Skip to main content Accessibility help
×
Home

Abstract motion is no longer abstract

  • Teenie Matlock (a1)

Abstract

Dynamic conceptualization is a fundamental notion in cognitive linguistics. Abstract motion is one type of dynamic conceptualization. It is said to structure descriptions of static scenes such as ‘The mountain range goes from Mexico to Canada’, and in doing so, invokes a subjective sense of motion or state change. In recent years, a growing body of experimental research supports this claim. However, additional work is needed to understand the dynamics of abstract motion and the extent to which it generalizes. This paper provides some background on abstract motion and reports two new experiments that investigate two unexplored types of abstract motion, including visual paths and pattern paths. Together, the results indicate that abstract motion plays a central role in language use and understanding.

Copyright

Corresponding author

Correspondence addresses: Teenie Matlock, Cognitive Science Program, School of Social Sciences, Humanities and Arts, University of California, Merced, CA 95343, USA. E-mail: tmatlock@ucmerced.edu

References

Hide All
Anderson, S., Matlock, T., Fausey, C. M. & Spivey, M. J.. 2008. On the path to understanding the on-line processing of grammatical aspect. Proceedings of the 30th annual meeting of the Cognitive Science Society. 22532258.
Anderson, S., Matlock, T. & Spivey, M. J.. 2010. The role of grammatical aspect in the dynamics of spatial descriptions. Proceedings of spatial cognition 2010. Springer Lecture Notes in Computer Science/Lecture Notes in Artificial Intelligence Series. 11981203.
Barsalou, L. W. 2008. Grounded cognition. Annual Review of Psychology 59. 617645.
Bergen, B. & Wheeler, K.. 2010. Grammatical aspect and mental simulation. Brain and Language. 150158.
Boroditsky, L. 2000. Metaphoric structuring: Understanding time through spatial metaphors. Cognition 75. 128.
Boroditsky, L. & Ramscar, M.. 2002. The roles of body and mind in abstract thought. Psychological Science 13. 185188.
Clark, H. H. 1973. Space, time, semantics, and the child. In Moore, T. E. (ed.), Cognitive development and the acquisition of language. 2763. San Diego: Academic Press.
Dehaene, Stanislas. 1997. The number sense: How the mind creates mathematics. Oxford University Press.
Evans, V. 2004. The structure of time: Language, meaning and temporal cognition. Amsterdam: John Benjamins.
Fauconnier, G. 1997. Mappings in language and thought. New York: Cambridge University Press.
Fausey, C. & Matlock, T.. 2010. Can grammar influence voting?Proceedings of the 32nd annual meeting of the Cognitive Science Society, Portland, Oregon. 13301335.
Gibbs, R. W. 2006. Embodiment and cognitive science. New York: Cambridge University Press.
Gibbs, R. W. & Matlock, T.. 2008. Metaphor, imagination, and simulation: Psycholinguistic evidence. In Gibbs, R. W. (ed.), Cambridge handbook of metaphor and thought, 161176. New York: Cambridge University Press.
Greenwood, M. & Matlock, T.. 2009. That's the way it goes with fictive motion. Proceedings of the 31st annual conference of the Cognitive Science Society. 611.
Hegarty, M. 2004. Mechanical reasoning by mental simulation. Trends in Cognitive Science 8. 280285.
Henderson, J. M. & Ferreira, F. (eds.). 2004. The integration of language, vision, and action: Eye movements and the visual world. New York: Psychology Press.
Huumo, T. 2005. How fictive dynamicity motivates aspect marking: The riddle of the Finnish quasi-resultative construction. Cognitive Linguistics 16. 113144.
Kourtzi, Z. & Kanwisher, N.. 2000. Activation in human MT/MST by static images with implied motion. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience 12. 4855.
Lakoff, G. & Johnson, M.. 1980. Metaphors we live by. Chicago, IL: Chicago University Press.
Lakoff, G. & Johnson, M.. 1999. Philosophy in the flesh: The embodied mind and its challenge to western thought. New York: Basic Books.
Lakoff, G. & Núñez, R.. 2000. Where mathematics comes from: How the embodied mind brings mathematics into being. New York: Basic Books.
Lakoff, G. & Turner, M.. 1989. More than cool reason. Chicago: The University of Chicago Press.
Langacker, R. W. 1986. Abstract motion. Proceedings of the 12th annual meeting of the Berkeley Linguistics Society. 455471.
Langacker, R. W. 1987. Foundations of cognitive grammar: Theoretical prerequisites. Stanford, CA: Stanford University Press.
Langacker, R. W. 1999. Grammar and conceptualization. Berlin: Mouton de Gruyter.
Madden, C. J. & Zwaan, R.. 2003. How does verb aspect constrain event representations? Memory and Cognition 31. 663672.
Madden, C. J. & Therriault, D. J.. 2009. How does verb aspect constrain perceptual representations? Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology 62. 12941302.
Magliano, J. P. & Schleich, M. C.. 2000. Verb aspect and situation models. Discourse Processes 29. 83112.
Matlock, T. 2004a. The conceptual motivation of fictive motion. In Radden, G. & Dirven, R. (eds.), Motivation in grammar, 221248. Amsterdam: John Benjamins.
Matlock, T. 2004b. Fictive motion as cognitive simulation. Memory and Cognition 32. 13891400.
Matlock, T. 2006. Depicting fictive motion in drawings. In Luchenbroers, J. (ed.), Cognitive linguistics: Investigations across languages, fields, and philosophical boundaries, 6785. Amsterdam: John Benjamins.
Matlock, T. In press. The conceptual motivation of aspect. In Radden, G., Koch, P. & Panther, K.-U. (eds.), Motivation in grammar and the lexicon: Cognitive, communicative, perceptual and socio-cultural factors. Amsterdam & Philadelphia: John Benjamins.
Matlock, T., Ramscar, M. & Boroditsky, L.. 2004. The experiential basis of motion language. In da Silva, A. Soares, Torres, A. & Gonçalves, M. (eds.), Linguagem, cultura e cognicao: Estudo de linguistica cognitiva, 4357. Coimbra: Almedina.
Matlock, T., Ramscar, M. & Boroditsky, L.. 2005. The experiential link between spatial and temporal language. Cognitive Science 29. 655664.
Matlock, T., Ramscar, M. & Srinivasan, M.. 2005. Even the most abstract motion influences temporal understanding. Proceedings of the 27th annual conference of the Cognitive Science Society. 2527.
Matlock, T. & Richardson, D. C.. 2004. Do eye movements go with fictive motion?Proceedings of the 26th annual conference of the Cognitive Science Society. 909914.
Matsumoto, Y. 1996. Subjective motion and English and Japanese verbs. Cognitive Linguistics 7. 183226.
McGlone, M. S. & Harding, J. L.. 1998. Back (or forward?) to the future: The role of perspective in temporal language comprehension. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning Memory and Cognition 24(5). 12111223.
Mishra, R. 2009. Interaction of language and visual attention: Evidence from production and comprehension. Progress in Brain Research. 277292.
Núñez, R., Motz, B. A. & Teuscher, U.. 2006. Time after time: The psychological reality of ego- and time-reference-point distinction in metaphorical construals of time. Metaphor and Symbol 21. 133146.
Pecher, D. & Zwaan, R. A. (eds.). 2005. Grounding cognition: The role of perception and action in memory, language, and thinking. Cambridge, UK: Cambridge University Press.
Ramscar, M., Matlock, T. & Dye, M.. In press. Running down the clock: The role of expectation in our understanding of time and motion. Language and Cognitive Processes.
Richardson, D. C. & Matlock, T. 2007. The integration of figurative language and static depictions: An eye movement study of fictive motion. Cognition 102. 129138.
Rizzolatti, G. & Sinigaglia, C.. 2008. Mirrors in the brain: How our minds share actions, emotions, and experience. Oxford: Oxford University Press.
Rojo, A. & Valenzuela, J.. 2003. Fictive motion in English and Spanish. International Journal of English Studies 3. 123150.
Saygin, A., McCullough, S., Alač, M. & Emmorey, K.. In press. Modulation of BOLD response in motion-sensitive lateral temporal cortex by real and fictive motion sentences. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience.
Schwartz, D. L. & Black, T.. 1999. Inferences through imagined actions: Knowing by simulated doing. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition 25. 116136.
Slobin, D. I. 2008. Relations between paths of motion and paths of vision: A crosslinguistic and developmental exploration. In Gathercole, V. C. Mueller (ed.), Routes to language: Studies in Honor of Melissa Bowerman, 197221. Mahwah, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates.
Spivey, M. J. 2007. Continuity of mind. New York: Oxford University Press.
Spivey, M. & Geng, J.. 2001. Oculomotor mechanisms activated by imagery and memory: Eye movements to absent objects. Psychological Research 65. 235241.
Talmy, L. 1983. How language structures space. In Pick, H. & Acredolo, L. P. (eds.), Spatial orientation: Theory, research, and application, 225282. New York: Plenum Press.
Talmy, L. 1996. Fictive motion in language and “ception”. In Bloom, P., Peterson, M. A., Nadel, L. & Garrett, M. F. (eds.), Language and space, 211276. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.
Talmy, L. 2000. Toward a cognitive semantics, volume I: Conceptual structuring systems. Cambridge: MIT Press.
Tanenhaus, M. K. & Spivey-Knowlton, M. J.. 1996. Eye-tracking. Special issue: A guide to spoken word recognition paradigms. Language and Cognitive Processes 11. 583588.
Teuscher, U., McQuire, M., Collins, J. & Coulson, S.. 2008. Congruity effects in time and space: Behavioral and ERP measures. Cognitive Science 32. 563578.
Wallentin, M., Lund, T. E., Østergaard, S., Østergaard, L. & Roepstorff, A.. 2005. Motion verb sentences activate left posterior middle temporal cortex despite static context. NeuroReport 16. 649652.

Keywords

Metrics

Altmetric attention score

Full text views

Total number of HTML views: 0
Total number of PDF views: 0 *
Loading metrics...

Abstract views

Total abstract views: 0 *
Loading metrics...

* Views captured on Cambridge Core between <date>. This data will be updated every 24 hours.

Usage data cannot currently be displayed