Hostname: page-component-8448b6f56d-c47g7 Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-04-19T07:50:48.661Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

Shawi (Chayahuita)

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  08 March 2019

Luis Miguel Rojas-Berscia
Affiliation:
Radboud Universiteit Nijmegen, Centre for Language Studies, & Max Planck Institute for Psycholinguistics, Language and Cognition Department & University of Queensland, School of Languages and Cultures l.rojasberscia@uq.edu.au
Andrés Napurí
Affiliation:
University of Oxford, School of Anthropology and Museum Ethnography, & Pontificia Universidad Católica del Perú, Departamento Académico de Humanidades a.napuri@pucp.edu.pe
Lei Wang
Affiliation:
Tongji University, School of Foreign Languages, & Radboud Universiteit Nijmegen, Centre for Language Studies wl1410512@gmail.com
Rights & Permissions [Opens in a new window]

Extract

Shawi1 is the language of the indigenous Shawi/Chayahuita people in Northwestern Amazonia, Peru. It belongs to the Kawapanan language family, together with its moribund sister language, Shiwilu. It is spoken by about 21,000 speakers (see Rojas-Berscia 2013) in the provinces of Alto Amazonas and Datem del Marañón in the region of Loreto and in the northern part of the region of San Martín, being one of the most vital languages in the country (see Figure 1).2 Although Shawi groups in the Upper Amazon were contacted by Jesuit missionaries during colonial times, the maintenance of their customs and language is striking. To date, most Shawi children are monolingual and have their first contact with Spanish at school. Yet, due to globalisation and the construction of highways by the Peruvian government, many Shawi villages are progressively westernising. This may result in the imminent loss of their indigenous culture and language.

Type
Illustrations of the IPA
Copyright
© International Phonetic Association 2019

ShawiFootnote 1 is the language of the indigenous Shawi/Chayahuita people in Northwestern Amazonia, Peru. It belongs to the Kawapanan language family, together with its moribund sister language, Shiwilu. It is spoken by about 21,000 speakers (see Rojas-Berscia Reference Rojas-Berscia2013) in the provinces of Alto Amazonas and Datem del Marañón in the region of Loreto and in the northern part of the region of San Martín, being one of the most vital languages in the country (see Figure 1).Footnote 2 Although Shawi groups in the Upper Amazon were contacted by Jesuit missionaries during colonial times, the maintenance of their customs and language is striking. To date, most Shawi children are monolingual and have their first contact with Spanish at school. Yet, due to globalisation and the construction of highways by the Peruvian government, many Shawi villages are progressively westernising. This may result in the imminent loss of their indigenous culture and language.

Figure 1 Map of the Kawapanan linguistic area.

As for documentation of Shawi, there was no single grammatical sketch of it until the 1980s, when the Christian missionary and SIL linguist Helen Hart initiated a project to study and describe the language. Hart, together with a large number of consultants coming from very different Shawi-speaking communities, translated the New Testament into Shawi (Hart Reference Hart1978), wrote a very complete dictionary of the language (Hart Reference Hart1988), as well as a short grammar sketch, and wrote the first phonological sketch of the language (Gordon de Powlison et al. Reference Gordon de Powlison, Hart and Hart1976). More recently, Barraza de García (Reference Barraza de García2005) wrote a Ph.D. dissertation on the verbal system of Shawi, and Luis Miguel Rojas-Berscia worked on the syntax and semantics of causative constructions (Rojas-Berscia Reference Rojas-Berscia2013) and the distribution of split ergativity in the language (Rojas-Berscia & Bourdeau Reference Rojas-Berscia and Bourdeau2018).

There were, however, a few substantial attempts to analyse the history of the language. One of these was Beuchat & Rivet (Reference Beuchat and Rivet1909), dedicated to the study of the origins of Kawapanan languages, mentioning for the first time a clear cut between this language family and Chicham/Jivaroan. The number of comparative and philological studies of the Kawapanan languages increased exponentially during the last decade. Valenzuela (Reference Valenzuela and Adelaar2011) worked extensively on the reconstruction of Proto-Kawapanan, as well as on possible areal connections with major Andean languages such as Quechua and Aymara (Valenzuela Reference Valenzuela2015). Thanks to current philological analyses of 18th-century Shiwilu (Alexander-Bakkerus Reference Alexander-Bakkerus2013, Reference Alexander-Bakkerus2016; Rojas-Berscia Reference Rojas-Berscia2017), and to current descriptions of both Shawi and Shiwilu, we will be able to better understand the underlying dynamics behind the emergence of the great linguistic diversity found in Northwestern Amazonia.

Consonants

The Shawi phoneme inventory consists of twelve consonants and four vowels. The consonants phoneme group consists of five plosives, two fricatives, two nasals, one tap, and two approximants. The following consonant chart is adapted from Rojas-Berscia (Reference Rojas-Berscia2013: 23).

Minimal pairs are offered below in phonemic transcriptions, with syllable boundaries indicated by dots and stress by [ˈ]. The list was kept as minimal as possible. Some consonants are repeated to illustrate contrastiveness as accurately as possible. The words are followed by their orthographic representations and English glosses. (Abbreviations used in this paper are listed at the end of the transcription section.)

Unlike Barraza de García (Reference Barraza de García2005: 37), we do not include the segment [ʰ] as a phoneme of the language. As already noted by Hart (Reference Hart1988) – evident from her transcriptions and previous analyses of the phonology of the language (Gordon de Powlison et al. Reference Gordon de Powlison, Hart and Hart1976) – [ʰ] is not phonemic, as will be shown in the ‘Processes’ section.

Plosives

There are five plosives in Shawi. On the basis of proprioceptive and visual observations, /p/ is an unaspirated bilabial plosive, /t/ is a laminal alveolar plosive, and /k/ is a dorsal velar plosive. All of them occur in onset position. None of these plosives are allowed in coda position. Below we present some examples:

/t͡ʃ/occurs at the beginning of the word, as in the word ˈt͡ʃi.mi.naw ‘I die’. / t͡ʃ/ is a laminal postalveolar affricate. The occurrence of this consonant inside the word or root is rare, but possible. Below we present some cases:

Some borrowings which present the prototypical /ʤ͡/ of Amazonian Spanish are accommodated to /t͡ʃ/ in the language, such as the word for ‘street’, kachi [ˈka.t͡ʃi] (Spanish ‘calle’). Apart from these cases and some reduplications such as [ˈt͡ʃiɁ.t͡ʃi.wi.tɘɁ] ‘intestine’, this phoneme is very productive in front of /i/, but not with the other vowels, which suggests a diachronic palatalisation process from *ki/ti >[t͡ʃi].Footnote 3

The glottal stop in Shawi does not behave like most Shawi consonants: it only occupies the coda position:

Plosives can undergo inter-vocalic voicing. This phenomenon occurs mostly in southern varieties:

In this case, /p/ underwent voicing and was realised as its allophone [b]. Nevertheless, in most southern varieties of Shawi, [b] is realised as [w] due to lenition. This process is progressively leading to the elision of the consonant. Below we present one of the most prototypical examples:

Voicing after nasals also occurs in Shawi, as well as in Shiwilu (see Valenzuela & Gussenhoven Reference Valenzuela and Gussenhoven2013, Madalengoitia Reference Madalengoitia2013). This phenomenon, however, is absent in southern varieties such as the Paranapura variety. Barraza de García (Reference Barraza de García2005: 49) documents the occurrence of this phenomenon in Cahuapanas and Sillay Shawi.Footnote 4 For example:

Fricatives

There are two fricatives in Shawi. /s/ is a laminal alveolar fricative, while /ʃ/ is a laminal post-alveolar fricative. Like the plosives, both occur in onset position, never in coda position.

Fricatives can also undergo voicing. This phenomenon occurs mostly in southern varieties, e.g.:

Nasals

There are two nasal consonants in Shawi. /m/ is a bilabial nasal, while /n/ is a laminal alveolar nasal. Both occur in onset position. /n/ can occur in coda position.

Nasals assimilate in place to a following consonant. Below we present some examples. In some cases, however, nasals coalesce with the preceeding vowel, which becomes nasalised. This is explained in the section on vowels.

As shown in the last two words, the nasal /n/ is realised as its velar allophone [ŋ] in coda position.

Tap

In Shawi there is only a single tap – the apical alveolar /ɾ/. As most consonant phonemes in the language, it only occurs at the beginning of the syllable, e.g.:

/ɾ/ is neutralised in word-initial onsets and, word-internally, after a glottal stop. Once neutralisation takes place, /ɾ/ is realised as [n]. Consequently, the occurrence of [ɾ] as onset of the first syllable is almostFootnote 6 inexistent.Footnote 7 Examples include:

Approximants

Shawi is characterised by a frequent occurrence of approximants, just like its sister language Shiwilu (Valenzuela & Gussenhoven Reference Valenzuela and Gussenhoven2013: 100). Approximants behave very much like their other consonantal counterparts. They always occur in onset position and never in coda position. /w/ is a labial-velar approximant. /j/ is a palatal approximant. Below we present some examples:

However, they cannot occur in onset position when a vowel of a similar articulation follows: /w/ never occurs before /o/, and /j/ never occurs before /i/.

Vowels

Shawi has four vowels: the close front unrounded /i/, the close-mid central unrounded /ɘ/, the open central /a/ and the close-mid back rounded /o/. A similar analysis can be found in Gordon de Powlison et al. (Reference Gordon de Powlison, Hart and Hart1976). Vowels occur in every word context and there is no clear synchronic quantity contrast.Footnote 8 The vowel chart for Shawi, including its main vocalic features, is adapted from Rojas-Berscia (Reference Rojas-Berscia2013: 30).

Minimal pairs are offered as phonemic transcriptions with syllable boundaries indicated by dots and stress by [ˈ]. Some vowels are repeated to illustrate contrastiveness as accurately as possible.

Below we provide an acoustic analysis, carried out with the help of a male consultant of Pueblo Chayahuita. The analysis is based on 16 words from a Swadesh list. The words used were: ka ‘I’, a’na ‘one’, sami ‘fish’, and nante’ ‘foot’ for /a/; pii ‘sun’, ira ‘path’, iwan ‘wind’ inchinan ‘left’ for /i/; werun ‘leaf’, pen ‘fire’, teme ‘louse’, and kema ‘you’ for /ɘ/; and nupa ‘soil’, shu’shu ‘nipple’, nuya ‘good’, and ku ‘no’ for /o/. Table 1 displays the means of F1 and F2 for each of the vowels in the four words:

Table 1 Means of F1 and F2 after four words.

The IPA symbol used for each token is based on vowel quality and our familiarity with the sounds. The distribution of vowel sounds in the plot is presented in Figure 2 (on next page).

Figure 2 Vowel ellipses for /i ɘ o a/ in the F1/F2 plane. Each vowel contains 24 scatters (4 words × 2 repetitions × 3 equidistant measurements taken from the steady-state portion). Raw formant values were converted to bark and sigma ellipses were superimposed (number of sigmas = 2) in Praat (Boersma & Weenink Reference Boersma and Weenink2016).

There are no nasal vowels in the language. However, Shawi vowels sometimes become nasalised when followed by a nasal, while the nasal itself is deleted. This occurs only in word-final position.Footnote 9 Below we present some examples:

Table 2 Distribution of consonantal phonemes. (Y) indicates a consonant occurs in position (X) while (N) indicates that it does not. (–) indicates that, although that combination is possible, such a syllable does not appear in the corpus. #_ = word-initial;. _/ = syllable-initial, V._ = post-vocalic; C._ = post-consonantal; _. = syllable final, _# = word-final; _(x) = preceding vowel (x). /p t k t ʃ s ʃ/constitute a natural class in the language.

Syllable structure

The general syllable structure of the language is (C)V(C[+nasal, +dorsal]/[+contr gl]), with both the onset and the coda being optional. CC-onsets are normally the product of reductions, such as the vm+non.fut construction te-ra>tra-, and the 1.incl marker, pu-wa>-pwa.

A detailed overview of the distribution and restrictions of occurrence of consonantal phonemes in syllable structure is presented in Table 2.

Processes

[ʰ]epenthesis in coda position

Shawi displays an [h]-like segment in some words. Figures 3 and 4 show the spectrograms of two words, kema ‘you’ and keken ‘heavy’, respectively. The former does not display [h]. The latter shows a friction in the coda position of the first syllable.

Figure 3 Spectrographic representation of /kɘma/ ‘you’. In this case no [ʰ] is found.

Figure 4 Spectrographic representation of /kɘkɘn/ ‘heavy’. The friction found in the coda of the first syllable has been marked.

The occurrence of [ʰ] was previously described as a co-vowel by Gordon de Powlison et al. (Reference Gordon de Powlison, Hart and Hart1976: 2), and as an independent phoneme of the language by Barraza de García (Reference Barraza de García2005). Both accounts agree upon the fact that [h] only occurs in coda position. Yet, the occurrence of can be explained in terms of a simple lexical rule of epenthesis (Gussenhoven & Jacobs Reference Gussenhoven and Jacobs2011: 164). Epenthesis is expressed as insertion of [ʰ] in coda position. This is a rule specified in the lexicon. Thus, the rule only applies before any derivational or inflectional processes have taken place. [ʰ] is then inserted as a coda to an open syllable if the onset of the following syllable is an obstruent (plosives and fricatives). All these conditions must be met in order for [ʰ] to occur. The fact that [h] only occurs before an obstruent resembles the phonetic environment required in Huariapano (Parker Reference Parker1994: 108–109) for the [ʰ] epenthesis.

Below we present some examples, including the underlying form as specified in the lexicon, followed by the form which already underwent [ʰ] epenthesis. Some derivational or inflectional processes were specified as occurring postlexically:

All examples meet all conditions. Nitepaw [ni+ˈtɘʰ.pa+w] is of special interest. Had the rule not applied at the lexical level, the outcome would have been *[niʰ.ˈtɘʰ.paw]. The rule, nevertheless, applied before the prefixation of ni- ‘reciprocal’.

There are some suffixes, such as the progressive -sa, the genitive -ken and the additive -pu, which also trigger the occurrence of [ʰ] outside the main lexical rule.Footnote 10 The suffixes -pu and -ken trigger the occurrence of [ʰ] in the preceding syllable. By contrast, -sa triggers the occurrence of [ʰ] in the following syllable. Although the rule applies postlexically, the syllable in question must be open. This area of morphophonemics, however, still awaits further exploration. Below we present some examples (bold indicates the place of these suffixes in the examples):

Syncope

Syncope is frequent in Shawi speech. There are two suffixes that are commonly syncopied: -we 1.sg and -te vm. Examples include the following:

Transcription of the recorded passage

The story of the North Wind and the Sun was translated from its original English version in one session with the help of speaker A. The transcription is phonemic. Parentheses indicate intonational phrases.

Phonemic transcription

This is a broad transcription, which exclusively employs segmental symbols that were assigned to vocalic and consonantal phonemes.

(ˈi.wan) (ˈpiʔ.i.ˈpi.pi.ɾin.ˈkɘ.ɾan) (i.ˈnawa) (ˈpiʔ.i) (ˈja.wa.pi.ɾi.na.wɘ) (ˈin.ta) (ˈt͡ʃi.ni t͡ʃi.ˈni.kɘn.ˈtaʔ.to.na) || (i.ˈna.ɾan) (ˈwɘʔ.nin) (ˈaʔ.na ˈi.ɾa.ka.ˈma.jo) (ˈjaʔ.sɘʔ.ko.tɘ.ɾin) (ˈaʔ.na) (wɘ.ˈto.ja ˈko.ton) || (i.na.ˈpi.ta) (ˈkan.pi) (ˈaʔ.na) (jon.ˈki.nan.kɘ) (ˈin.ta) (ˈi.na) (ˈi.ɾa.ka.ˈma.jo) (ˈko.ton) (ˈmaʔ.tɘ.ɾin.so) (ˈi.na) (ˈni.sa.ɾin) (ˈt͡ʃi.ni t͡ʃi.ˈni.kɘn.ˈto.pi) || (i.ˈna.ran) (ˈpiʔ.i.ˈpi.pi.ɾin.ˈkɘ.ɾan) (ˈi.wan) (ˈt͡ʃi.ni ˈt͡ʃi.ni.kɘn) (ˈpi.wi.pi.ɾin.wɘ) (ˈi.ɾa.ka.ˈma.jo.so) (a.ˈkɘ.tɘ) (ˈja.sɘ.ko.tɘ.ɾin) (ˈko.to.nɘn.kɘ) || (i.ˈna.ɾan) (to.ˈpi.nan) (ˈi.wan) (ˈpiʔ.i.ˈpi.pi.ɾin.kɘ.ɾan) (ka.ˈno.tɘ.ɾin) || (i.ˈna.ɾan) (ˈpiʔ.i) (ˈaʔ.pi.nin) (ˈwɘ.no.kaʔ) | (i.ˈna.ɾan) (ˈna.po.a.wa.ton) (ˈi.ɾa.ka.ˈma.jo) (ˈko.ton) (ˈi.nan.pi.ɾa.ɾin) || (i.ˈna.po.t͡ʃin) (ˈi.wan) (ˈpiʔ.i.ˈpi.pi.ɾin.ˈkɘ.ɾan) (ˈna.ni.tɘ.ɾin ˈno.wi.tɘ.ka.ˈma.ɾɘ) (ˈpiʔ.i) (ˈt͡ʃi.ni t͡ʃi.ˈni.kɘn ˈni.ton) (ˈka.to.ˈɾo.sa.ˈkɘ.ɾan) ||

Orthographic version

This is an orthographic version, in the current official Shawi alphabet, as requested by our consultants.

Iwan pi’ipihpirinkeran inawa Pi’i yawapirinawe inta chini chinihken ta’tuna. Inaran we’nin a’na irakamayu yase’kuterin a’na wehtuya kuhtun. Inapita kampi a’na yunkinanke inta ina irahkamayu kuhtun ma’terinsu ina nihsarin chini chinihken tuhpi. Inaran pi’ipihpirinkeran Iwan chini chinihken piwipirinwe irahkamayu ahkete yasekuterin kuhtunenke. Inaran tuhpinan Iwan pi’ipihpirinkeran kanuhterin. Inaran Pi’i a’pini wenuka’, inaran nahpuawatun irakamayu kuhtun inanpirarin. Inahpuchin Iwan pi’ipihpirinkeran naniterin nuwitekamare Pi’i chini chinihken nihtun kahturusakeran.

Glossed and translated version

Abbreviations

The abbreviations used in this Illustration rely primarily on the Leipzig Glossing Rules.

Acknowledgements

We are grateful to Amalia Arvaniti and two anonymous referees for the insightful comments on a previous version of this Illustration. We also thank André Radtke for his help with the optimisation of the audio files. The first author acknowledges the financial and academic support from the Language in Interaction Research Consortium in the Netherlands. The authors greatly acknowledge the useful comments and criticisms on previous drafts of the present Illustration by Carlos Gussenhoven, Pieter Muysken, Steve Levinson, Pieter Seuren, Hamed Rahmani, Gunter Senft, Simeon Floyd, and Roberto Zariquiey. The first author is grateful to Pilar Valenzuela for the initial encouragement to study Kawapanan languages, as well as for her insighful comments during the Amazonicas VI conference in Leticia, Colombia, and Tabatinga, Brazil, in May 2016.

Supplementary material

To view supplementary material for this article, please visit: https://doi.org/10.1017/S0025100318000415.

Footnotes

1 The audio files were recorded using a Zoom H6 Handy Recorder and a ME64 Sennheiser Cardioid microphone.

2 The map also shows the area where Munichi, an isolate, is spoken. This language is of special importance since it has been in contact with Kawapanan languages before and after the settlement of the Spaniards in this region.

3 Colonial evidence for this phenomenon can be found in Beuchat & Rivet (Reference Beuchat and Rivet1909) who document timin- for the verb ‘to die’, as in modern Shawi chiminaw ‘I die’. We can infere, therefore, that Proto-Kawapanan *t palatalised. Other consonants, such as *k, palatalised as well. A more detailed discussion can be found in Rojas-Berscia & Nikulin (Reference Rojas-Berscia and Bourdeau2018).

4 This phenomenon is also documented in Southern Mayna, the possible ancestor of Modern Shawi (Rojas-Berscia Reference Rojas-Berscia2015).

5 We are not marking the primary stress in this word because of its complex morphosyntactic nature. More on this phenomenon can be found in the section on metrical stress.

6 (Hart Reference Hart1988: 198) registers the word ropatërin [ɾu.pa.tɨ.ɾin] ‘to tremble/shiver’. However, it is absent in our corpus.

7 The Proto-Kawapanan phoneme */l/ became Shawi [n] in onset position at the beginning of words, and [ɾ] in word-internal onsets (see Rojas-Berscia & Nikulin Reference Rojas-Berscia and Bourdeau2018).

8 However, quantity constrast can be marginally observed in some words, as observed by Gordon de Powlison et al. (Reference Gordon de Powlison, Hart and Hart1976). This awaits further investigation.

9 It must be noted that this is subject to variation. There is no categorical nasalisation of vowels before word-final nasals. This is a topic which awaits further research.

10 There is one single exception to the rule, the verb nih- ‘to be’, which requires [ʰ].

References

Alexander-Bakkerus, Astrid. 2013. Vocabulario enla lengua castellano, la del ynga y xebera [Vocabulary in the Castilian, Inga and Xebera languages]. STUF – Language Typology and Universals 66(3), 229256.Google Scholar
Alexander-Bakkerus, Astrid. 2016. Eighteenth Century Xebero: Mss. Add. 25,323 and 25,324 of The British Library, London (LINCOM Studies in Native American Linguistics 77). München: LINCOM.Google Scholar
Barraza de García, Yris Julia. 2005. El sistema verbal en la lengua shawi [The verbal system in the Shawi language]. Ph.D. thesis, Federal University of Pernambuco.Google Scholar
Beuchat, Henri & Rivet, Paul. 1909. La famille linguistique Cahuapana [The Kawapanan language family]. Zeitschrift für Ethnologie 41, 616634.Google Scholar
Boersma, Paul & Weenink, David. 2016. Praat: Doing phonetics by computer [computer program]. Version 6.0.16. http://www.praat.org/ (5 April 2016).Google Scholar
Gordon de Powlison, Esther, Hart, Helen & Hart, George. 1976. La fonología del chayahuita [The phonology of Shawi]. Datos Etno-Lingüísticos: Colección de los archivos del ILV 28 [Collection of the archives of the Summer Institute of LInguistics], Lima–Summer Institute of Linguistics. Lima: Instituto Lingüstico de Verano.Google Scholar
Gussenhoven, Carlos & Jacobs, Haike. 2011. Understanding phonology, 3rd edn. London: Hodder.Google Scholar
Hart, Helen. 1978. El Nuevo Testamento de nuestro señor Jesucristo [The New Testament of our Lord Jesus Christ]. Orlando, FL: Wycliffe Bible Translator.Google Scholar
Hart, Helen. 1988. Diccionario Chayahuita-Castellano Castellano-Chayahuita, Canponanquë Nisha Nisha Nonacaso [Shawi–Spanish and Spanish–Shawi dictionary, to say many things in our language]. Lima: Summer Institute of Linguistics.Google Scholar
Madalengoitia, María Gracia. 2013. Bosquejo fonológico de la lengua jebero (shiwilu) [Phonological sketch of the Shiwilu language (Kawapanan)]. BA thesis, Pontificia Universidad Católica del Perú, Lima.Google Scholar
Parker, Steve. 1994. Coda epenthesis in Huariapano. International Journal of American Linguistics 60(2), 95119.Google Scholar
Rojas-Berscia, Luis Miguel. 2013. La sintaxis y semántica de las construcciones causativas en el chayahuita balsaportino [The syntax and semantics of causative constructions in the Shawi language as spoken in Balsapuerto]. BA thesis, Pontifical Catholic University of Peru, Lima.Google Scholar
Rojas-Berscia, Luis Miguel. 2015. Mayna: The lost Kawapanan language. LIAMES: Línguas Indígenas Americanas 15, 393407.Google Scholar
Rojas-Berscia, Luis Miguel. 2017. Review of Samuel Fritz (?), El vocabulario de la lengua xebera, una doctrina cristiana en xebero y quechua, y la gramática de la lengua xebera (siglo XVIII) [The vocabulary of the Xebero language, a Christian doctrine in Xebero and Quechua, and the gramar of the Xebero language (18th century)], edited by Astrid Alexander-Bakkerus. Madrid & Frankfurt am Main: Iberoamericana Vervuert (2016). Lexis (Peru) 40(2), 479489.Google Scholar
Rojas-Berscia, Luis Miguel & Bourdeau, Corentin. 2018. Optional or syntactic ergativity in Shawi? Distribution and possible origins. Linguistic Discovery 15(1), 5065.Google Scholar
Rojas-Berscia, Luis Miguel & Nikulin, Andrey. 2018. Proto-Kawapanan, phonological and lexical reconstruction. Ms., Radboud University Nijmegen.Google Scholar
Valenzuela, Pilar. 2011. Contribuciones para la reconstrucción del protocahuapana: Comparación léxica y gramatical de las lenguas jebero y chayahuita [Contributions for the reconstruction of Proto-Kawapanana: Lexical and gramatical comparison of the Shiwilu and Shawi languages]. In Adelaar, Willem F. H., Pilar Valenzuela Bismarck & Roberto Zariquiey Biondi (eds.), Estudios sobre lenguas andinas y amazónicas. Homenaje a Rodolfo Cerrón-Palomino [Studies on Andean and Amazonian languages: Tribute to Rodolfo Cerrón-Palomino], 271304. Lima: Fondo Editorial Pontificia Universidad Católica del Perú.Google Scholar
Valenzuela, Pilar. 2015. ¿Qué tan “amazónicas” son las lenguas kawapana? Contacto con las lenguas centroandinas y elementos para un área lingüística intermedia [How ‘Amazonian’ are Kawapanan Languages? Contact with central-Andean languages and elements for an intermediary linguistic area]. Lexis (Peru) 39(1), 556.Google Scholar
Valenzuela, Pilar & Gussenhoven, Carlos. 2013. Shiwilu (Jebero). Journal of the International Phonetic Association 43(1), 97106.Google Scholar
Figure 0

Figure 1 Map of the Kawapanan linguistic area.

Figure 1

Table 1 Means of F1 and F2 after four words.

Figure 2

Figure 2 Vowel ellipses for /i ɘ o a/ in the F1/F2 plane. Each vowel contains 24 scatters (4 words × 2 repetitions × 3 equidistant measurements taken from the steady-state portion). Raw formant values were converted to bark and sigma ellipses were superimposed (number of sigmas = 2) in Praat (Boersma & Weenink 2016).

Figure 3

Table 2 Distribution of consonantal phonemes. (Y) indicates a consonant occurs in position (X) while (N) indicates that it does not. (–) indicates that, although that combination is possible, such a syllable does not appear in the corpus. #_ = word-initial;. _/ = syllable-initial, V._ = post-vocalic; C._ = post-consonantal; _. = syllable final, _# = word-final; _(x) = preceding vowel (x). /p t k t ʃ s ʃ/constitute a natural class in the language.

Figure 4

Figure 3 Spectrographic representation of /kɘma/ ‘you’. In this case no [ʰ] is found.

Figure 5

Figure 4 Spectrographic representation of /kɘkɘn/ ‘heavy’. The friction found in the coda of the first syllable has been marked.

Supplementary material: File

Rojas-Berscia et al. supplementary material

Rojas-Berscia et al. supplementary material
Download Rojas-Berscia et al. supplementary material(File)
File 15.3 MB