Hostname: page-component-8448b6f56d-dnltx Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-04-16T18:24:47.175Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

Description and measurement of concentration problems in depressed patients

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  09 July 2009

Fraser N. Watts*
Affiliation:
MRC Applied Psychology Unit, Cambridge
Robert Sharrock
Affiliation:
MRC Applied Psychology Unit, Cambridge
*
1Address for correspondence: Dr F. N. Watts, MRC Applied Psychology Unit, 15 Chaucer Road, Cambridge CB2 2EF.

Synopsis

Depressed patients commonly complain of concentration problems, yet these have seldom been the focus of systematic investigation. A structured interview about concentration problems was administered to a group of relatively severely depressed patients. Problems in reading and watching television were the most common, and were highly correlated with each other. Direct report of the number of concentration lapses on a reading task was the most generally satisfactory task-performance correlate of complaints of reading/TV concentration problems. Evidence both from this task and from the interview suggests that depressive concentration problems may often be due to ‘mind-wandering’. The correlations with concentration problems with the severity and endogeneity of depression and with state anxiety were generally similar.

Type
Research Article
Copyright
Copyright © Cambridge University Press 1985

Access options

Get access to the full version of this content by using one of the access options below. (Log in options will check for institutional or personal access. Content may require purchase if you do not have access.)

References

Cohen, G. (1979). Language comprehension in old age. Cognitive Psychology 11, 412429.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Condiff, G. & Gold, S. R. (1979). Daydreaming: a measurable concept. Perceptual and Motor Skills 49, 347353.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Giambra, L. M. & Traynor, T. D. (1978). Depression and daydreaming: an analysis of self-ratings. Journal of Clinical Psychology 34, 1425.3.0.CO;2-#>CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Hamilton, V. (1980). An information processing analysis of environmental stress and life crises. In Stress and Anxiety, Vol. 7 (ed. Sarason, I. G. and Spielberger, C. D.), pp. 1330. Hemisphere Publishing Company: New York.Google Scholar
Hasher, L. & Zacks, R. T. (1979). Automatic and effortful processes in memory. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General 108, 356388.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Kahn, R. L., Zarit, S. H., Hilbert, N. M. & Niedereke, G. (1975). Memory complaint and impairment in the aged. Archives of General Psychiatry 32, 15691573.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
McAllister, T. W. (1981). Cognitive functioning in the affective disorders. Comprehensive Psychiatry 22, 572586.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Nelson, J. G. & Charney, D. S. (1981). The symptoms of major depressive illness. American Journal of Psychiatry 138, 113.Google ScholarPubMed
Pilowsky, I. (1979). Further validation of a questionnaire method for classifying depressive illness. Journal of Affective Disorders 1, 179185.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Pilowsky, I. & Boulton, D. M. (1970). Development of a questionnaire-based decision rule for classifying depressed patients. British Journal of Psychiatry 116, 647650.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Pilowsky, I. & Spalding, D. (1972). A method for measuring depression: validity studies on a depression questionnaire. British Journal of Psychiatry 121, 411416.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Singer, J. L. (1966). Daydreaming. Random House: New York.Google ScholarPubMed
Weingartner, H. & Silberman, E. (1982). Models of cognitive impairment: cognitive changes in depression. Psychopharmacology Bulletin 18, 2742.Google ScholarPubMed
Wine, J. (1971). Test anxiety and direction of attention. Psychological Bulletin 76, 92104.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed