Hostname: page-component-848d4c4894-wg55d Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-05-14T03:50:27.678Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

Altered performance in attention tasks in patients with seasonal allergic rhinitis: seasonal dependency and association with disease characteristics

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  02 October 2014

K. Trikojat*
Affiliation:
Department of Psychology, Technische Universität Dresden, Germany
A. Buske-Kirschbaum
Affiliation:
Department of Psychology, Technische Universität Dresden, Germany
J. Schmitt
Affiliation:
Centre for Evidence-Based Healthcare, University Hospital Carl Gustav Carus Dresden, Germany
F. Plessow
Affiliation:
Department of Psychology, Technische Universität Dresden, Germany Berenson-Allen Center for Noninvasive Brain Stimulation, Department of Neurology, Beth Israel Deaconess Medical Center, Harvard Medical School, Boston, MA, USA
*
*Address for correspondence: K. Trikojat, Dipl.-Psych., Department of Psychology, Technische Universität Dresden, Zellescher Weg 19, D-01069 Dresden, Germany. (Email: katharina.trikojat@tu-dresden.de)

Abstract

Background.

Seasonal allergic rhinitis (SAR) is a chronic disease affecting about 23% of the European population with increasing prevalence rates. Beside classical symptoms (i.e. sneezing, nasal congestion), patients frequently complain about subjective impairments in cognitive functioning during periods of acute allergic inflammation. However, objective evidence for such deficits or the role of potential modulators and underlying mechanisms is limited. The present study aimed to investigate the effect of SAR on attention-related cognitive processes. In addition, relationships between attention performance, sleep and mood disturbances as well as specific disease characteristics as potential modulators of this link were explored.

Method.

SAR patients (n = 41) and non-allergic healthy controls (n = 42) completed a set of attention tasks during a symptomatic allergy period and during a non-symptomatic period. Influences of sleep, mood, total immunoglobulin E (IgE) levels and individual allergy characteristics on cognitive performance were evaluated.

Results.

Compared to healthy controls, SAR patients had a slower processing speed during both symptomatic and non-symptomatic allergy periods. Additionally, they showed a more flexible adjustment in attention control, which may serve as a compensatory strategy. Reduction in processing speed was positively associated with total IgE levels whereas flexible adjustment of attention was linked with anxious mood. No association was found between SAR-related attention deficits and allergy characteristics or sleep.

Conclusions.

SAR represents a state that is crucially linked to impairments in information processing and changes in attentional control adjustments. These cognitive alterations are more likely to be influenced by mood and basal inflammatory processes than sleep impairments or subjective symptom severity.

Type
Original Articles
Copyright
Copyright © Cambridge University Press 2014 

Access options

Get access to the full version of this content by using one of the access options below. (Log in options will check for institutional or personal access. Content may require purchase if you do not have access.)

References

Bauchau, V, Durham, SR (2004). Prevalence and rate of diagnosis of allergic rhinitis in Europe. European Respiratory Journal 24, 758764.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Belenky, G, Wesensten, NJ, Thorne, DR, Thomas, ML, Sing, HC, Redmond, DP, Russo, MB, Balkin, TJ (2003). Patterns of performance degradation and restoration during sleep restriction and subsequent recovery: a sleep dose-response study. Journal of Sleep Research 12, 112.Google Scholar
Benitez, A, Gunstad, J (2012). Poor sleep quality diminishes cognitive functioning independent of depression and anxiety in healthy young adults. Clinical Neuropsychologist 26, 214223.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Bousquet, J, Khaltaev, N, Cruz, AA, Denburg, J, Fokkens, WJ, Togias, A, Zuberbier, T, Baena-Cagnani, CE, Canonica, GW, van Weel, C, Agache, I, Aït-Khaled, N, Bachert, C, Blaiss, MS, Bonini, S, Boulet, L-P, Bousquet, P-J, Camargos, P, Carlsen, K-H, Chen, Y, Custovic, A, Dahl, R, Demoly, P, Douagui, H, Durham, SR, van Wijk, RG, Kalayci, O, Kaliner, MA, Kim, Y-Y, Kowalski, ML, Kuna, P, Le, LT, Lemiere, C, Li, J, Lockey, RF, Mavale-Manuel, S, Meltzer, EO, Mohammad, Y, Mullol, J, Naclerio, R, O'Hehir, RE, Ohta, K, Ouedraogo, S, Palkonen, S, Papadopoulos, N, Passalacqua, G, Pawankar, R, Popov, TA, Rabe, KF, Rosado-Pinto, J, Scadding, GK, Simons, FER, Toskala, E, Valovirta, E, van Cauwenberge, P, Wang, D-Y, Wickman, M, Yawn, BP, Yorgancioglu, A, Yusuf, OM, Zar, H, Annesi-Maesano, I, Bateman, ED, Ben Kheder, A, Boakye, DA, Bouchard, J, Burney, P, Busse, WW, Chan-Yeung, M, Chavannes, NH, Chuchalin, A, Dolen, WK, Emuzyte, R, Grouse, L, Humbert, M, Jackson, C, Johnston, SL, Keith, PK, Kemp, JP, Klossek, J-M, Larenas-Linnemann, D, Lipworth, B, Malo, J-L, Marshall, GD, Naspitz, C, Nekam, K, Niggemann, B, Nizankowska-Mogilnicka, E, Okamoto, Y, Orru, MP, Potter, P, Price, D, Stoloff, SW, Vandenplas, O, Viegi, G, Williams, D (2008 a). Allergic Rhinitis and its Impact on Asthma (ARIA) 2008 update (in collaboration with the World Health Organization, GA(2)LEN and AllerGen). Allergy 63 (Suppl. 86), 8160.Google Scholar
Bousquet, J, Reid, J, Van Weel, C, Baena Cagnani, C, Canonica, GW, Demoly, P, Denburg, J, Fokkens, WJ, Grouse, L, Mullol, K, Ohta, K, Schermer, T, Valovirta, E, Zhong, N, Zuberbier, T (2008 b). Allergic rhinitis management pocket reference 2008. Allergy 63, 990996.Google Scholar
Bower, GH (1981). Mood and memory. American Psychologist 36, 129148.Google Scholar
Buske-Kirschbaum, A, Ebrecht, M, Hellhammer, DH (2010). Blunted HPA axis responsiveness to stress in atopic patients is associated with the acuity and severeness of allergic inflammation. Brain, Behavior, and Immunity 24, 13471353.Google Scholar
Buysse, DJ, Reynolds, CF 3rd, Monk, TH, Berman, SR, Kupfer, DJ (1989). The Pittsburgh Sleep Quality Index: a new instrument for psychiatric practice and research. Psychiatry Research 28, 193213.Google Scholar
Capuron, L, Miller, AH (2011). Immune system to brain signaling: neuropsychopharmacological implications. Pharmacology and Therapeutics 130, 226238.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Dreisbach, G, Fischer, R (2012). The role of affect and reward in the conflict-triggered adjustment of cognitive control. Frontiers in Human Neuroscience 6, 342.Google Scholar
Ferreira, MAR (2003). Cytokine expression in allergic inflammation: systematic review of in vivo challenge studies. Mediators of Inflammation 12, 259267.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Fischer, R, Dreisbach, G, Goschke, T (2008). Context-sensitive adjustments of cognitive control: conflict-adaptation effects are modulated by processing demands of the ongoing task. Journal of Experimental Psychology. Learning, Memory, and Cognition 34, 712718.Google Scholar
Gendolla GHE (2000). On the impact of mood on behavior: an integrative theory and a review. Review of General Psychology 4, 378408.Google Scholar
Gratton, G, Coles, MG, Donchin, E (1992). Optimizing the use of information: strategic control of activation of responses. Journal of Experimental Psychology. General 121, 480506.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Hartgerink-Lutgens, I, Vermeeren, A, Vuurman, E, Kremer, B (2009). Disturbed cognitive functions after nasal provocation in patients with seasonal allergic rhinitis. Clinical and Experimental Allergy 39, 500508.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Hashizume, H, Horibe, T, Ohshima, A, Ito, T, Yagi, H, Takigawa, M (2005). Anxiety accelerates T-helper 2-tilted immune responses in patients with atopic dermatitis. British Journal of Dermatology 152, 11611164.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Heesen, C, Schulz, KH, Fiehler, J, Von der Mark, U, Otte, C, Jung, R, Poettgen, J, Krieger, T, Gold, SM (2010). Correlates of cognitive dysfunction in multiple sclerosis. Brain, Behavior, and Immunity 24, 11481155.Google Scholar
Juniper, EF, Thompson, AK, Ferrie, PJ, Roberts, JN (1999). Validation of the standardized version of the Rhinoconjunctivitis Quality of Life Questionnaire. Journal of Allergy and Clinical Immunology 104, 364369.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Klokk, M, Götestam, KG, Mykletun, A (2007). There are no association between IgE levels and symptoms of anxiety and depression in the adult female general population. The Hordaland Health Study (HUSK). Nordic Journal of Psychiatry 61, 410417.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Knye, M, Roth, N, Westhus, W, Heine, A (1996). Continuous Performance Test (CPT). In Kinderdiagnostisches System (ed. Lauth, G. W. and Hänsgen, K. D.), pp. 3437. Hogrefe: Göttingen.Google Scholar
Koinis-Mitchell, D, Craig, T, Esteban, CA, Klein, RB (2012). Sleep and allergic disease: a summary of the literature and future directions for research. Journal of Allergy and Clinical Immunology 130, 12751281.Google Scholar
Krabbe, KS, Reichenberg, A, Yirmiya, R, Smed, A, Pedersen, BK, Bruunsgaard, H (2005). Low-dose endotoxemia and human neuropsychological functions. Brain, Behavior, and Immunity 19, 453460.Google Scholar
Kremer, B, den Hartog, HM, Jolles, J (2002). Relationship between allergic rhinitis, disturbed cognitive functions and psychological well-being. Clinical and Experimental Allergy 32, 13101315.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Kuhbandner, C, Zehetleitner, M (2011). Dissociable effects of valence and arousal in adaptive executive control. PLoS ONE 6, e29287.Google Scholar
Laux, L, Glanzmann, P, Schaffner, P, Spielberger, CD (1981). State-Trait Anxiety Inventory (STAI). Theoretical Foundations and Instructions [in German]. Beltz: Weinheim.Google Scholar
Manalai, P, Hamilton, RG, Langenberg, P, Kosisky, SE, Lapidus, M, Sleemi, A, Scrandis, D, Cabassa, JA, Rogers, CA, Regenold, WT, Dickerson, F, Vittone, BJ, Guzman, A, Balis, T, Tonelli, LH, Postolache, TT (2012). Pollen-specific immunoglobulin E positivity is associated with worsening of depression scores in bipolar disorder patients during high pollen season. Bipolar Disorders 14, 9098.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Marshall, PS, Colon, EA (1993). Effects of allergy season on mood and cognitive function. Annals of Allergy 71, 251258.Google Scholar
Marshall, PS, O'Hara, C, Steinberg, P (2000). Effects of seasonal allergic rhinitis on selected cognitive abilities. Annals of Allergy, Asthma and Immunology 84, 403410.Google Scholar
Marshall, PS, O'Hara, C, Steinberg, P (2002). Effects of seasonal allergic rhinitis on fatigue levels and mood. Psychosomatic Medicine 64, 684691.Google ScholarPubMed
Marsland, AL, Petersen, KL, Sathanoori, R, Muldoon, MF, Neumann, SA, Ryan, C, Flory, JD, Manuck, SB (2006). Interleukin-6 covaries inversely with cognitive performance among middle-aged community volunteers. Psychosomatic Medicine 68, 895903.Google Scholar
McAfoose, J, Baune, BT (2009). Evidence for a cytokine model of cognitive function. Neuroscience and Biobehavioral Reviews 33, 355366.Google Scholar
Meltzer, EO, Nathan, R, Derebery, J, Stang, PE, Campbell, UB, Yeh, W-S, Corrao, M, Stanford, R (2009). Sleep, quality of life, and productivity impact of nasal symptoms in the United States: findings from the Burden of Rhinitis in America survey. Allergy and Asthma Proceedings 30, 244254.Google Scholar
Muliol, J, Maurer, M, Bousquet, J (2008). Sleep and allergic rhinitis. Journal of Investigational Allergology and Clinical Immunology 18, 415419.Google ScholarPubMed
Padmala, S, Bauer, A, Pessoa, L (2011). Negative emotion impairs conflict-driven executive control. Frontiers in Psychology 2, 192.Google Scholar
Plessow, F, Fischer, R, Kirschbaum, C, Goschke, T (2011). Inflexibly focused under stress: acute psychosocial stress increases shielding of action goals at the expense of reduced cognitive flexibility with increasing time lag to the stressor. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience 23, 32183227.Google Scholar
Simon, JR (1990). The effects of an irrelevant directional cue on human information processing. In Stimulus-Response Compatibility: An Integrated Perspective (ed. Proctor, R. W. and Reeve, T. G.), pp. 3186. North-Holland: Amsterdam.Google Scholar
Steyer, R, Schwenkmetzger, P, Notz, P, Eid, M (1997). The Multidimensional Mood Questionnaire (MDBF) [in German]. Hogrefe: Göttingen.Google Scholar
Streufert, S, Satish, U (2005). Impact of allergic rhinitis on simulated real-world performance. Journal of Applied Social Psychology 35, 14551473.Google Scholar
Stürmer, B, Leuthold, H, Soetens, E, Schröter, H, Sommer, W (2002). Control over location-based response activation in the Simon task: behavioral and electrophysiological evidence. Journal of Experimental Psychology. Human Perception and Performance 28, 13451363.Google Scholar
Thayer, JF, Sternberg, EM (2010). Neural aspects of immunomodulation: focus on the vagus nerve. Brain, Behavior, and Immunity 24, 12231228.Google Scholar
Thompson, A, Sardana, N, Craig, TJ (2013). Sleep impairment and daytime sleepiness in patients with allergic rhinitis: the role of congestion and inflammation. Annals of Allergy, Asthma and Immunology 111, 446451.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Timonen, M, Jokelainen, J, Hakko, H, Silvennoinen-Kassinen, S, Meyer-Rochow, VB, Herva, A, Räsänen, P (2003). Atopy and depression: results from the Northern Finland 1966 Birth Cohort Study. Molecular Psychiatry 8, 738744.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Van Steenbergen, H, Band, GPH, Hommel, B (2010). In the mood for adaptation: how affect regulates conflict-driven control. Psychological Science 21, 16291634.Google Scholar
Van Steenbergen, H, Booij, L, Band, GPH, Hommel, B, van der Does, AJW (2012). Affective regulation of cognitive-control adjustments in remitted depressive patients after acute tryptophan depletion. Cognitive, Affective and Behavioral Neuroscience 12, 280286.Google Scholar
Virchow, JC, Kay, S, Demoly, P, Mullol, J, Canonica, W, Higgins, V (2011). Impact of ocular symptoms on quality of life (QoL), work productivity and resource utilisation in allergic rhinitis patients – an observational, cross sectional study in four countries in Europe. Journal of Medical Economics 14, 305314.Google Scholar
Wilken, JA, Berkowitz, R, Kane, R (2002). Decrements in vigilance and cognitive functioning associated with ragweed-induced allergic rhinitis. Annals of Allergy, Asthma and Immunology 89, 372380.Google Scholar