19, Reference Jackson, Rashed and Saad-Eldin20, Reference Mciza, Goedecke, Steyn, Charlton, Puoane, Meltzer, Levitt and Lambert25Reference Shih and Kubo28) or have been conducted among US ethnic minority groups(Reference Cachelin, Rebeck, Chung and Pelayo29Reference Fitzgibbon, Blackman and Avellone32). Few studies have considered this issue among non-Western migrants to European countries(Reference Brug, Wammes, Kremers, Giskes and Oenema33Reference Furnham and Alibhai36). Likewise, the association between body size preference and acculturation among ethnic minorities has seldom been studied in Europe. We found two UK studies, one that reported similar attitudes to body size among second-generation Asian women and white British women(Reference Bush, Williams, Lean and Anderson34) and another where Kenyan Asians residing in the UK were more similar to white British women in their body size preferences than to women still living in Kenya(Reference Furnham and Alibhai36). Considering that body size preference may be an important factor in motivating individuals to lose weight, information about this determinant among European migrants is necessary.

Two of the largest non-Western migrant groups in Western Europe originate from Turkey (The Netherlands, Germany, Denmark, Sweden) and Morocco (The Netherlands, Belgium, France, Spain). Migration from Turkey and Morocco was encouraged in the 1970s in order to fill labour shortages, particularly for lower manual positions. The initial migrants came from particularly deprived areas of their home country. Since that time further migration has occurred due to family reunification and formation, with many young Turkish and Moroccan adults choosing partners from their country of origin(37). In The Netherlands many reside in the larger cities; in Amsterdam, persons of Turkish or Moroccan origin form respectively 5 % and 9 % of the total population(38).

Literature from Morocco indicates that overweight and obesity prevalence, particularly among women, is on the increase and that this may be partly due to a tradition that values plumpness(Reference Benjelloun16, Reference Mokhtar, Elati, Chabir, Bour, Elkari, Schlossman, Caballero and Aguenaou17, Reference Rguibi and Belahsen39). Although we found literature that indicates high overweight and obesity prevalence in Turkey(Reference Yumuk40, Reference Ertem, Bahceci, Tuzcu, Saka, Ozturk and Gokalp41), we found no studies of body size preference among Turkish adults. Anecdotal evidence indicates that, also in Turkey, there is a tradition of preference for large figures.

As already mentioned, the prevalence of overweight and obesity among Turkish and Moroccan migrants to The Netherlands is high. However, there is little information about body size preference (the body size considered to be ‘ideal’) or the perception of overweight (whether individuals classify their body weight correctly) among these two migrant populations. It may be that a preference for larger figures and/or the misperception of overweight is highly prevalent in these groups, which would have important implications for intervention development. Therefore the present study aimed to:

  1. 1. Evaluate body size preference and body weight perception among young men and women of Turkish and Moroccan origin.

  2. 2. Investigate whether preference and perception are related to actual body weight and to whether individuals pay attention to their body weight.

  3. 3. Examine the association between body size preference and acculturation level.

Methods

Study population

Participants for the present study were 18–30-year-olds who took part in LASER, a study of health behaviour among Turks and Moroccans aged 10 to 30 years living in Amsterdam, The Netherlands. In the LASER study a random sample of people born in Turkey or Morocco or with at least one parent born in Turkey or Morocco was drawn from the Amsterdam municipal population register. Participants born in Turkey or Morocco were classified as first-generation migrants. Second-generation migrants were those born in The Netherlands and who had at least one parent born in either Turkey or Morocco. As mentioned earlier, migration to the Netherlands from Turkey and Morocco began in the 1970s. Therefore, the majority of adult Turks and Moroccans are of the first generation, as has been observed in studies that have included adults(Reference Hosper, Klazinga and Stronks42). The LASER study focused on younger participants in order to ensure more equal representation of the second generation.

The total Turkish sample consisted of 1556 persons. Approximately 13 % of the sample could not be traced because of incorrect address information. Of the 1354 respondents who could be traced, 768 participated in the study (57 %). Most cases of ‘non-response’ were due to refusals to participate (32 %) or not being able to contact potential participants after three attempts (12 %). The Moroccan sample consisted of 995 individuals of whom 12 % had incorrect address information. Of the 872 persons who could be traced, 476 participated in the study (55 %). Within the non-response group, 26 % refused to participate and approximately 19 % could not be reached after three attempts. The study population is similar to the Turkish and Moroccan population aged 10–30 years living in Amsterdam, according to sex, generational status (country of birth) and city district; except for the Moroccan male population, among whom the age category 20–30 years is under-represented, and participants from one city district being slightly over-represented.

For the current study we analysed participants aged 18–30 years (n 567). We excluded respondents with missing information on body weight perception (n 50) and BMI (n 66) to end with a total sample size of 451.

Data collection

Face-to-face interviews were held from April 2003 until December 2004, by trained interviewers of the same ethnic background and sex. A structured questionnaire was used; this was available in Dutch, Turkish or Moroccan Arabic so that the interview could be held in the participants’ language of preference. Included were questions about demographics, socio-economic status and migration status. Acculturation was evaluated using a proxy measure, place of birth (generation), as well as scale measures of the level of social contact with ethnic Dutch (three questions on social contacts in leisure time) and orientation towards Dutch society (ten items measuring language and media use, shopping preference and emancipation as examples of Western norms and values). The data collection strategy, the questionnaire design and the acculturation measures have been fully described elsewhere(Reference Hosper, Klazinga and Stronks42).

Body size preference and body weight perception

Body size preference was evaluated using seven silhouette drawings developed by Collins(Reference Collins43). Respondents were asked to select the silhouette that most corresponded to they way they currently look and also to select the ideal silhouette for themselves. A discrepancy score was calculated by subtracting the ideal from the current silhouette as was done by Fallon and Rozin(Reference Fallon and Rozin44). Participants were also asked to select ideals for members of the same sex and opposite sex.

Body weight perception was evaluated by how participants described their body weight; answer categories included ‘thin’, ‘average’ or ‘overweight’. Participants received no specific instructions relating to this question, therefore the answers obtained are not standardised but reflect individuals’ personal evaluations. Finally, we asked ‘Do you pay attention to your weight?’ Possible answers were either ‘yes’ or ‘no’. We formulated this question in a neutral way to avoid the association with dieting to lose weight.

Measurement of body weight

Participants were weighed and measured during the home visit. Weight was measured using an electronic scale to the nearest 0·1 kg after removal of shoes, jackets, heavier clothing and pocket contents. Height was measured twice without shoes in an upright position with a measuring tape and ruler to the nearest 0·1 cm. We calculated BMI as weight divided by the square of height (kg/m2). Underweight was classified as having a BMI lower than 18·5 kg/m2. There were six participants who fell into this category (two Moroccan women, two Turkish women, one Moroccan man and one Turkish man). We chose to include them together with the normal-weight group for the analyses. Due to logistic problems not all participants could be weighed during the interview. In these cases (40 % of all participants) weight and height were based on self-report. We performed additional analyses to explore the impact on our results. We found no difference in the prevalence of overweight between self-reporters and the weighed group. We also tested mean BMI between the two groups using the Mann–Whitney test and found no differences in BMI between them, with the exception of Moroccan women where median BMI was 22·3 kg/m2 in the self-reporters v. 24·1 kg/m2 in the measured group (P = 0·05). Finally, we stratified according to self-report and compared on the basis of demographics, socio-economic status, acculturation variables and perception variables, but found no significant differences between them. We therefore decided to include the self-reporters in the analyses in order to maximise our sample size.

Statistical analyses

We analysed data using the Statistical Package for the Social Sciences statistical software package version 12 (SPSS Inc., Chicago, IL, USA). The sample was stratified according to sex and ethnicity for all analyses. For categorical variables we applied the χ 2 test. Differences in body size preference and weight awareness between groups of respondents were tested using the Mann–Whitney test. Differences within groups were tested using the Wilcoxon signed-rank test. Spearman’s rank correlations were calculated for the relationships of current and ideal silhouettes (body size preference) and BMI. In order to explore the association with acculturation we performed the analyses with the ‘ideal’ silhouette as dependent variable.

Results

Table 1 summarises the participants’ characteristics. There were some statistically significant differences between the groups. Fewer Moroccan men were married or had children compared with the other groups. Within the ethnic groups, more women were married, had children and were not in paid employment. Moroccan men and women had a slightly higher education level than the Turkish participants although this was not statistically significant (P = 0·08 in men and P = 0·07 in women). BMI values ranged from 15·6 to 36·7 kg/m2. Women were more often first-generation migrants. The majority of participants had a low level of social contact with ethnic Dutch (22·1–33·9 % had high a level of contact) with no significant differences between the groups. More Moroccans than Turks were highly culturally oriented towards Dutch society (42·0 % v. 25·6 % of women and 48·2 % v. 35·5 % of men, P < 0·001), but this was still less than half of the Moroccan group. More Turkish men were overweight (35·5 %) and the prevalence of overweight and obesity was 37·6 % among Turkish women and 34·6 % among Moroccan women.

Table 1 Participant characteristics: male and female migrants of non-Western origin residing in Amsterdam, 2003–2004

Education level: 1 = never been to school or primary school only; 2 = lower vocational and lower secondary education; 3 = intermediate vocational and intermediate/higher secondary education; 4 = higher vocational education and university.

*Within ethnic groups, women more likely to be from first generation, be married and have children, P < 0·01.

†Turkish participants had lower cultural orientation than Moroccan participants, P < 0·001.

No other significant differences between Turkish and Moroccan participants (P>0·05).

Table 2 shows the results of body size preference, body weight perception and the percentage of participants who pay attention to their weight. Men’s preferred size was similar to their current size whereas women selected an ideal that was significantly smaller than their current size. Based on the calculated discrepancy between current and ideal silhouette, the majority of women (60·3 % Turkish and 57·7 % Moroccan) preferred a smaller size for themselves whereas this was less the case among the men, 21·8 % and 29·0 % for the Moroccan and Turkish men respectively.

Table 2 Body size preference, body weight perception and percentage paying attention to own weight: male and female migrants of non-Western origin residing in Amsterdam, 2003–2004

*Difference between ideal body size and current size significant for women, P < 0·001.

†Ideal for others larger than ideal for self: men P < 0·05, women P < 0·001.

‡Turkish men and women had smaller ideal for the opposite sex than Moroccan participants, P < 0·05.

§Based on discrepancy in silhouettes chosen to represent ideal and current size.

All groups chose a thinner ideal size for themselves than for others of their own sex. Further, Turkish men and women had a significantly thinner ideal for members of the opposite sex than did Moroccans. Most of the men (67·7–70·9 %) and almost half the women (47·1–51·6 %) perceived their body weight to be ‘average’. The majority of women (60·2–69·0 %) and Turkish men (56·8 %) reported that they pay attention to their weight, whereas less than half (45·5 %) of Moroccan men did so.

Table 3 shows mean BMI according to silhouette selected to represent current body size. The correlation coefficient for BMI and current silhouette was low but statistically significant in all groups. We tested ethnic differences and found that per silhouette Moroccan women had lower BMI than Turkish women (P < 0·05), while there were no differences between the men.

Table 3 Mean BMI of participants according silhouette they selected to represent current body size: male and female migrants of non-Western origin residing in Amsterdam, 2003–2004

Ethnic groups combined: in men Spearman’s ρ = 0·60, P < 0·001; in women Spearman’s ρ = 0·73, P < 0·001.

*Significant difference between women in actual BMI according to selected silhouette, P < 0·05.

Figure 1 shows body weight perception according to BMI category. Here we see that a relatively large proportion of men who are overweight (BMI = 25·0–29·9 kg/m2) described themselves as ‘average’, 63·0 % of Turkish men and 82·0 % of Moroccan men. One-third of overweight Turkish and Moroccan women respectively described themselves as ‘average’.

Fig. 1 Body weight perception (▒, perceives self as thin; ░, perceives self as overweight) according to actual BMI in Turkish (Turk.) and Moroccan (Moroc.) male and female migrants residing in Amsterdam, 2003–2004. *Numbers insufficient (n 2) to present results

Table 4 shows that body size preference did not differ according to generation status, level of cultural orientation to Dutch society or level of social contacts with ethnic Dutch. Among Turkish women who have more contacts with ethnic Dutch the ideal silhouette was slightly thinner (P = 0·06). Among second-generation men the ideal was slightly larger (P = 0·06 in Turkish men and P = 0·09 in Moroccan men).

Table 4 Acculturation and body size preference (mean silhouette chosen to represent ideal): male and female migrants of non-Western origin residing in Amsterdam, 2003–2004

None of the differences by acculturation variables were statistically significant (P>0·05), although there were some borderline significant results as follows.

*P = 0·06 in Turkish men, P = 0·09 in Moroccan men.

†Turkish women, P = 0·06.

Table 5 shows self-reported weight behaviour offset against the main outcome variables. There were no statistically significant differences between participants who reported watching their weight v. those who did not on the basis of preferred body size (mean silhouette), body size discrepancy (difference between current and ideal size) or overweight perception.

Table 5 Pays attention to body weight v. body size preference and body weight perception: male and female migrants of non-Western origin residing in Amsterdam, 2003–2004

No = does not pay attention to body weight; Yes = pays attention to body weight.

*Based on discrepancy in silhouettes chosen to represent ideal and current size.

As the findings followed a similar trend for both ethnic groups we combined them and stratified according to sex in order to increase the power of our calculations. We found that more women reported paying attention to their weight if their preferred body size was smaller than their current body size (χ 2 = 6·7, P = 0·01). Among men, this was the case if they perceived themselves to be overweight (χ 2 = 8·8, P = 0·01).

Discussion

The present population-based study is the first to offer insight into the issue of body size preference and body weight perception among these two important European non-Western migrant groups. In our study we did not observe a preference for a large body size; in contrast, this group of Turkish and Moroccan participants selected relatively thin silhouettes to represent their ideal. The silhouette selected to represent current body size was correlated with BMI, indicating that individuals’ perception of their body size is realistic. As is generally found in Western populations, the preferred body size among women was significantly thinner than their current size whereas this was not the case among men. Furthermore, a large proportion of overweight and obese men perceived their weight to be ‘average’, which may indicate greater acceptance of overweight. A discrepancy between ideal and current size was associated with paying attention to body weight in women. Among men, paying attention to body weight was associated with the perception of overweight. Finally, we did not observe an association between body size preference and acculturation variables such as generation status, social contacts with Dutch or orientation with Dutch society.

The study has some limitations that merit mention. First the response rate was quite low, 57 % among Turks and 55 % among Moroccans, which means that our study population may not be representative of Turkish and Moroccan residents of The Netherlands. However, the response is comparable to that obtained by other studies of the same groups(Reference Dijkshoorn, Uitenbroek and Middelkoop9, Reference Agyemang, Ujcic-Voortman, Uitenbroek, Foets and Droomers45), and our participants are comparable to the Turkish and Moroccan population living in Amsterdam. Second, we used a combination of both measured and self-reported data of weight and height. We tested the effect of this methodological issue but found no differences in the main outcomes between the self-reporters and the weighed group, indicating limited impact on our main conclusions. Third, we cannot rule out the possibility that participants gave socially desirable answers. As culture is likely to influence body size preference, our use of interviewers matched for sex and ethnicity may have minimised this possibility. Fourth, we cannot place the results from the study within the context of their host environment as we did not include ethnic Dutch participants. It may be that ethnic Dutch men and women prefer even thinner bodies. A study of US residents aged 18–30 years that used similar figural stimuli showed that men tended to prefer figure 4 and women figure 3(Reference Bulik, Wade, Heath, Martin, Stunkard and Eaves46). This preference is not dissimilar to the silhouettes chosen by participants in our study. However it is not certain whether this population can be used as a reference considering that overweight and obesity prevalence is higher in the USA than in The Netherlands(Reference Schokker, Visscher, Nooyens, van Baak and Seidell7, Reference Ogden, Carroll, Curtin, McDowell, Tabak and Flegal47). It may be that the overweight/obesity prevalence in the general population may influence body size preference as speculated by Cash et al. in their study of changes of body image among US college students(Reference Cash, Morrow, Hrabosky and Perry48) and by Bennett et al. in a study of attitudes towards overweight in African Americans(Reference Bennett, Wolin, Goodman, Samplin-Salgado, Carter, Dutton, Hill and Emmons49). Finally, although the figural stimuli used in our study have not been validated for the Turkish and Moroccan populations, similar instruments have been widely used in various study populations which lends support to our choice of instrument(Reference Al-Sendi, Shetty and Musaiger19, Reference Jackson, Rashed and Saad-Eldin20, Reference Mciza, Goedecke, Steyn, Charlton, Puoane, Meltzer, Levitt and Lambert25, Reference Cachelin, Striegel-Moore and Elder50).

Much of the research conducted in this area originates in the USA and compares white American, African American, Hispanic and Asian groups. The results and conclusions have been mixed but two recent meta-analyses have shown that ethnic differences seem to be diminishing: Roberts et al. found that differences in body dissatisfaction between African American and white women were converging(Reference Roberts, Cash, Feingold and Johnson51); Grabe and Hyde found that ethnic differences in the perception of body weight between women were small(Reference Grabe and Hyde30). Our finding that that these groups of Turkish and Moroccan origin have a preference for thinner body size is consistent with the idea that a preference for thinness is common, particularly among women.

In addition, our study indicates that this preference is not related to acculturation, as might have been expected based on the findings of others(Reference Cachelin, Monreal and Juarez22, Reference Bush, Williams, Lean and Anderson34, Reference Furnham and Alibhai36, Reference Olvera, Suminski and Power52). However the comparison of these findings with ours is problematic due to differences in the measurement of acculturation. Furthermore, we measured a limited number of indicators of acculturation; it may be that other dimensions of acculturation (e.g. ethnic identity) and migration-related issues (e.g. acculturative stress) are more relevant for body size preference and the associated consequences thereof(Reference Perez, Voelz, Pettit and Joiner53, Reference Reddy and Crowther54). The issue of methodology has been highlighted by others and remains a barrier to understanding the relationships between acculturation, body size preference and body dissatisfaction(Reference Soh, Touyz and Surgenor55, Reference Wildes, Emery and Simons56).

Our finding that a large proportion of women of non-Western origin experience a discrepancy between desired and actual body size is similar to those among Western populations(Reference Blokstra, Burns and Seidell14, Reference Madrigal, Sanchez-Villegas, Martinez-Gonzalez, Kearney, Gibney, Irala and Martínez15). Interestingly, although the majority of women chose a thinner ideal body size for themselves (57·7–60·3 %), fewer women perceived themselves as ‘overweight’ (35·7–45·2 %), which implies that wanting to be thinner is not always related to the belief that one is overweight. Fitzgibbon et al.(Reference Fitzgibbon, Blackman and Avellone32) argue that these are two distinct issues, the discrepancy between the ideal and current body size is more likely to reflect body dissatisfaction and may be a factor that stimulates attention to weight-loss activities. Our observation that a larger majority of women pay attention to their weight if their perceived size is larger than their desired size supports this idea. Body dissatisfaction, whether appropriate or not, may be a stimulus for weight-watching behaviour among women and may contribute to reduced quality of life(Reference Burns, Tijhuis and Seidell57) as well as to eating pathology(Reference Madrigal, Sanchez-Villegas, Martinez-Gonzalez, Kearney, Gibney, Irala and Martínez15, Reference Bennett and Wolin31). The latter does not appear to differ for women of non-Western origin(Reference Fitzgibbon, Blackman and Avellone32). Further research of this issue among these population groups is warranted. But in the meantime, interventions should be sensitive for potentially negative effects on women’s dieting practices.

Two studies in Europe have shown a lower awareness of overweight among migrant-origin groups(Reference Brug, Wammes, Kremers, Giskes and Oenema33), including South Asian groups(Reference Patel, Bhopal, Unwin, White, Alberti and Yallop35). Within our populations we observed that a high proportion of overweight men (63–82 %) and overweight women (35 %) perceived themselves as average. Although we cannot compare our group to the general population, the finding that more men than women are unaware of their overweight is consistent with that of others(Reference Blokstra, Burns and Seidell14, Reference Paeratakul, White, Williamson, Ryan and Bray58). As mentioned previously, in a setting where overweight and obesity are common, overweight individuals may be more likely to experience themselves as being ‘average’. This effect may be stronger among men whereas among women the socio-cultural stigma associated with overweight may dominate. Considering that paying attention to body weight is more prevalent among men who perceive themselves as overweight, it appears that raising awareness of weight status may be an important stimulus for weight-watching behaviour among men, including those of non-Western origin.

Although we did not aim to compare Turks and Moroccans with each other, we did observe some differences between these groups. Per silhouette selected to represent current body size Moroccan women had lower BMI levels. In other words, they viewed themselves as ‘larger’ than did Turkish women. In addition, Turkish women preferred a smaller ideal silhouette than did Moroccan women and both Turkish men and women preferred smaller sizes in members of the opposite sex. This apparently stronger preference for thinner body sizes among Turkish participants could be reflected in their more ‘optimistic’ choice of silhouette to represent themselves. Our finding of a thin body size ideal among this group is in line with a study in Turkey which showed that the desire to be thinner in Turkish adolescents is similar to that among adolescents in developed countries(Reference Canpolat, Orsel, Akdemir and Ozbay59). Among Moroccans there may be a greater acceptance for larger body sizes. As literature from Morocco indicates that adiposity in women is associated with higher social status(Reference Benjelloun16, Reference Mokhtar, Elati, Chabir, Bour, Elkari, Schlossman, Caballero and Aguenaou17), it may be that Moroccans may be less concerned with thinness as an ideal. However, this is not entirely consistent with our finding that 45 % of Moroccan women perceived themselves as ‘overweight’ even though the prevalence of overweight and obesity in this group was 36·6 % and that the majority of women, 57·7 %, preferred a smaller body size. Comparable data from Turkey and Morocco would help in understanding these results.

Conclusions

In this first exploration of body size preference and body weight perception in young adults of Turkish and Moroccan origin, we did not observe a preference for large body sizes. Similar to Western populations, most women wished to be thinner than they were whereas this was less the case among men, the majority of whom were also unaware of being overweight.

Among the majority of men raising awareness of overweight appears to be an important intervention goal. However, caution is advisable when targeting women. Although a third do not perceive themselves to be overweight, the majority exhibit dissatisfaction with their body size which may make them susceptible to inappropriate dieting practices.

Acknowledgements

The LASER study was funded by The Netherlands Heart Foundation and The Netherlands Organisation for Health Research and Development (NWO, ZonMw). We declare no conflict of interests.

Authors’ contributions: M.N., K.S. and K.H. designed the study. M.N. conducted the analysis and wrote the original draft with input from all the other authors.

References

1.Visscher, TL & Seidell, JC (2001) The public health impact of obesity. Annu Rev Public Health 22, 355375.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
2.World Health Organization (2004) Obesity and overweight. In Global Strategy on Diet, Physical Activity and Health. Geneva: WHO.Google Scholar
3.International Organization for Migration (2004) World Migration 2005: Costs and Benefits of International Migration. Geneva: IOM.Google Scholar
4.Kumar, BN, Meyer, HE, Wandel, M, Dalen, I & Holmboe-Ottesen, G (2006) Ethnic differences in obesity among immigrants from developing countries, in Oslo, Norway. Int J Obes (Lond) 30, 684690.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
5.Tremblay, MS, Perez, CE, Ardern, CI, Bryan, SN & Katzmarzyk, PT (2005) Obesity, overweight and ethnicity. Health Rep 16, 2334.Google ScholarPubMed
6.Wandell, PE, Hjorleifsdottir, SK & Johansson, SE (2003) Diabetes mellitus in Turkish immigrants in Sweden. Diabetes Metab 29, 435439.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
7.Schokker, DF, Visscher, TL, Nooyens, AC, van Baak, MA & Seidell, JC (2007) Prevalence of overweight and obesity in the Netherlands. Obes Rev 8, 101107.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
8.Brussaard, JH, van Erp-Baart, MA, Brants, HA, Hulshof, KF & Lowik, MR (2001) Nutrition and health among migrants in The Netherlands. Public Health Nutr 4, 659664.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
9.Dijkshoorn, H, Uitenbroek, DG & Middelkoop, BJ (2003) [Prevalence of diabetes mellitus and cardiovascular disease among immigrants from Turkey and Morocco and the indigenous Dutch population]. Ned Tijdschr Geneeskd 147, 13621366.Google ScholarPubMed
10.Wammes, B, Breedveld, B, Looman, C & Brug, J (2005) The impact of a national mass media campaign in The Netherlands on the prevention of weight gain. Public Health Nutr 8, 12501257.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
11.Rodin, J (1993) Cultural and psychosocial determinants of weight concerns. Ann Intern Med 119, 643645.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
12.Stice, E (1994) Review of the evidence for a sociocultural model of bulimia-nervosa and an exploration of the mechanisms of action. Clin Psychol Rev 14, 633661.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
13.Pope, HG Jr, Olivardia, R, Gruber, A & Borowiecki, J (1999) Evolving ideals of male body image as seen through action toys. Int J Eat Disord 26, 6572.3.0.CO;2-D>CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
14.Blokstra, A, Burns, CM & Seidell, JC (1999) Perception of weight status and dieting behaviour in Dutch men and women. Int J Obes Relat Metab Disord 23, 717.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
15.Madrigal, H, Sanchez-Villegas, A, Martinez-Gonzalez, MA, Kearney, J, Gibney, MJ, Irala, J & Martínez, JA (2000) Underestimation of body mass index through perceived body image as compared to self-reported body mass index in the European Union. Public Health 114, 468473.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
16.Benjelloun, S (2002) Nutrition transition in Morocco. Public Health Nutr 5, 135140.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
17.Mokhtar, N, Elati, J, Chabir, R, Bour, A, Elkari, K, Schlossman, NP, Caballero, B & Aguenaou, H (2001) Diet culture and obesity in northern Africa. J Nutr 131, 887S892S.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
18.Becker, AE, Gilman, SE & Burwell, RA (2005) Changes in prevalence of overweight and in body image among Fijian women between 1989 and 1998. Obes Res 13, 110117.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
19.Al-Sendi, AM, Shetty, P & Musaiger, AO (2004) Body weight perception among Bahraini adolescents. Child Care Health Dev 30, 369376.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
20.Jackson, RT, Rashed, M & Saad-Eldin, R (2003) Rural urban differences in weight, body image, and dieting behavior among adolescent Egyptian schoolgirls. Int J Food Sci Nutr 54, 111.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
21.Simeon, DT, Rattan, RD, Panchoo, K, Kungeesingh, KV, Ali, AC & Abdool, PS (2003) Body image of adolescents in a multi-ethnic Caribbean population. Eur J Clin Nutr 57, 157162.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
22.Cachelin, FM, Monreal, TK & Juarez, LC (2006) Body image and size perceptions of Mexican American women. Body Image 3, 6775.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
23.Redfield, R, Linton, R & Heskovitz, MJ (1935) Outline for the study of acculturation. Am Anthropol N Series 9, 149152.Google Scholar
24.Berry, JW (1997) Immigration, acculturation and adaptation. Appl Psychol 46, 568.Google Scholar
25.Mciza, Z, Goedecke, JH, Steyn, NP, Charlton, K, Puoane, T, Meltzer, S, Levitt, NS & Lambert, EV (2005) Development and validation of instruments measuring body image and body weight dissatisfaction in South African mothers and their daughters. Public Health Nutr 8, 509519.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
26.Rasheed, P (1998) Perception of body weight and self-reported eating and exercise behaviour among obese and non-obese women in Saudi Arabia. Public Health 112, 409414.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
27.Siervo, M, Grey, P, Nyan, OA & Prentice, AM (2006) A pilot study on body image, attractiveness and body size in Gambians living in an urban community. Eat Weight Disord 11, 100109.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
28.Shih, MY & Kubo, C (2005) Body shape preference and body satisfaction of Taiwanese and Japanese female college students. Psychiatry Res 133, 263271.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
29.Cachelin, FM, Rebeck, RM, Chung, GH & Pelayo, E (2002) Does ethnicity influence body-size preference? A comparison of body image and body size. Obes Res 10, 158166.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
30.Grabe, S & Hyde, JS (2006) Ethnicity and body dissatisfaction among women in the United States: a meta-analysis. Psychol Bull 132, 622640.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
31.Bennett, GG & Wolin, KY (2006) Satisfied or unaware? Racial differences in perceived weight status. Int J Behav Nutr Phys Act 3, 40.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
32.Fitzgibbon, ML, Blackman, LR & Avellone, ME (2000) The relationship between body image discrepancy and body mass index across ethnic groups. Obes Res 8, 582589.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
33.Brug, J, Wammes, B, Kremers, S, Giskes, K & Oenema, A (2006) Underestimation and overestimation of personal weight status: associations with socio-demographic characteristics and weight maintenance intentions. J Hum Nutr Diet 19, 253262.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
34.Bush, HM, Williams, RG, Lean, ME & Anderson, AS (2001) Body image and weight consciousness among South Asian, Italian and general population women in Britain. Appetite 37, 207215.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
35.Patel, S, Bhopal, R, Unwin, N, White, M, Alberti, KG & Yallop, J (2001) Mismatch between perceived and actual overweight in diabetic and non-diabetic populations: a comparative study of South Asian and European women. J Epidemiol Community Health 55, 332333.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
36.Furnham, A & Alibhai, N (1983) Cross-cultural differences in the perception of female body shapes. Psychol Med 13, 829837.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
37. de Beer J & Harmsen C (2003) Ethnic marriages. http://www.cbs.nl/ (accessed January 2008).Google Scholar
38. Research and Statistics (2007) Population of Amsterdam according to ethnic origin. http://www.os.amsterdam.nl/ (accessed January 2008).Google Scholar
39.Rguibi, M & Belahsen, R (2006) Body size preferences and sociocultural influences on attitudes towards obesity among Moroccan Sahraoui women. Body Image 3, 395400.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
40.Yumuk, VD (2005) Prevalence of obesity in Turkey. Obes Rev 6, 910.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
41.Ertem, M, Bahceci, M, Tuzcu, A, Saka, G, Ozturk, U & Gokalp, D (2008) The association between high parity and obesity in women living in South-eastern Turkey. Eat Weight Disord 13, e4e7.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
42.Hosper, K, Klazinga, NS & Stronks, K (2007) Acculturation does not necessarily lead to increased physical activity during leisure time: a cross-sectional study among Turkish young people in the Netherlands. BMC Public Health 7, 230.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
43.Collins, ME (1991) Body figure perceptions and preferences among preadolescent children. Int J Eat Disord 10, 199208.3.0.CO;2-D>CrossRefGoogle Scholar
44.Fallon, AE & Rozin, P (1985) Sex differences in perceptions of desirable body shape. J Abnorm Psychol 94, 102105.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
45.Agyemang, C, Ujcic-Voortman, J, Uitenbroek, D, Foets, M & Droomers, M (2006) Prevalence and management of hypertension among Turkish, Moroccan and native Dutch ethnic groups in Amsterdam, the Netherlands: The Amsterdam Health Monitor Survey. J Hypertens 24, 21692176.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
46.Bulik, CM, Wade, TD, Heath, AC, Martin, NG, Stunkard, AJ & Eaves, LJ (2001) Relating body mass index to figural stimuli: population-based normative data for Caucasians. Int J Obes Relat Metab Disord 25, 15171524.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
47.Ogden, CL, Carroll, MD, Curtin, LR, McDowell, MA, Tabak, CJ & Flegal, KM (2006) Prevalence of overweight and obesity in the United States, 1999–2004. JAMA 295, 15491555.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
48.Cash, TF, Morrow, JA, Hrabosky, JI & Perry, AA (2004) How has body image changed? A cross-sectional investigation of college women and men from 1983 to 2001. J Consult Clin Psychol 72, 10811089.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
49.Bennett, GG, Wolin, KY, Goodman, M, Samplin-Salgado, M, Carter, P, Dutton, S, Hill, R & Emmons, K (2006) Attitudes regarding overweight, exercise, and health among blacks (United States). Cancer Causes Control 17, 95101.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
50.Cachelin, FM, Striegel-Moore, RH & Elder, KA (1998) Realistic weight perception and body size assessment in a racially diverse community sample of dieters. Obes Res 6, 6268.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
51.Roberts, A, Cash, TF, Feingold, A & Johnson, BT (2006) Are black–white differences in females’ body dissatisfaction decreasing? A meta-analytic review. J Consult Clin Psychol 74, 11211131.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
52.Olvera, N, Suminski, R & Power, TG (2005) Intergenerational perceptions of body image in Hispanics: role of BMI, gender, and acculturation. Obes Res 13, 19701979.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
53.Perez, M, Voelz, ZR, Pettit, JW & Joiner, TE Jr (2002) The role of acculturative stress and body dissatisfaction in predicting bulimic symptomatology across ethnic groups. Int J Eat Disord 31, 442454.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
54.Reddy, SD & Crowther, JH (2007) Teasing, acculturation, and cultural conflict: psychosocial correlates of body image and eating attitudes among South Asian women. Cultur Divers Ethnic Minor Psychol 13, 4553.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
55.Soh, NL, Touyz, SW & Surgenor, LJ (2006) Eating and body image disturbances across cultures: a review. Eur Eat Disord Rev 14, 5465.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
56.Wildes, JE, Emery, RE & Simons, AD (2001) The roles of ethnicity and culture in the development of eating disturbance and body dissatisfaction: a meta-analytic review. Clin Psychol Rev 21, 521551.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
57.Burns, CM, Tijhuis, MA & Seidell, JC (2001) The relationship between quality of life and perceived body weight and dieting history in Dutch men and women. Int J Obes Relat Metab Disord 25, 13861392.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
58.Paeratakul, S, White, MA, Williamson, DA, Ryan, DH & Bray, GA (2002) Sex, race/ethnicity, socioeconomic status, and BMI in relation to self-perception of overweight. Obes Res 10, 345350.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
59.Canpolat, BI, Orsel, S, Akdemir, A & Ozbay, MH (2005) The relationship between dieting and body image, body ideal, self-perception, and body mass index in Turkish adolescents. Int J Eat Disord 37, 150155.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Figure 0

Table 1 Participant characteristics: male and female migrants of non-Western origin residing in Amsterdam, 2003–2004

Figure 1

Table 2 Body size preference, body weight perception and percentage paying attention to own weight: male and female migrants of non-Western origin residing in Amsterdam, 2003–2004

Figure 2

Table 3 Mean BMI of participants according silhouette they selected to represent current body size: male and female migrants of non-Western origin residing in Amsterdam, 2003–2004

Figure 3

Fig. 1 Body weight perception (▒, perceives self as thin; ░, perceives self as overweight) according to actual BMI in Turkish (Turk.) and Moroccan (Moroc.) male and female migrants residing in Amsterdam, 2003–2004. *Numbers insufficient (n 2) to present results

Figure 4

Table 4 Acculturation and body size preference (mean silhouette chosen to represent ideal): male and female migrants of non-Western origin residing in Amsterdam, 2003–2004

Figure 5

Table 5 Pays attention to body weight v. body size preference and body weight perception: male and female migrants of non-Western origin residing in Amsterdam, 2003–2004