Skip to main content Accessibility help
×
Home

Semantic and lexical features of words dissimilarly affected by non-fluent, logopenic, and semantic primary progressive aphasia

  • Jet M. J. Vonk (a1), Roel Jonkers (a2), H. Isabel Hubbard (a3), Maria Luisa Gorno-Tempini (a4), Adam M. Brickman (a1) and Loraine K. Obler (a5)...

Abstract

Objective:

To determine the effect of three psycholinguistic variables—lexical frequency, age of acquisition (AoA), and neighborhood density (ND)—on lexical-semantic processing in individuals with non-fluent (nfvPPA), logopenic (lvPPA), and semantic primary progressive aphasia (svPPA). Identifying the scope and independence of these features can provide valuable information about the organization of words in our mind and brain.

Method:

We administered a lexical decision task—with words carefully selected to permit distinguishing lexical frequency, AoA, and orthographic ND effects—to 41 individuals with PPA (13 nfvPPA, 14 lvPPA, 14 svPPA) and 25 controls.

Results:

Of the psycholinguistic variables studied, lexical frequency had the largest influence on lexical-semantic processing, but AoA and ND also played an independent role. The results reflect a brain-language relationship with different proportional effects of frequency, AoA, and ND in the PPA variants, in a pattern that is consistent with the organization of the mental lexicon. Individuals with nfvPPA and lvPPA experienced an ND effect consistent with the role of inferior frontal and temporoparietal regions in lexical analysis and word form processing. By contrast, individuals with svPPA experienced an AoA effect consistent with the role of the anterior temporal lobe in semantic processing.

Conclusions:

The findings are in line with a hierarchical mental lexicon structure with a conceptual (semantic) and a lexeme (word-form) level, such that a selective deficit at one of these levels of the mental lexicon manifests differently in lexical-semantic processing performance, consistent with the affected language-specific brain region in each PPA variant.

Copyright

Corresponding author

*Correspondence and reprint requests to: Jet M. J. Vonk, Department of Neurology, Taub Institute for Research on Alzheimer’s Disease and the Aging Brain, Columbia University Medical Center, 630 W 168th St, P&S Box 16, New York, NY 10032-3784, USA, Tel: (212) 342 1399, E-mail: jv2528@cumc.columbia.edu

References

Hide All
Aguinis, H. (1995). Statistical power problems with moderated multiple regression in management research. Journal of Management, 21(6), 11411158.
Balota, D.A., Cortese, M.J., Sergent-Marshall, S.D., Spieler, D.H., & Yap, M. (2004). Visual word recognition of single-syllable words. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 133(2), 283316.
Balota, D.A., Yap, M.J., Hutchison, K.A., Cortese, M.J., Kessler, B., Loftis, B., Neely, J.H., Nelson, D.L., Simpson, G.B., & Treiman, R. (2007). The English lexicon project. Behavior Research Methods, 39(3), 445459.
Barry, C., Hirsh, K.W., Johnston, R.A., & Williams, C.L. (2001). Age of acquisition, word frequency, and the locus of repetition priming of picture naming. Journal of Memory and Language, 44(3), 350375.
Binney, R.J., Embleton, K.V., Jefferies, E., Parker, G.J., & Lambon Ralph, M.A. (2010). The ventral and inferolateral aspects of the anterior temporal lobe are crucial in semantic memory: evidence from a novel direct comparison of distortion-corrected fMRI, rTMS, and semantic dementia. Cerebral Cortex, 20(11), 27282738.
Bock, K. & Levelt, W. (1994). Language production: Grammatical encoding, In Gernsbacher, M.A. (Ed.), Handbook of psycholinguistics (pp. 945984). San Diego, CA: Academic Press.
Brown, G.D.A. & Watson, F.L. (1987). First in, first out: Word learning age and spoken word frequency as predictors of word familiarity and word naming latency. Memory & Cognition, 15(3), 208216.
Brysbaert, M. & Ghyselinck, M. (2006). The effect of age of acquisition: partly frequency related, partly frequency independent. Visual Cognition, 13(7–8), 9921011.
Brysbaert, M. & New, B. (2009). Moving beyond Kučera and Francis: a critical evaluation of current word frequency norms and the introduction of a new and improved word frequency measure for American English. Behavior Research Methods, 41(4), 977990.
Brysbaert, M., Stevens, M., De Deyne, S., Voorspoels, W., & Storms, G. (2014). Norms of age of acquisition and concreteness for 30,000 Dutch words. Acta Psychologica, 150, 8084.
Brysbaert, M., Van Wijnendaele, I., & De Deyne, S. (2000). Age-of-acquisition effects in semantic processing tasks. Acta Psychologica, 104(2), 215226.
Carreiras, M., Perea, M., & Grainger, J. (1997). Effects of orthographic neighborhood in visual word recognition: cross-task comparisons. Journal of Experimental Psychology-Learning Memory and Cognition, 23(4), 857871.
Cohen, J. (1992). A power primer. Psychological Bulletin, 112(1), 155159.
Coltheart, M., Davelaar, E., Jonasson, T., & Besner, D. (1977). Access to the internal lexicon, In Dornic, S. (Ed.), Attention and performance VI (pp. 535555). Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates.
Cortese, M.J. & Khanna, M.M. (2007). Age of acquisition predicts naming and lexical-decision performance above and beyond 22 other predictor variables: an analysis of 2,342 words. The Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 60(8), 10721082.
Cortese, M.J. & Schock, J. (2013). Imageability and age of acquisition effects in disyllabic word recognition. The Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 66(5), 946972.
Diesfeldt, H. (2011). The phonological variant of primary progressive aphasia, a single case study. Tijdschrift Gerontologie Geriatrie, 42(2), 7990.
Ferrand, L., New, B., Brysbaert, M., Keuleers, E., Bonin, P., Méot, A., Augustinova, M., & Pallier, C. (2010). The French Lexicon Project: lexical decision data for 38,840 French words and 38,840 pseudowords. Behavior Research Methods, 42(2), 488496.
Folstein, M.F., Folstein, S.E., & McHugh, P.R. (1975). “Mini-mental state”: a practical method for grading the cognitive state of patients for the clinician. Journal of Psychiatric Research, 12(3), 189198.
Gerhand, S. & Barry, C. (1998). Word frequency effects in oral reading are not merely age-of-acquisition effects in disguise. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 24(2), 267283.
Gerhand, S. & Barry, C. (1999). Age of acquisition, word frequency, and the role of phonology in the lexical decision task. Memory & Cognition, 27(4), 592602.
Gilhooly, K.J. & Logie, R.H. (1982). Word age-of-acquisition and lexical decision making. Acta Psychologica, 50(1), 2134.
Gorno-Tempini, M.L., Hillis, A.E., Weintraub, S., Kertesz, A., Mendez, M., Cappa, S.F., Ogar, J.M., Rohrer, J.D., Black, S., Boeve, B.F., Manes, F., Dronkers, N.F., Vandenberghe, R., Rascovsky, K., Patterson, K., Miller, B.L., Knopman, D.S., Hodges, J.R., Mesulam, M.M., & Grossman, M. (2011). Classification of primary progressive aphasia and its variants. Neurology, 76(11), 10061014.
Hirsh, K.W. & Funnell, E. (1995). Those old, familiar things: age of acquisition, familiarity and lexical access in progressive aphasia. Journal of Neurolinguistics, 9(1), 2332.
IBM Corp. (2016). IBM SPSS Statistics for Windows, Version 24. Armonk, NY: IBM Corp.
Keuleers, E. & Brysbaert, M. (2010). Wuggy: a multilingual pseudoword generator. Behavior Research Methods, 42(3), 627633.
Keuleers, E., Diependaele, K., & Brysbaert, M. (2010). Practice effects in large-scale visual word recognition studies: a lexical decision study on 14,000 Dutch mono-and disyllabic words and nonwords. Frontiers in Psychology, 1(174), 115.
Keuleers, E., Lacey, P., Rastle, K., & Brysbaert, M. (2012). The British Lexicon Project: lexical decision data for 28,730 monosyllabic and disyllabic English words. Behavior Research Methods, 44(1), 287304.
Kielar, A., Deschamps, T., Jokel, R., & Meltzer, J. (2018). Abnormal language-related oscillatory responses in primary progressive aphasia. NeuroImage: Clinical, 18, 560574.
Kremin, H., Perrier, D., De Wilde, M., Dordain, M., Le Bayon, A., Gatignol, P., Rabine, C., Corbineau, M., Lehoux, E., & Arabia, C. (2001). Factors predicting success in picture naming in Alzheimer’s disease and primary progressive aphasia. Brain and Cognition, 46(1), 180183.
Kuperman, V., Stadthagen-Gonzalez, H., & Brysbaert, M. (2012). Age-of-acquisition ratings for 30,000 English words. Behavior Research Methods, 44(4), 978990.
Laganaro, M., Croisier, M., Bagou, O., & Assal, F. (2012). Progressive apraxia of speech as a window into the study of speech planning processes. Cortex, 48(8), 963971.
Lambon Ralph, M.A., Sage, K., Green, H., Berthier, M.L., Maritnez Cuitin, M., Torralva, T., Manes, F., & Patterson, K. (2011). El-La: the impact of degraded semantic representations on knowledge of grammatical gender in semantic dementia. Acta Neuropsychologica, 9(2), 1151132.
Levelt, W.J.M., Roelofs, A., & Meyer, A.S. (1999). A theory of lexical access in speech production. Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 22(1), 138.
Lynch, C.A., Walsh, C., Blanco, A., Moran, M., Coen, R.F., Walsh, J.B., & Lawlor, B.A. (2006). The clinical dementia rating sum of box score in mild dementia. Dementia and Geriatric Cognitive Disorders, 21(1), 4043.
Macmillan, N.A. (2002). Signal detection theory, In Wixted, J. & Pashler, H. (Eds.), Stevens’ handbook of experimental psychology. Volume 4: Methodology in experimental psychology (3 ed., pp. 4390). New York, NY: John Wiley & Sons.
Marcotte, K., Graham, N.L., Black, S.E., Tang-Wai, D., Chow, T.W., Freedman, M., Rochon, E., & Leonard, C. (2014). Verb production in the nonfluent and semantic variants of primary progressive aphasia: the influence of lexical and semantic factors. Cognitive Neuropsychology, 31(7–8), 565583.
McClelland, G.H. & Judd, C.M. (1993). Statistical difficulties of detecting interactions and moderator effects. Psychological Bulletin, 114(2), 376.
Middleton, E.L. & Schwartz, M.F. (2010). Density pervades: an analysis of phonological neighbourhood density effects in aphasic speakers with different types of naming impairment. Cognitive Neuropsychology, 27(5), 401427.
Morris, J.C. (1993). The Clinical Dementia Rating (CDR): current version and scoring rules. Neurology, 43(11), 24122414.
Morrison, C.M., Ellis, A.W., & Quinlan, P.T. (1992). Age of acquisition, not word frequency, affects object naming, not object recognition. Memory & Cognition, 20(6), 705714.
Mummery, C.J., Patterson, K., Price, C.J., Ashburner, J., Frackowiak, R.S., & Hodges, J.R. (2000). A voxel‐based morphometry study of semantic dementia: relationship between temporal lobe atrophy and semantic memory. Annals of Neurology, 47(1), 3645.
Nilakantan, A.S., Voss, J.L., Weintraub, S., Mesulam, M.-M., & Rogalski, E.J. (2017). Selective verbal recognition memory impairments are associated with atrophy of the language network in non-semantic variants of primary progressive aphasia. Neuropsychologia, 100, 1017.
Nusbaum, H.C., Pisoni, D.B., & Davis, C.K. (1984). Sizing up the Hoosier mental lexicon: measuring the familiarity of 20,000 words. Research on Speech Perception Progress Report, 10(10), 357376.
Obler, L.K., Obermann, L., Samuels, I., & Albert, M.L. (1999). Written input to enhance comprehension in dementia of the Alzheimer’s type. Language and Communication in Old Age: Multidisciplinary Perspectives, 9, 63.
Patterson, K., Lambon Ralph, M.A., Jefferies, E., Woollams, A., Jones, R., Hodges, J.R., & Rogers, T.T. (2006). “Presemantic” cognition in semantic dementia: six deficits in search of an explanation. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 18(2), 169183.
Pobric, G., Jefferies, E., & Ralph, M.A.L. (2010). Amodal semantic representations depend on both anterior temporal lobes: evidence from repetitive transcranial magnetic stimulation. Neuropsychologia, 48(5), 13361342.
Pollatsek, A., Perea, M., & Binder, K.S. (1999). The effects of "neighborhood size" in reading and lexical decision. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 25(4), 11421158.
Roelofs, A., Meyer, A.S., & Levelt, W.J.M. (1996). Interaction between semantic and orthographic factors in conceptually driven naming: comment on Starreveld and La Heij (1995). Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 22, 246251.
Schneider, W., Eschman, A., & Zuccolotto, A. (2002). E-Prime User’s Guide. Pittsburgh, PA: Psychology Software Tools, Inc.
Sears, C.R., Hino, Y., & Lupker, S.J. (1995). Neighborhood size and neighborhood frequency effects in word recognition. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 21(4), 876900.
Shaywitz, S.E., Shaywitz, B.A., Pugh, K.R., Fulbright, R.K., Constable, R.T., Mencl, W.E., Shankweiler, D.P., Liberman, A.M., Skudlarski, P., Fletcher, J.M., Katz, L., Marchione, K.E., Lacadie, C., Gatenby, C., & Gore, J.C. (1998). Functional disruption in the organization of the brain for reading in dyslexia. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America, 95(5), 26362641.
Steyvers, M. & Tenenbaum, J.B. (2005). The large‐scale structure of semantic networks: statistical analyses and a model of semantic growth. Cognitive Science, 29(1), 4178.
Taylor, A. (2015). Standardised effect size in a mixed/multilevel model. Department of Psychology, Macquarie University. Retrieved from http://www.psy.mq.edu.au/psystat/documents/standardised_effect_size_in_mixed_ML_models.pdf
Treiman, R., Mullennix, J., Bijeljac-Babic, R., & Richmond-Welty, E.D. (1995). The special role of rimes in the description, use, and acquisition of English orthography. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 124(2), 107136.
Turner, J.E., Valentine, T., & Ellis, A.W. (1998). Contrasting effects of age of acquisition and word frequency on auditory and visual lexical decision. Memory & Cognition, 26(6), 12821291.
Vonk, J.M.J. (2017). Cognitive and neurobiological degeneration of the mental lexicon in primary progressive aphasia. (Doctoral Dissertation). New York, NY: The Graduate Center of the City University of New York.
Wilson, S.M., Brandt, T.H., Henry, M.L., Babiak, M., Ogar, J.M., Salli, C., Wilson, L., Peralta, K., Miller, B.L., & Gorno-Tempini, M.L. (2014). Inflectional morphology in primary progressive aphasia: an elicited production study. Brain and Language, 136, 5868.
Yarkoni, T., Balota, D., & Yap, M. (2008). Moving beyond Coltheart’s N: a new measure of orthographic similarity. Psychonomic Bulletin & Review, 15(5), 971979.
Zevin, J.D. & Seidenberg, M.S. (2002). Age of acquisition effects in word reading and other tasks. Journal of Memory and Language, 47(1), 129.
Zipf, G.K. (1935). The Psychobiology of Language. Boston, MA: Houghton Mifflin.

Keywords

Semantic and lexical features of words dissimilarly affected by non-fluent, logopenic, and semantic primary progressive aphasia

  • Jet M. J. Vonk (a1), Roel Jonkers (a2), H. Isabel Hubbard (a3), Maria Luisa Gorno-Tempini (a4), Adam M. Brickman (a1) and Loraine K. Obler (a5)...

Metrics

Altmetric attention score

Full text views

Total number of HTML views: 0
Total number of PDF views: 0 *
Loading metrics...

Abstract views

Total abstract views: 0 *
Loading metrics...

* Views captured on Cambridge Core between <date>. This data will be updated every 24 hours.

Usage data cannot currently be displayed