Hostname: page-component-78c5997874-g7gxr Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-11-18T05:23:33.716Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

Medical Research Council Developmental Psychology Unit

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  09 July 2009

Extract

In 1967 a proposal was made by the Medical Research Council that a unit be set up to consider the scholastic and behavioural skills of handicapped children and adolescents. In the beginning, three groups of studies were envisaged—those concerned with visual displays and orientation, those concerned with verbal ability and its development, and those concerned with immediate memory and the organization of perception.

Type
Research Report
Copyright
Copyright © Cambridge University Press 1975

Access options

Get access to the full version of this content by using one of the access options below. (Log in options will check for institutional or personal access. Content may require purchase if you do not have access.)

References

REFERENCES

Attneave, F., and Benson, B. (1969). Spatial coding of tactual stimulation. Journal of Experimental Psychology, 81, 216222.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Chomsky, N. (1957). Syntactic Structures. Mouton: Hague.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Cromer, R. F. (1970). ‘Children are nice to understand’: surface structure clues for the recovery of a deep structure. British Journal of Psychology, 61, 397408.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Cromer, R. F. (1975). An experimental investigation of a putative linguistic universal: marking and the indirect object. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology. (In press.)CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Dodd, B. (1972). Comparison of babbling patterns in normal and Down-syndrome infants. Journal of Mental Deficiency Research, 16, 3540.Google ScholarPubMed
Frith, U. (1970). Studies in pattern detection in normal and autistic children. I. Immediate recall of auditory sequences. Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 76, 412420.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Frith, U. (1971). Why do children reverse letters? British Journal of Psychology. 62, 459468.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Frith, U. (1974). Perceptual coding and classification. In International Association for the Scientific Study of Mental Deficiency Proceedings of the Third CongressThe Hague4–12 September 1973, pp. 496499. Edited by Primrose, D. A.. Polish Medical Publishers of Warsaw: Warsaw.Google Scholar
Frith, U. (1974a). Internal schemata for letters in good and bad readers. British Journal of Psychology, 65, 233241.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Frith, U. (1974b). Scanning for reversed and rotated targets. Acta Psychologica, 38, 343349.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Gibson, E. J., Osser, H., Schiff, W., and Smith, J. (1963). An Analysis of Critical Features of Letters Tested by a Confusion of Matrix. Final Report on Cooperative Research Project No. 639. Office of Education, Department of Health, Education and Welfare: Washington.Google Scholar
Hebb, D. O. (1949). The Organization of Behavior. Wiley: New York.Google Scholar
Hermelin, B., and O'Connor, N. (1971). Spatial coding in normal, autistic and blind children. Perceptual and Motor Skills, 53, 127132.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Hermelin, B., and O'Connor, N. (1973). Ordering in recognition memory after ambiguous initial or recognition displays. Canadian Journal of Psychology, 27, 191199.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Hirsh, I. J., Bilger, R. C., and Deatherage, B. H. (1956). The effect of auditory and visual background on apparent duration. American Journal of Psychology, 69, 561574.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Hubel, D. H., and Wiesel, T. N. (1962). Receptive fields, binocular interaction and functional architecture in the cat's visual cortex. Journal of Physiology, 160, 106154.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Hughes, J. (1975). Acquisition of non-vocal ‘language’ by aphasic children. Cognition. (In press.)Google Scholar
Hull, C. L. (1943). Principles of Behavior. Appleton: New York.Google Scholar
Jakobson, R. (1941). Child Language: aphasia and Phonological Universals. Mouton: Hague.Google Scholar
Medical Research Council (1973). Medical Research Council Handbook. 1973–74. Medical Research Council: London.Google Scholar
Neisser, U. (1967). Cognitive Psychology. Appleton: New York.Google Scholar
O'Connor, N., and Hermelin, B. (1972). Seeing and hearing and space and time. Perception and Psychophysics, 11, 4648.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
O'Connor, N., and Hermelin, B. (1973). The spatial or temporal organization of short-term memory. Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 25, 335343.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Paivio, I. (1971). Imagery and Verbal Processes. Holt: New York.Google Scholar
Pavlov, I. P. (1927). Conditioned Reflexes. Translated and edited by Anrep, G. V.. Oxford University Press: London.Google Scholar
Sloboda, J. (1974). The eye hand span—an approach to the study of sight reading. Psychology of Music, 2, 410.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Smith, N. (1973). The Acquisition of Phonology: A Case Study. Cambridge University Press: London.Google Scholar
Spence, K. W. (1936). The nature of discrimination learning in animals. Psychological Review, 43, 427449.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Sutherland, N. S. (1959). Stimulus analysing mechanisms. In The Mechanisation of Thought Processes. Proceedings of Symposium No. 2 held at the National Physical Laboratory 1958.HMSO:London.Google Scholar