Skip to main content Accessibility help
×
Home
Hostname: page-component-7f7b94f6bd-5bz6h Total loading time: 0.317 Render date: 2022-06-30T18:07:06.814Z Has data issue: true Feature Flags: { "shouldUseShareProductTool": true, "shouldUseHypothesis": true, "isUnsiloEnabled": true, "useRatesEcommerce": false, "useNewApi": true } hasContentIssue true

Acquired mirroring and intentional communication in primates

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  11 March 2014

Klaus Zuberbühler*
Affiliation:
School of Psychology and Neuroscience, University of St Andrews, Scotland; Institute of Biology, Department of Comparative Cognition, University of Neuchatel, Switzerland. E-mail: kz3@st-andrews.ac.uk

Abstract

Arbib offers a coherent proposal of how the brain has evolved to become language-capable. Integral to the argument are mirror neurons, cells discovered in macaque brains with interesting firing patterns, and studies on gestural communication of great apes. Here, I first discuss some complexities of the recent mirror neuron literature, which suggest that ‘mirroring’ may be an ontogenetically acquired, not an evolved, feature of neurons. Second, it is now clear that chimpanzee vocal behaviour is strongly mediated by social variables, and that individuals can use vocalisations to persuade and inform others, facts that have implications for gestural theories of language evolution. I conclude with discussing research most needed for making progress in understanding how human language has evolved.

Type
Learning from primate communication
Copyright
Copyright © UK Cognitive Linguistics Association 2013

Access options

Get access to the full version of this content by using one of the access options below. (Log in options will check for institutional or personal access. Content may require purchase if you do not have access.)

References

Arbib, M. A. 2012. How the brain got language. Oxford: Oxford University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Arnold, K. & Zuberbühler, K.. 2006. Language evolution: Semantic combinations in primate calls. Nature 441. 303.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Clay, Z. & Zuberbühler, K.. 2011. Bonobos extract meaning from call sequences. Plos One 6(4). e18786.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Cook, R. 2012. The ontogenetic origins of mirror neurons: Evidence from ‘tool-use’ and ‘audiovisual’ mirror neurons. Biology Letters 8(5). 856859.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Crockford, C., Wittig, R. M., Mundry, R. & Zuberbühler, K.. 2012. Wild chimpanzees inform ignorant group members of danger. Current Biology 22(2). 142146.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Enard, W., Przeworski, M., Fisher, S. E., Lai, C. S. L., Wiebe, V., Kitano, T., Monaco, A. P. & Päabo, S.. 2002. Molecular evolution of FOXP2, a gene involved in speech and language. Nature 418(6900). 869872.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Fitch, W. T. & Zuberbühler, K.. 2013. Primate precursors to human language: Beyond discontinuity. In Altenmuller, E., Schmidt, S. & Zimmermann, E. (Eds.), The evolution of emotional communication. From sounds in nonhuman mammals to speech and music in man, 2648. Oxford: Oxford University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Gallese, V. & Goldman, A.. 1998. Mirror neurons and the simulation theory of mind-reading. Trends in Cognitive Sciences 2(12). 493501.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Gallese, V. & Sinigaglia, C.. 2011. What is so special about embodied simulation? Trends in Cognitive Sciences 15(11). 512519.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Genty, E., Breuer, T., Hobaiter, C. & Byrne, R. W.. 2009. Gestural communication of the gorilla (gorilla gorilla): Repertoire, intentionality and possible origins. Animal Cognition 12(3). 527546. doi: 10.1007/s10071-009-0213-4CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Herbinger, I., Papworth, S., Boesch, C. & Zuberbühler, K.. 2009. Vocal, gestural and locomotor responses of wild chimpanzees to familiar and unfamiliar intruders: A playback study. Animal Behaviour 78(6). 13891396.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Heyes, C. 2010. Where do mirror neurons come from? Neuroscience and Biobehavioral Reviews 34(4). 575583.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Hobaiter, C. & Byrne, R. W.. 2011. The gestural Repertoire of the wild chimpanzee. Animal Cognition 14(5). 745767.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Hockett, C. F. 1960. The origin of speech. Scientific American 203(3). 8996.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Langergraber, K. E., Pruefer, K., Rowney, C., Boesch, C., Crockford, C., Fawcett, K., Inoue, E., Inoue-Muruyama, M., Mitani, J. C., Muller, M. N., Robbins, M. M., Schubert, G., Stoinski, T. S., Viola, B., Watts, D., Wittig, R. M., Wrangham, R. W., Zuberbühler, K., Pääbo, S. & Vigilant, L.. 2012. Generation times in wild chimpanzees and gorillas suggest earlier divergence times in great ape and human evolution. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America 109(39). 1571615721.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Laporte, M. N. C. & Zuberbühler, K.. 2010. Vocal greeting behaviour in wild chimpanzee females. Animal Behaviour 80(3). 467473.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Liebal, K., Carpenter, M. & Tomasello, M.. 2010. Infants' use of shared experience in declarative pointing. Infancy 15(5). 545556.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
McComb, K. & Semple, S.. 2005. Coevolution of vocal communication and sociality in primates. Biology Letters 1(4). 381385.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Mukamel, R., Ekstrom, A. D., Kaplan, J., Iacoboni, M. & Fried, I.. 2010. Single-neuron responses in humans during execution and observation of actions. Current Biology 20(8). 750756.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Ouattara, K., Lemasson, A. & Zuberbühler, K.. 2009. Wild campbell's monkeys concatenate vocalizations into context-specific call sequences. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America 106(51). 2202622031.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Papworth, S., Böse, A.-S., Barker, J. & Zuberbühler, K.. 2008. Male blue monkeys alarm call in response to danger experienced by others. Biology Letters 4. 472475.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Pika, S., Liebal, K. & Tomasello, M.. 2005. Gestural communication in subadult bonobos (pan paniscus): Repertoire and use. American Journal of Primatology 65(1). 3961.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Rizzolatti, G. & Arbib, M. A.. 1998. Language within our grasp. Trends in Neurosciences 21(5). 188194.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Rizzolatti, G. & Craighero, L.. 2004. The mirror-neuron system. Annual Review of Neuroscience 27. 169192.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Roberts, A. I., Vick, S.-J., Roberts, S. G. B., Buchanan-Smith, H. M. & Zuberbühler, K.. 2012. A structure-based Repertoire of manual gestures in wild chimpanzees: Statistical analyses of a graded communication system. Evolution and Human Behavior 33(5). 578589.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Seyfarth, R. M., Cheney, D. L. & Marler, P.. 1980. Monkey responses to three different alarm calls: Evidence of predator classification and semantic communication. Science 210. 801803.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Slocombe, K. E., Kaller, T., Turman, L., Townsend, S., Papworth, S., Squibbs, P. & Zuberbühler, K.. 2010. Production of food-associated calls in wild male chimpanzees is dependent on the composition of the audience. Behavioral Ecological Sociobiology 64(12). 19591966.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Slocombe, K. E. & Zuberbuhler, K.. 2005. Agonistic screams in wild chimpanzees (pan troglodytes schweinfurthii) vary as a function of social role. Journal of Comparative Psychology 119(1). 6777.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Slocombe, K. E. & Zuberbühler, K.. 2006. Food-associated calls in chimpanzees: Responses to food types or food preferences? Animal Behaviour 72. 989999.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Slocombe, K. E. & Zuberbühler, K.. 2007. Chimpanzees modify recruitment screams as a function of audience composition. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America 104. 1722817233.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Tomasello, M. 2008. Origins of human communication. Cambridge MA: MIT Press.Google Scholar
Tomasello, M. 2010. Why we cooperate. Boston: Review Books.Google Scholar
Townsend, S. W., Deschner, T. & Zuberbühler, K.. 2008. Female chimpanzees use copulation calls flexibly to prevent social competition. PLoS One 3(6). e2431.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Townsend, S. W., Slocombe, K. E., Thompson, M. E. & Zuberbuhler, K.. 2007. Female-led infanticide in wild chimpanzees. Current Biology 17(10). R355R356.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Wich, S. A. & de Vries, H.. (2006). Male monkeys remember which group members have given alarm calls. Proceedings of the Royal Society B-Biological Sciences 73(1587). 735740.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Zuberbühler, K. 2012. Cooperative breeding and the evolution of vocal flexibility. In Tallerman, M. & Gibson, K. (Eds.), Oxford handbook of language evolution, 7181. Oxford: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
6
Cited by

Save article to Kindle

To save this article to your Kindle, first ensure coreplatform@cambridge.org is added to your Approved Personal Document E-mail List under your Personal Document Settings on the Manage Your Content and Devices page of your Amazon account. Then enter the ‘name’ part of your Kindle email address below. Find out more about saving to your Kindle.

Note you can select to save to either the @free.kindle.com or @kindle.com variations. ‘@free.kindle.com’ emails are free but can only be saved to your device when it is connected to wi-fi. ‘@kindle.com’ emails can be delivered even when you are not connected to wi-fi, but note that service fees apply.

Find out more about the Kindle Personal Document Service.

Acquired mirroring and intentional communication in primates
Available formats
×

Save article to Dropbox

To save this article to your Dropbox account, please select one or more formats and confirm that you agree to abide by our usage policies. If this is the first time you used this feature, you will be asked to authorise Cambridge Core to connect with your Dropbox account. Find out more about saving content to Dropbox.

Acquired mirroring and intentional communication in primates
Available formats
×

Save article to Google Drive

To save this article to your Google Drive account, please select one or more formats and confirm that you agree to abide by our usage policies. If this is the first time you used this feature, you will be asked to authorise Cambridge Core to connect with your Google Drive account. Find out more about saving content to Google Drive.

Acquired mirroring and intentional communication in primates
Available formats
×
×

Reply to: Submit a response

Please enter your response.

Your details

Please enter a valid email address.

Conflicting interests

Do you have any conflicting interests? *