Hostname: page-component-8448b6f56d-42gr6 Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-04-20T02:55:54.130Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

Hippocampal volume and volume asymmetry prospectively predict PTSD symptom emergence among Iraq-deployed soldiers

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  22 November 2021

Adam R. Cobb
Affiliation:
Department of Psychology, The University of Texas at Austin, Austin, TX, USA Department of Psychiatry and Behavioral Sciences, Medical University of South Carolina, Charleston, SC, USA PTSD Clinical Team, Ralph H. Johnson VA Medical Center, Charleston, SC, USA
Mikael Rubin
Affiliation:
Department of Psychology, The University of Texas at Austin, Austin, TX, USA
Deborah L. Stote
Affiliation:
Department of Psychology, The University of Texas at Austin, Austin, TX, USA
Brian C. Baldwin
Affiliation:
Department of Psychology, The University of Texas at Austin, Austin, TX, USA
Han-Joo Lee
Affiliation:
Department of Psychology, University of Wisconsin-Milwaukee, Milwaukee, WI, USA
Ahmad R. Hariri
Affiliation:
Department of Psychology and Neuroscience, Duke University, Durham, NC, USA
Michael J. Telch*
Affiliation:
Department of Psychology, The University of Texas at Austin, Austin, TX, USA
*
Author for correspondence: Michael J. Telch, E-mail: telch@austin.utexas.edu
Rights & Permissions [Opens in a new window]

Abstract

Background

Evidence suggests a link between smaller hippocampal volume (HV) and post-traumatic stress disorder (PTSD). However, there has been little prospective research testing this question directly and it remains unclear whether smaller HV confers risk or is a consequence of traumatization and PTSD.

Methods

U.S. soldiers (N = 107) completed a battery of clinical assessments, including structural magnetic resonance imaging pre-deployment. Once deployed they completed monthly assessments of traumatic-stressors and symptoms. We hypothesized that smaller HV would potentiate the effects of traumatic stressors on PTSD symptoms in theater. Analyses evaluated whether total HV, lateral (right v. left) HV, or HV asymmetry (right – left) moderated the effects of stressor-exposure during deployment on PTSD symptoms.

Results

Findings revealed no interaction between total HV and average monthly traumatic-stressors on PTSD symptoms b = −0.028, p = 0.681 [95% confidence interval (CI) −0.167 to 0.100]. However, in the context of greater exposure to average monthly traumatic stressors, greater right HV was associated with fewer PTSD symptoms b = −0.467, p = 0.023 (95% CI −0.786 to −0.013), whereas greater left HV was unexpectedly associated with greater PTSD symptoms b = 0.435, p = 0.024 (95% CI 0.028–0.715).

Conclusions

Our findings highlight the importance of considering the complex role of HV, in particular HV asymmetry, in predicting the emergence of PTSD symptoms in response to war-zone trauma.

Type
Original Article
Creative Commons
Creative Common License - CCCreative Common License - BY
This is an Open Access article, distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution licence (http://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/4.0/), which permits unrestricted re-use, distribution and reproduction, provided the original article is properly cited.
Copyright
Copyright © The Author(s), 2021. Published by Cambridge University Press

Introduction

The hippocampus is a primary brain region implicated in neurobiological models of post-traumatic stress disorder (PTSD) (Pitman et al., Reference Pitman, Rasmusson, Koenen, Shin, Orr, Gilbertson and Liberzon2012), and there is extensive evidence for smaller hippocampal volume (HV) in PTSD (Karl et al., Reference Karl, Schaefer, Malta, Dörfel, Rohleder and Werner2006; Logue et al., Reference Logue, van Rooij, Dennis, Davis, Hayes, Stevens and Morey2018; O'Doherty, Chitty, Saddiqui, Bennett, & Lagopoulos, Reference O'Doherty, Chitty, Saddiqui, Bennett and Lagopoulos2015; Smith, Reference Smith2005; Woon, Sood, & Hedges, Reference Woon, Sood and Hedges2010). Smaller HV has been linked with PTSD following trauma exposure (Gilbertson et al., Reference Gilbertson, Shenton, Ciszewski, Kasai, Lasko, Orr and Pitman2002; Apfel et al., Reference Apfel, Ross, Hlavin, Meyerhoff, Metzler, Marmar and Neylan2011), whereas greater baseline HV has been shown to predict a favorable response to trauma-focused cognitive-behavioral therapy (Rubin et al., Reference Rubin, Shvil, Papini, Chhetry, Helpman, Markowitz and Neria2016; van Rooij et al., Reference van Rooij, Kennis and Vink2016). A common explanation for the link between smaller HV and PTSD is that trauma exposure causes HV reductions, which is plausible, given established evidence for stress-evoked glucocorticoid-mediated hippocampal atrophy (Sapolsky, Reference Sapolsky2000). An alternative, but not mutually exclusive explanation is supported by evidence suggesting smaller HV may potentially operate as a pre-existing risk factor (e.g. Gilbertson et al., Reference Gilbertson, Shenton, Ciszewski, Kasai, Lasko, Orr and Pitman2002). However, a lack of prospective studies has made it difficult to address the possibility that smaller HV confers risk as well and is not merely a consequence of trauma or the post-trauma emergence of PTSD symptoms.

Only one prior study has examined HV as a prospective risk factor for PTSD symptoms (Admon et al., Reference Admon, Leykin, Lubin, Engert, Andrews, Pruessner and Hendler2013). Israeli combat paramedics (N = 33) underwent structural brain imaging prior to and 18 months later during military service. Smaller HV was not shown to predict the later emergence of PTSD symptoms, whereas decreases in HV were associated with PTSD symptoms at follow-up. This suggests that instead of smaller HV reflecting pre-trauma susceptibility, reductions in HV may reflect the sequela of trauma or development of PTSD. This conclusion is consistent with the hypothesized contribution of stress-evoked hippocampal atrophy to stress-related psychopathology (Sapolsky, Reference Sapolsky2000), and prior cross-sectional findings of reduced HV in individuals with PTSD (O'Doherty et al., Reference O'Doherty, Chitty, Saddiqui, Bennett and Lagopoulos2015; Woon et al., Reference Woon, Sood and Hedges2010).

While existing evidence implies a causal relation between stress-evoked HV reductions and PTSD, a limitation of prior research is a lack of evaluating pathways through which reduced HV may cause PTSD. Diathesis-stress models provide more complete functional accounts of how individual differences moderate the impact of stress. These frameworks have been used to evaluate the risk for several forms of stress-related psychopathology and other adverse outcomes (Edmondson et al., Reference Edmondson, Kronish, Wasson, Giglio, Davidson and Whang2014; Elwood, Hahn, Olatunji, & Williams, Reference Elwood, Hahn, Olatunji and Williams2009), but have not been used to identify pre-trauma neurological factors that predict the development of PTSD. The key advantage of a diathesis-stress approach is the potential to inform how smaller HV might functionally relate to the development of PTSD symptoms. If smaller HV operates as a pre-trauma susceptibility factor, it should be expected to moderate the impact of traumatic stress.

The current investigation is one of a series from the Texas Combat Stress Risk Project, which aims to identify biological, cognitive, and psychosocial risk factors for war-zone stress-evoked psychopathology (Beevers, Lee, Wells, Ellis, & Telch, Reference Beevers, Lee, Wells, Ellis and Telch2011a; Beevers et al., Reference Beevers, Marti, Lee, Stote, Ferrell, Hariri and Telch2011b; Cobb, Josephs, Lancaster, Lee, & Telch, Reference Cobb, Josephs, Lancaster, Lee and Telch2018, Reference Cobb, Lancaster, Meyer, Lee and Telch2017; Disner et al., Reference Disner, Beevers, Lee, Ferrell, Hariri and Telch2013; Josephs, Cobb, Lancaster, Lee, & Telch, Reference Josephs, Cobb, Lancaster, Lee and Telch2017; Lancaster, Cobb, Lee, & Telch, Reference Lancaster, Cobb, Lee and Telch2016; Lee, Goudarzi, Baldwin, Rosenfield, & Telch, Reference Lee, Goudarzi, Baldwin, Rosenfield and Telch2011; Telch et al., Reference Telch, Beevers, Rosenfield, Lee, Reijntjes, Ferrell and Hariri2015, Reference Telch, Rosenfield, Lee and Pai2012) An exploratory analysis probed the effect of hippocampal asymmetry (the difference between left and right HV) as a moderator of war-zone stressors impact on the emergence of PTSD symptoms during war-zone deployment.

Methods and materials

Participants

Soldiers (N = 107) from the parent project were recruited from nine Army units (four combat service support units, four combat units, and one combat support unit) deployed from Ft. Hood to Iraq between August 2007 and August 2009. Unit leaders were absent from recruitment and briefing, which was provided by the PI (MJ Telch) and the project manager (BC Baldwin) in the presence of an ombudsman to ensure soldiers felt free to choose whether to participate. Participants were (1) ⩾ 18 years of age, with (2) no prior deployments, and (3) planned deployment within 3 months of study enrollment.

Of the 223 soldiers who attended the briefings, 184 consented to participate, six did not deploy, and one withdrew consent. Of the 177 remaining, 32 did not complete the neuroimaging assessment, 17 (13%) were excluded due to non-viable structural scans, and 21 did not complete in-theater assessments. Among the 107 included in the final sample, participants completed an average of 10.88 monthly in-theater assessments, with a range of deployment durations from 2 to 21 months (see Table 1 for demographics and descriptive statistics). There were no apparent patterns to missing data, including with respect to completing neuroimaging and in-theater assessments.

Table 1. Descriptive statistics

Note. Table 1 presents descriptive statistics for all modeled variables. Sex (male = 0) and Axis I psychopathology (0 = absent) were entered as dichotomous predictors, whereas all other predictors were z-transformed. CEL, Combat experiences log, completed monthly in theater; PTEs, potentially traumatic events; STAI, State-Trait Anxiety Inventory; CES-D-10, Center for Epidemiological Studies Depression Scale (10 items), administered monthly during deployment; SPRINT, Short Post-Traumatic Stress Disorder Rating Interview), administered monthly during deployment. L HV and R HV, left and right hippocampal volume (cm3), adjusted for total intracranial volume (ICV; cm3). R – L HV, right – left hippocampal volume. The threshold for soldiers that met possible PTSD at any point in their deployment reflects the standard cutoff for the SPRINT and was derived by calculating the maximum SPRINT score endorsed at any point during deployment.

Procedure

All study procedures were approved by the IRBs at Brooke Army Medical Center at Fort Sam Houston and the University of Texas at Austin. Prior to their deployment, soldiers completed an extensive pre-deployment assessment spanning biological, cognitive, and psychosocial domains, and a structured diagnostic interview at the University of Texas at Austin (see Lee et al., Reference Lee, Goudarzi, Baldwin, Rosenfield and Telch2011 for details). Once deployed to Iraq, soldiers received monthly email reminders prompting their completion of the Combat Experiences Log (19), which assessed the type and severity of war-zone stressors and stress-related symptoms during the previous month.

Pre-deployment assessment measures

Demographics, Trauma Exposure, and Lifetime Psychopathology. At pre-deployment, all soldiers provided demographics, and were assessed using the Structured Clinical Interview for DSM-IV Axis I Disorders (SCID-I-IV) by advanced doctoral students with > 1 year of assessment experience, supervised by the PI (MJ Telch). The SCID-I-IV was used to assess past and current psychopathology, and past exposure to trauma as defined by PTSD Criterion A.

Trait Anxiety. Trait anxiety was assessed with the State-Trait Anxiety Inventory (Spielberger, Gorsuch, & Lushene, Reference Spielberger, Gorsuch and Lushene1970). The STAI-T indexes the tendency to experience anxiety in multiple contexts, with items rated by frequency (0 = ‘Almost never; 3 = Almost always). It has been extensively validated (Bieling, Antony, & Swinson, Reference Bieling, Antony and Swinson1998). Internal consistency for this sample was excellent (α = 0.91).

Brain Imaging. Volumetric magnetic resonance imaging data were collected using a GE 3.0T Signa Excite scanner. An automated shim procedure was applied to minimize possible magnetic field inhomogeneities. Two high-resolution T1-weighted SPGR scans optimized for high contrast between gray, white, and CSF on a sagittal plane using 1.3 mm slice thickness (T = 5 ms, TE = 1.2 ms, flip angle = 11°) were acquired for morphometric analyses. Cortical reconstruction and volumetric segmentation were performed using the FreeSurfer image analysis suite (version 4.02; http://surfer.nmr.mgh.harvard.edu/). This involved motion correction and averaging of two volumetric T1 weighted images, removal of non-brain tissue, automated Talairach transformation, segmentation of the subcortical white and deep gray matter volumetric structures, intensity normalization, tessellation of the gray−white matter boundary, automated topology correction, and surface deformation following intensity gradients to optimally place gray-white and gray−CSF borders at the location where the greatest shift in intensity defines the transition to the other tissue class. FreeSurfer morphometric procedures have been demonstrated to show good test–retest reliability across scanner manufacturers and field strengths. The primary values of interest from the structural MRI were total HV as well as lateral (left and right) HV. We also explored right – left HV asymmetry. We controlled for total intracranial volume (ICV) by calculating the proportion of total HV and lateral HV relative to ICV.

Deployment assessment measures

The Combat Experiences Log (CEL)

During deployment, soldiers completed monthly self-report measures of stressor exposure and stress-related symptoms using a web-based self-monitoring assessment system (Lee et al., Reference Lee, Goudarzi, Baldwin, Rosenfield and Telch2011; see below).

War-zone stressors

The monthly occurrence of war-zone stressors in the past 30 days was assessed with dichotomous (present v. absent) items adapted from the Deployment Risk and Resilience Inventory (King, King, Foy, Keane, & Fairbank, Reference King, King, Foy, Keane and Fairbank1999). Because we were interested in the impact of potentially traumatic events (PTEs), two advanced doctoral students and the PI (MJ Telch) independently selected for inclusion the subset of DRRI items that met the DSM-5 criterion A definition for trauma exposure (‘exposure to actual or threatened death, serious injury or sexual violence’, p. 271; American Psychiatric Association, 2013). Additionally, two free-response items that allowed coding of stressors not included in the checklist were independently coded. Raters agreed on 100% of the selected items.

From these items representing in-theater stressor counts, we derived two stressor variables: between-soldier differences in average monthly exposure to traumatic war-zone stressors (PTEBP), and within-soldier monthly deviation from this average (PTEWP). Separately considering these components avoids the problematic assumption that the effects of traumatic-stressors aggregated over time and acute changes in stressor exposure are equal (Hoffman & Stawski, Reference Hoffman and Stawski2009). Disentangling these components is critical, especially in the context of a longitudinal diathesis-stress framework.

In-theater depression symptoms (CES-D-10)

In-theater monthly depression symptoms were assessed using the Center for Epidemiological Studies Depression Scale – 10 item version (Andresen et al., Reference Andresen, Malmgren, Carter and Patrick1994). Items were rated according to frequency of each core symptom of depression (0 = ‘Rarely/None of the time’; 3 = ‘Most/All of the time’) in the past 30 days. The CES-D-10 is well-validated and highly correlated with the full 20-item version. Internal consistency was good in the present sample (α = 0.81).

In-theater post-traumatic stress symptoms (PTSD symptoms)

In-theater monthly severity of PTSD symptoms was assessed using an expanded self-report version of The Short Post-Traumatic Stress Disorder Interview Scale (SPRINT; Connor & Davidson, Reference Connor and Davidson2001; Norris, Hamblen, Brown, & Schinka, Reference Norris, Hamblen, Brown and Schinka2008). This measure includes 11 Likert-type items rated according to the severity of core PTSD symptoms occurring in the past 30-days, with total monthly scores as our primary outcome of interest. The SPRINT demonstrates excellent psychometric properties and has good convergent validity with related measures. Internal consistency for the present sample was excellent (α = 0.90).

Statistical analyses

To address our primary aims we conducted generalized mixed-effects models using the lme4 package in R (Bates, Mächler, Bolker, & Walker, Reference Bates, Mächler, Bolker and Walker2015). We used a maximal approach to model building, with initial selection of the combination of fixed and random effects of time as well as the predictors of interest. All models fit best using fixed and random linear effects of time and were retained. We used the Poisson family to model monthly in-theater PTSD symptoms, and a square-root link function to address non-normal residuals. To handle overdispersion in the model (a common issue with Poisson distributed data), we also included an observation-level random effect, which addressed the overdispersion effectively. We included age, sex (0 = male), current and past Axis I psychopathology (0 = absent), prior trauma exposure (0 = absent), trait anxiety, time since deployment, and monthly in-theater depression symptoms as covariates in all of the models. All HV indices were calculated as a proportion of ICV. All continuous predictors were standardized (scaled and centered), with the exception of time, which was entered in its natural metric, and centered at 9 months (i.e. to stabilize residuals).

To address our primary aims we (1) evaluated the interactions between pre-deployment total HV and between-soldier differences in average monthly exposure to traumatic war-zone stressors (PTEBP), and within-soldier monthly deviation from this average (PTEWP) and (2) evaluated the distinct interactions between lateral (right and left) HV and PTEBP or PTEWP. We then examined HV asymmetry (the right-left HV discrepancy) as the moderator of PTEBP or PTEWP to further probe the findings of our analysis of lateralized HVs. Additionally, in response to reviewer feedback, we replicated all analyses excluding covariates to help clarify the effects of HV and in-theater trauma-exposure on PTSD symptoms. The results of these additional analyses are provided in online Supplementary Tables S3–S5. All continuous predictors were standardized. Each fixed effect estimate was exponentiated to allow for interpretation of coefficients as the change in the outcome given one standardized unit change in the predictor. We do not report random effects from the models. We bootstrapped all models based on profile likelihood with 1000 iterations, using the default methods from the MASS package in R to generate 95% confidence intervals (CIs) around parameter estimates.

Results

Main effects for all models are reported in the tables (Table 2 and online Supplementary Tables S1–S2), which provide direct effects for HV on PTSD symptoms. Findings revealed no direct effects of pre-deployment HV on in-theater PTSD symptoms across HV indices, including for total HV [b = −0.20, p = 0.071 (95% CI −0.42 to 0.02)], left HV [b = −0.03, p = 0.872 (95% CI −0.39 to 0.33)], right HV [b = −0.18, p = 0.460 (95% CI −0.52 to 0.17)], and right – left HV asymmetry [b = −0.08, p = 0.460 (95% CI −0.29 to 0.13)].

Table 2. Regression models

Note. Table 2 presents model results, with main effects reported only for the total HV model (main effects for other models can be found in online Supplementary Tables 1 and 2). All continuous predictors were z-transformed. CES-D-10 = Center for Epidemiological Studies Depression Scale (10 items). PTEBP, Between-soldier effect of monthly average exposure to potentially traumatic war-zone stressors; PTEWP, within-soldier effect of monthly deviation from their own monthly average exposure to potentially traumatic war-zone stressors; HV, hippocampal volume (cm3), adjusted for total intracranial volume (ICV; cm3). L HV and R HV, left and right hippocampal volumes. Avg. HV, average of left and right hippocampal volumes. R – L HV, right – left hippocampal volume.

Does total HV moderate the effect of traumatic stress on PTSD symptoms?

Total HV did not moderate the effects of within-person deviation from the average number of traumatic stressors [b = −0.025, p = 0.858 (95% CI −0.297 to 0.221)], nor the average exposure to traumatic stressors on PTSD symptoms [b = −0.028, p = 0.681 (95% CI −0.167 to 0.100)]. See Table 2 for a summary of the main effects from the model (including the direct effect of HV on PTSD symptoms).

Does lateral HV (right or left) moderate the effect of traumatic stress on PTSD symptoms?

Right b = −0.467, p = 0.023 (95% CI −0.786 to −0.013) and left b = 0.435, p = 0.024 (95% CI 0.028–0.715) HV moderated the average effect of traumatic stressors on PTSD symptoms (Fig. 1a, 1b), but not the within-person deviation from the average number of stressors b = −0.058, p = 0.537 (95% CI −0.258 to 0.113); b = 0.022, p = 0.813 (95% CI −0.164 to 0.229). Smaller right HV was associated with greater PTSD symptoms with greater average traumatic stress. However, larger left HV was associated with greater PTSD symptoms under greater traumatic stress. See online Supplementary Table S1 for a summary of the main effects from the model.

Fig. 1. (a) Depicts that with greater number of traumatic stressors, greater right HV is associated with fewer PTSD symptoms (i.e. protective); (b) depicts that with greater number of traumatic stressors, greater left HV is associated with greater PTSD symptoms; (c) shows that when right HV is greater than left HV with greater number of traumatic stressors there are fewer PTSD symptoms, whereas the opposite is true when right HV is smaller than left HV. HV = Hippocampal Volume, ‘smaller’ = −1 standard deviation and ‘greater’ = + 1 standard deviation. ‘R > L’ reflects + 1 standard deviation from the mean lateral difference; ‘R < L’ reflects −1 standard deviation from the mean lateral difference. Values for Average Monthly Traumatic Stressor are converted from the scaled values to the approximate raw (unscaled) values. Gray bands around each regression line reflect 95% CIs.

Does HV asymmetry (right−left) moderate the effect of traumatic stress on PTSD symptoms?

Exploratory analyses were then conducted to probe the lateral asymmetry (within-person difference in right and left HV) in an exploratory analysis and found right - left asymmetry moderated the effects of average traumatic stressors on PTSD symptoms b = 0.287, p = 0.014 (95% CI −0.464 to −0.063) (Fig. 1c), although not within-person deviation b = 0.025, p = 0.657 (95% CI −0.137 to 0.077). Those with greater asymmetry (right > left) and greater average traumatic stressors reported fewer PTSD symptoms. In contrast, the opposite appeared to be true when greater asymmetry was in the opposite direction (right < left). See online Supplementary Table S2 for a summary of the main effects from the model.

Discussion

Using a diathesis stress framework, we sought to examine the relationship between pre-deployment HV and the later in-theater emergence of monthly PTSD symptoms in response to war-zone stressors among army soldiers with no prior history of war-zone deployment. Contrary to expectations, total HV did not predict PTSD symptom emergence, nor did it moderate the effects of war-zone stressors on PTSD symptoms. However, lateral HV exerted significant effects on the relationship between war-zone stressors and PTSD symptoms. As expected, greater right HV exerted a protective effect on PTSD, whereas greater left HV was associated with greater PTSD symptom emergence. Consistent with these lateral HV findings, our post-hoc exploratory findings revealed that right > left HV asymmetry conferred reduced risk for PTSD symptom emergence even after controlling for depression and several other factors capable of influencing HV.

Prior work has shown strong support for the clinical relevance of HV asymmetry. Meta-analyses of cross-sectional studies suggest that HV asymmetry is associated with PTSD. Woon et al. (Reference Woon, Sood and Hedges2010) found smaller right HV, but not smaller left HV in PTSD patients relative to trauma-exposed controls. A more recent meta-analysis found that smaller left but not right HV was associated with greater PTSD symptom severity (Nelson & Tumpap, Reference Nelson and Tumpap2017) and a whole brain analysis of individuals with PTSD identified smaller HV compared to trauma-exposed individuals without PTSD (Kühn & Gallinat, Reference Kühn and Gallinat2013). Importantly, right < left HV has been associated with heightened attention to trauma-related information (Hall et al., Reference Hall, Galletly, Clark, Veltmeyer, Metzger, Gilbertson and McFarlane2012), suggesting possible functional associations with risk for PTSD. Notably, prior research on HV asymmetry has been entirely cross-sectional, thus highlighting the importance of the current findings. Right > left HV asymmetry is reliably observed in healthy populations (Lucarelli et al., Reference Lucarelli, Peshock, McColl, Hulsey, Ayers, Whittemore and King2013; Woolard & Heckers, Reference Woolard and Heckers2012), whereas right < left HV asymmetry has been linked to several neuropsychiatric disorders including schizophrenia (Oertel et al., Reference Oertel, Knöchel, Rotarska-Jagiela, Schönmeyer, Lindner, van de Ven and Linden2010), dementia, (Geuze, Vermetten, & Bremner, Reference Geuze, Vermetten and Bremner2005), and reduced cognitive ability (Woolard & Heckers, Reference Woolard and Heckers2012). This suggests that instead of being a PTSD-specific risk factor, right < left HV asymmetry might operate as a more general marker of stress-related psychopathology.

Hippocampal atrophy has been shown to mediate the link between stress and stress-related psychopathology (Gilbertson et al., Reference Gilbertson, Shenton, Ciszewski, Kasai, Lasko, Orr and Pitman2002; Sapolsky, Reference Sapolsky2000), whereas hippocampal growth has been shown to mediate the effects of successful treatment for PTSD (Levy-Gigi, Szabó, Kelemen, & Kéri, Reference Levy-Gigi, Szabó, Kelemen and Kéri2013) and depression (Warner-Schmidt & Duman, Reference Warner-Schmidt and Duman2006). Furthermore, a range of neurotrophic factors promotes hippocampal neurogenesis (Hurley et al., Reference Hurley, Akinfiresoye, Nwulia, Kamiya, Kulkarni and Tizabi2013; Warner-Schmidt & Duman, Reference Warner-Schmidt and Duman2006) and recent work suggests that visuo-spatial cognitive tasks (e.g. Tetris) may enhance treatment outcomes, in part due to hippocampal neurogenesis or increased synaptic plasticity (Butler et al., Reference Butler, Herr, Willmund, Gallinat, Kühn and Zimmermann2020). Future work should test whether interventions targeting hippocampal growth can protect against PTSD emergence.

Limitations

Several limitations of the study deserve mention. First, our analyses were limited to HV measures obtained at pre-deployment and stressor exposure and stress reactions obtained during deployment in-theater. An important unanswered question is whether HV changed during deployment. Second, although there is clear value in examining hippocampal sub-regions (Chen & Etkin, Reference Chen and Etkin2013), we were unable to do so due to the scanner resolution. Third, although biological sex was included as a covariate, the small number of female soldiers in our sample (n = 14) precluded examination of biological sex differences, thus limiting the generalizability of our findings. However, while females generally expressed higher symptom levels, there were no apparent differences in the modeled relationships as a function of sex. Fourth, although the average PTSD symptom scores for the entire sample during deployment were relatively low suggesting an overall pattern of resilience, more than 25% of soldiers at some point during deployment reported a PTSD symptom score above the recommended cutoff for PTSD. Fifth, given the inherent challenges of data collection in the war-zone, missing observations were not uncommon; however, there were no patterns to missingness, our analytic approach is well suited for addressing missingness (Raudenbush & Bryk, Reference Raudenbush and Bryk2002), and in-theater data were obtained from most soldiers (91%). Sixth, for similar reasons, we were unable to collect gold-standard interview measures in-theater, and thus our outcome measures are limited by the subjective nature of self-report. Finally, we applied an older version of Freesurfer (version 4.02), whereas newer versions have shown slightly less bias in the direction of overestimating HV (Schmidt et al., Reference Schmidt, Storrs, Freeman, Jack, Turner, Griswold and Mosley2018); however, this does not preclude validly assessing individual differences in HV as a prospective stress-moderator.

Conclusions

The present findings suggest that HV may confer risk for the emergence of trauma-related symptoms in the war zone. Importantly, we identified differential PTSD symptom emergence to traumatic-stressor exposure based on left v. right HV, where greater left HV was protective and smaller right HV potentiated the effect of traumatic-stressors on PTSD symptoms. Our observation that right < left HV asymmetry confers risk for the emergence of PTSD symptoms in the war zone helps explain this finding in the context of a broader asymmetry literature highlighting right < left HV asymmetry as a general risk factor for psychopathology. The findings demonstrate the utility of diathesis-stress frameworks in estimating risk conferred by neurobiological markers. Future investigations are needed to examine further the role of lateral HV and HV asymmetry as a vulnerability marker for PTSD and other stress-related disorders. Investigation of interventions that promote hippocampal neurogenesis deserves serious consideration in the prevention of PTSD and other stress-related disorders.

Supplementary material

The supplementary material for this article can be found at https://doi.org/10.1017/S0033291721003548

Acknowledgements

This research was funded by the U.S. Army RDECOM Acquisition Center, Natick Contracting Division, and U. S. Defense Advanced Agency under Contract No. W911QY-07-C-0002 awarded to Michael J. Telch. Views expressed in this article are those of the authors and may not necessarily be endorsed by the U. S. Army or the Department of Veterans Affairs. Adam Cobb was supported by a T32 postdoctoral fellowship (MH18869) at the National Crime Victims Research and Treatment Center at the Medical University of South Carolina.

Conflict of interest

The authors have no conflicts of interest or financial disclosures to report.

Footnotes

*

Adam R. Cobb and Mikael Rubin contributed equally to the manuscript.

References

Admon, R., Leykin, D., Lubin, G., Engert, V., Andrews, J., Pruessner, J., & Hendler, T. (2013). Stress-induced reduction in hippocampal volume and connectivity with the ventromedial prefrontal cortex are related to maladaptive responses to stressful military service. Human Brain Mapping, 34(11), 28082816. https://doi.org/10.1002/hbm.22100.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
American Psychiatric Association. (2013). Diagnostic and statistical manual of mental disorders (5th ed.). Arlington, VA: Author. https://play.google.com/store/books/details?id=-JivBAAAQBAJ.Google Scholar
Andresen, E. M., Malmgren, J. A., Carter, W. B., & Patrick, D. L. (1994). Screening for depression in well older adults: Evaluation of a short form of the CES-D. American Journal of Preventive Medicine, 10(2), 7784. https://doi.org/10.1016/S0749-3797(18)30622-6.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Apfel, B. A., Ross, J., Hlavin, J., Meyerhoff, D. J., Metzler, T. J., Marmar, C. R., … Neylan, T. C. (2011). Hippocampal volume differences in Gulf War veterans with current versus lifetime posttraumatic stress disorder symptoms. Biological Psychiatry, 69(6), 541548. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.biopsych.2010.09.044.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Bates, D., Mächler, M., Bolker, B., & Walker, S. (2015). Fitting linear mixed-effects models using lme4. Journal of Statistical Software, Articles, 67(1), 148. https://doi.org/10.18637/jss.v067.i01.Google Scholar
Beevers, C. G., Lee, H.-J., Wells, T. T., Ellis, A. J., & Telch, M. J. (2011a). Association of predeployment gaze bias for emotion stimuli with later symptoms of PTSD and depression in soldiers deployed in Iraq. The American Journal of Psychiatry, 168(7), 735741. https://doi.org/10.1176/appi.ajp.2011.10091309.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Beevers, C. G., Marti, C. N., Lee, H.-J., Stote, D. L., Ferrell, R. E., Hariri, A. R., & Telch, M. J. (2011b). Associations between serotonin transporter gene promoter region (5-HTTLPR) polymorphism and gaze bias for emotional information. Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 120(1), 187197. https://doi.org/10.1037/a0022125.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Bieling, P. J., Antony, M. M., & Swinson, R. P. (1998). The state-trait anxiety inventory, trait version: Structure and content re-examined. Behaviour Research and Therapy, 36(7–8), 777788. https://doi.org/10.1016/s0005-7967(98)00023-0.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Butler, O., Herr, K., Willmund, G., Gallinat, J., Kühn, S., & Zimmermann, P. (2020). Trauma, treatment and tetris: Video gaming increases hippocampal volume in male patients with combat-related posttraumatic stress disorder. Journal of Psychiatry & Neuroscience: JPN, 45(4), 279287. https://doi.org/10.1503/jpn.190027.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Chen, A. C., & Etkin, A. (2013). Hippocampal network connectivity and activation differentiates post-traumatic stress disorder from generalized anxiety disorder. Neuropsychopharmacology: Official Publication of the American College of Neuropsychopharmacology, 38(10), 18891898. https://doi.org/10.1038/npp.2013.122.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Cobb, A. R., Josephs, R. A., Lancaster, C. L., Lee, H.-J., & Telch, M. J. (2018). Cortisol, testosterone, and prospective risk for war-zone stress-evoked depression. Military Medicine, 183(11–12), e535e545. https://doi.org/10.1093/milmed/usy065.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Cobb, A. R., Lancaster, C. L., Meyer, E. C., Lee, H.-J., & Telch, M. J. (2017). Pre-deployment trait anxiety, anxiety sensitivity and experiential avoidance predict war-zone stress-evoked psychopathology. Journal of Contextual Behavioral Science, 6(3), 276287. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jcbs.2017.05.002.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Connor, K. M., & Davidson, J. R. T. (2001). SPRINT: A brief global assessment of post-traumatic stress disorder. International Clinical Psychopharmacology, 16(5), 279284. http://dx.doi.org/10.1097/00004850-200109000-00005.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Disner, S. G., Beevers, C. G., Lee, H.-J., Ferrell, R. E., Hariri, A. R., & Telch, M. J. (2013). War zone stress interacts with the 5-HTTLPR polymorphism to predict the development of sustained attention for negative emotion stimuli in soldiers returning from Iraq. Clinical Psychological Science, 1(4), 413425. https://doi.org/10.1177/2167702613485564.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Edmondson, D., Kronish, I. M., Wasson, L. T., Giglio, J. F., Davidson, K. W., & Whang, W. (2014). A test of the diathesis-stress model in the emergency department: Who develops PTSD after an acute coronary syndrome? Journal of Psychiatric Research, 53, 813. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jpsychires.2014.02.009.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Elwood, L. S., Hahn, K. S., Olatunji, B. O., & Williams, N. L. (2009). Cognitive vulnerabilities to the development of PTSD: A review of four vulnerabilities and the proposal of an integrative vulnerability model. Clinical Psychology Review, 29(1), 87100. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.cpr.2008.10.002.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Geuze, E., Vermetten, E., & Bremner, J. D. (2005). MR-based in vivo hippocampal volumetrics: 2. Findings in neuropsychiatric disorders. Molecular Psychiatry, 10(2), 160184. https://doi.org/10.1038/sj.mp.4001579.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Gilbertson, M. W., Shenton, M. E., Ciszewski, A., Kasai, K., Lasko, N. B., Orr, S. P., & Pitman, R. K. (2002). Smaller hippocampal volume predicts pathologic vulnerability to psychological trauma. Nature Neuroscience, 5(11), 12421247. https://doi.org/10.1038/nn958.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Hall, T., Galletly, C., Clark, C. R., Veltmeyer, M., Metzger, L. J., Gilbertson, M. W., … McFarlane, A. (2012). The relationship between hippocampal asymmetry and working memory processing in combat-related PTSD: A monozygotic twin study. Biology of Mood & Anxiety Disorders, 2, 21. https://doi.org/10.1186/2045-5380-2-21.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Hoffman, L., & Stawski, R. S. (2009). Persons as contexts: Evaluating between-person and within-person effects in longitudinal analysis. Research in Human Development, 6(2–3), 97120. https://doi.org/10.1080/15427600902911189.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Hurley, L. L., Akinfiresoye, L., Nwulia, E., Kamiya, A., Kulkarni, A. A., & Tizabi, Y. (2013). Antidepressant-like effects of curcumin in WKY rat model of depression is associated with an increase in hippocampal BDNF. Behavioural Brain Research, 239, 2730. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.bbr.2012.10.049.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Josephs, R. A., Cobb, A. R., Lancaster, C. L., Lee, H.-J., & Telch, M. J. (2017). Dual-hormone stress reactivity predicts downstream war-zone stress-evoked PTSD. Psychoneuroendocrinology, 78, 7684. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.psyneuen.2017.01.013.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Karl, A., Schaefer, M., Malta, L. S., Dörfel, D., Rohleder, N., & Werner, A. (2006). A meta-analysis of structural brain abnormalities in PTSD. Neuroscience and Biobehavioral Reviews, 30(7), 10041031. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neubiorev.2006.03.004.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
King, D. W., King, L. A., Foy, D. W., Keane, T. M., & Fairbank, J. A. (1999). Posttraumatic stress disorder in a national sample of female and male Vietnam veterans: Risk factors, war-zone stressors, and resilience-recovery variables. Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 108(1), 164170. https://doi.org/10.1037//0021-843x.108.1.164.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Kühn, S., & Gallinat, J. (2013). Gray matter correlates of posttraumatic stress disorder: A quantitative meta-analysis. Biological Psychiatry, 73(1), 7074. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.biopsych.2012.06.029.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Lancaster, C. L., Cobb, A. R., Lee, H.-J., & Telch, M. J. (2016). The role of perceived threat in the emergence of PTSD and depression symptoms during warzone deployment. Psychological Trauma: Theory, Research, Practice and Policy, 8(4), 528534. https://doi.org/10.1037/tra0000129.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Lee, H.-J., Goudarzi, K., Baldwin, B., Rosenfield, D., & Telch, M. J. (2011). The combat experience Log: A web-based system for the in theater assessment of war zone stress. Journal of Anxiety Disorders, 25(6), 794800. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.janxdis.2011.03.018.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Levy-Gigi, E., Szabó, C., Kelemen, O., & Kéri, S. (2013). Association among clinical response, hippocampal volume, and FKBP5 gene expression in individuals with posttraumatic stress disorder receiving cognitive behavioral therapy. Biological Psychiatry, 74(11), 793800. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.biopsych.2013.05.017.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Logue, M. W., van Rooij, S. J., Dennis, E. L., Davis, S. L., Hayes, J. P., Stevens, J. S., … Morey, R. A. (2018). Smaller hippocampal volume in posttraumatic stress disorder: A multisite ENIGMA-PGC study: Subcortical volumetry results from posttraumatic stress disorder consortia. Biological Psychiatry, 83(3), 244253. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.biopsych.2017.09.006.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Lucarelli, R. T., Peshock, R. M., McColl, R., Hulsey, K., Ayers, C., Whittemore, A. R., & King, K. S. (2013). MR Imaging of hippocampal asymmetry at 3T in a multiethnic, population-based sample: Results from the Dallas heart study. AJNR. American Journal of Neuroradiology, 34(4), 752757. https://doi.org/10.3174/ajnr.A3308.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Nelson, M. D., & Tumpap, A. M. (2017). Posttraumatic stress disorder symptom severity is associated with left hippocampal volume reduction: A meta-analytic study. CNS Spectrums, 22(4), 363372. https://doi.org/10.1017/S1092852916000833.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Norris, F. H., Hamblen, J. L., Brown, L. M., & Schinka, J. A. (2008). Validation of the Short Posttraumatic Stress Disorder Rating Interview (expanded version, Sprint-E) as a measure of postdisaster distress and treatment need. American Journal of Disaster Medicine, 3(4), 201212. https://doi.org/10.5055/ajdm.2008.0027.Google ScholarPubMed
O'Doherty, D. C. M., Chitty, K. M., Saddiqui, S., Bennett, M. R., & Lagopoulos, J. (2015). A systematic review and meta-analysis of magnetic resonance imaging measurement of structural volumes in posttraumatic stress disorder. Psychiatry Research, 232(1), 133. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.pscychresns.2015.01.002.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Oertel, V., Knöchel, C., Rotarska-Jagiela, A., Schönmeyer, R., Lindner, M., van de Ven, V., … Linden, D. E. J. (2010). Reduced laterality as a trait marker of schizophrenia:Evidence from structural and functional neuroimaging. The Journal of Neuroscience: The Official Journal of the Society for Neuroscience, 30(6), 22892299. https://doi.org/10.1523/JNEUROSCI.4575-09.2010.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Pitman, R. K., Rasmusson, A. M., Koenen, K. C., Shin, L. M., Orr, S. P., Gilbertson, M. W., … Liberzon, I. (2012). Biological studies of post-traumatic stress disorder. Nature Reviews. Neuroscience, 13(11), 769787. https://doi.org/10.1038/nrn3339.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Raudenbush, S. W., & Bryk, A. S. (2002). Hierarchical linear models: Applications and data analysis methods. Thousand Oaks: SAGE. https://play.google.com/store/books/details?id=uyCV0CNGDLQC.Google Scholar
Rubin, M., Shvil, E., Papini, S., Chhetry, B. T., Helpman, L., Markowitz, J. C., … Neria, Y. (2016). Greater hippocampal volume is associated with PTSD treatment response. Psychiatry Research: Neuroimaging, 252, 3639. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.pscychresns.2016.05.001.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Sapolsky, R. M. (2000). Glucocorticoids and hippocampal atrophy in neuropsychiatric disorders. Archives of General Psychiatry, 57(10), 925935. https://doi.org/10.1001/archpsyc.57.10.925.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Schmidt, M. F., Storrs, J. M., Freeman, K. B., Jack, C. R. Jr., Turner, S. T., Griswold, M. E., & Mosley, T. H. Jr. (2018). A comparison of manual tracing and FreeSurfer for estimating hippocampal volume over the adult lifespan. Human Brain Mapping, 39(6), 25002513. http://dx.doi.org/10.1002/hbm.24017.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Smith, M. E. (2005). Bilateral hippocampal volume reduction in adults with post-traumatic stress disorder: A meta-analysis of structural MRI studies. Hippocampus, 15(6), 798807. https://doi.org/10.1002/hipo.20102.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Spielberger, C. D., Gorsuch, R. L., & Lushene, R. E. (1970). STAI Manual for the state-trait anxiety inventory (“Self-evaluation Questionnaire”). Palo Alto: Consulting Psychologists Press. https://play.google.com/store/books/details?id=Pp3ZAAAAMAAJ..Google Scholar
Telch, M. J., Beevers, C. G., Rosenfield, D., Lee, H.-J., Reijntjes, A., Ferrell, R. E., & Hariri, A. R. (2015). 5-HTTLPR Genotype potentiates the effects of war zone stressors on the emergence of PTSD, depressive and anxiety symptoms in soldiers deployed to Iraq. World Psychiatry: Official Journal of the World Psychiatric Association, 14(2), 198206. https://doi.org/10.1002/wps.20215.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Telch, M. J., Rosenfield, D., Lee, H.-J., & Pai, A. (2012). Emotional reactivity to a single inhalation of 35% carbon dioxide and its association with later symptoms of posttraumatic stress disorder and anxiety in soldiers deployed to Iraq. Archives of General Psychiatry, 69(11), 11611168. https://doi.org/10.1001/archgenpsychiatry.2012.8.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
van Rooij, S. J. H., Kennis, M., &Vink, M. (2016). Predicting Treatment Outcome in PTSD: A Longitudinal Functional MRI Study on Trauma-Unrelated Emotional Processing. Neuropsychopharmacology, 41(4), 11561165. https://doi.org/10.1038/npp.2015.257.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Warner-Schmidt, J. L., & Duman, R. S. (2006). Hippocampal neurogenesis: Opposing effects of stress and antidepressant treatment. Hippocampus, 16(3), 239249. https://doi.org/10.1002/hipo.20156.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Woolard, A. A., & Heckers, S. (2012). Anatomical and functional correlates of human hippocampal volume asymmetry. Psychiatry Research, 201(1), 4853. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.pscychresns.2011.07.016.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Woon, F. L., Sood, S., & Hedges, D. W. (2010). Hippocampal volume deficits associated with exposure to psychological trauma and posttraumatic stress disorder in adults: A meta-analysis. Progress in Neuro-Psychopharmacology & Biological Psychiatry, 34(7), 11811188. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.pnpbp.2010.06.016.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Figure 0

Table 1. Descriptive statistics

Figure 1

Table 2. Regression models

Figure 2

Fig. 1. (a) Depicts that with greater number of traumatic stressors, greater right HV is associated with fewer PTSD symptoms (i.e. protective); (b) depicts that with greater number of traumatic stressors, greater left HV is associated with greater PTSD symptoms; (c) shows that when right HV is greater than left HV with greater number of traumatic stressors there are fewer PTSD symptoms, whereas the opposite is true when right HV is smaller than left HV. HV = Hippocampal Volume, ‘smaller’ = −1 standard deviation and ‘greater’ = + 1 standard deviation. ‘R > L’ reflects + 1 standard deviation from the mean lateral difference; ‘R < L’ reflects −1 standard deviation from the mean lateral difference. Values for Average Monthly Traumatic Stressor are converted from the scaled values to the approximate raw (unscaled) values. Gray bands around each regression line reflect 95% CIs.

Supplementary material: File

Cobb et al. supplementary material

Cobb et al. supplementary material

Download Cobb et al. supplementary material(File)
File 73.3 KB