Skip to main content Accessibility help
×
Home

The influence of attention and age on the occurrence of mirror movements

  • YASMIN BALIZ (a1), CHRISTINE ARMATAS (a1), MAREE FARROW (a2), KATE E. HOY (a2) (a3), PAUL B. FITZGERALD (a3), JOHN L. BRADSHAW (a2) and NELLIE GEORGIOU-KARISTIANIS (a2)...

Abstract

This study utilised a finger force task to investigate the influence of attention and age on the occurrence of motor overflow in the form of mirror movements in neurologically intact adults. Forty right-handed participants were recruited from three age groups: 20–30 years, 40–50 years, and 60–70 years. Participants were required to maintain a target force using both their index and middle fingers, representing 50% of their maximum strength capacity for that hand. Attention was directed to a hand by activating a bone conduction vibrator attached to the small finger of that hand. Based on Cabeza's (2002) model of hemispheric asymmetry reduction in older adults, it was hypothesised that mirror movements would increase with age. Furthermore, it was expected that when the attentional demands of the task were increased, motor overflow occurrence would be exacerbated for the older adult group. The results obtained provide support for the model, and qualified support for the hypothesis that increasing the attentional demands of a task results in greater motor overflow. It is proposed that the association between mirror movements and age observed in this study may result from an age-related increase in bihemispheric activation that occurs in older adults, who, unlike younger adults, benefit from bihemispheric processing for task performance. (JINS, 2005, 11, 855–862.)

Copyright

Corresponding author

Address correspondence to: Dr. Nellie Georgiou-Karistianis, Experimental Neuropsychology Research Unit, Department of Psychology, Monash University, Victoria, 3800, Australia. E-mail: Nellie.Georgiou-Karistianis@med.monash.edu.au

References

Hide All

REFERENCES

Abercombie, M., Lindon, R., & Tyson, M. (1964). Associated movements in normal and physically handicapped children. Developmental Medicine and Child Neurology, 6, 573580.
Aboitiz, F., Scheibel, A.B., Fisher, R.S., & Zaidel, E. (1992). Fibre composition of the human corpus callosum. Brain Research, 598, 143153.
Allen, L.S., Richey, M.F., Chai, Y.M., & Gorski, R.A. (1991). Sex differences in the corpus callosum of the living human being. The Journal of Neuroscience, 11, 933942.
Aranyi, Z. & Rosler, K.M. (2002). Effort-induced mirror movements: A study of transcallosal inhibition in humans. Experimental Brain Research, 145, 7682.
Armatas, C.A., Summers, J.J., & Bradshaw, J.L. (1994). Mirror movements in normal adult subjects. Journal of Clinical and Experimental Neuropsychology, 16, 405413.
Armatas, C.A., Summers, J.J., & Bradshaw, J.L. (1996a). Strength as a factor influencing mirror movements. Human Movement Science, 15, 689705.
Armatas, C.A., Summers, J.J., & Bradshaw, J.L. (1996b). Handedness and performance variability as factors influencing mirror movement occurrence. Journal of Clinical and Experimental Neuropsychology, 18, 823835.
Bauman, G. (1932). Absence of the cervical spine: Klippel-Feil Syndrome. Journal of the American Medical Association, 98, 129132.
Beck, A.T., Ward, C.H., Mendelson, M., Mock, J., & Erbaugh, J. (1961). An inventory for measuring depression. Archives of General Psychiatry, 4, 5363.
Bodwell, J.A., Mahurin, R.K., Waddle, S., Price, R., & Cramer, S.C. (2003). Age and features of movement influence motor overflow. Journal of the American Geriatrics Society, 51, 17351739.
Cabeza, R. (2002). Hemispheric asymmetry reduction in older adults: The HAROLD model. Psychology and Aging, 17, 85100.
Calautti, C., Serrati, C., & Baron, J.C. (2001). Effects of age on brain activation during auditory-cued thumb-to-index opposition: A positron emission tomography study. Stroke, 32, 139146.
Cernacek, J. (1961). Contralateral motor irradiation—cerebral dominance. Archives of Neurology, 4, 6168.
Cheyne, D., Weinberg, H., Gaetz, W., & Jantzen, K.J. (1995). Motor cortex activity and predicting side of movement: Neuronal network and dipole of premovement magnetic fields. Neuroscience Letters, 188, 8184.
Cincotta, M., Borgheresi, A., Ragazzoni, P., Vanni, P., Balestrieri, F., Benvenuti, F., Zaccara, G., Arnetoli, G., & Ziemann, U. (2003). Separate ipsilateral and contralateral corticospinal projections in congenital mirror movements: Neurophysiological evidence and significance for motor rehabilitation. Movement Disorders, 18, 12941300.
Connolly, K. & Stratton, P. (1968). Developmental changes in associated movements. Developmental Medicine and Child Neurology, 10, 4956.
Cowell, P.E., Allen, L.S., Zalatimo, N.S., & Denenberg, V.H. (1992). A developmental study of sex and age interactions in the human corpus callosum. Developmental Brain Research, 66, 187192.
Craik, F.I.M. (1986). A functional account of age differences in memory. In F. Lix & H. Hagendord (Eds.), Human memory and cognitive capabilities, mechanisms, and performances (pp. 409422). Amsterdam: Elsevier Science.
Cramer, S.C., Finklestein, S.P., Schaechter, J.D., Bush, G., & Rosen, B.R. (1999). Activation of distinct motor cortex regions during ipsilateral and contralateral finger movements. Journal of Neurophysiology, 74, 383387.
de Lacoste-Utamsing, C. & Holloway, R.L. (1982). Sexual dimorphism in the human corpus callosum. Science, 216, 14311432.
Folstein, M.F., Folstein, S.E., & McHugh, P.R. (1975). Mini-mental state: A practical method for grading the cognitive state of patients for the clinician. Journal of Psychiatric Research, 12, 189198.
Georgiou-Karistianis, N., Hoy, K., Bradshaw, J., Farrow, M., Chiu, E., Churchyard, A., Fitzgerald, P., & Armatas, C. (2004). Motor overflow in Huntington's disease. Journal of Neurology, Neurosurgery, and Psychiatry, 75, 904906.
Heck, A.F. (1964). A study of neural and extraneural findings in a large family with Friedreich's ataxia. Journal of Neurological Sciences, 1, 226255.
Hoy, K., Fitzgerald, P., Bradshaw, J., Armatas, C., Farrow, M., Brown, T., & Georgiou-Karistianis, N. (2004a). Motor overflow in schizophrenia. Psychiatry Research, 125, 129137.
Hoy, K., Fitzgerald, P., Bradshaw, J., Armatas, C., & Georgiou-Karistianis, N. (2004b). Investigating the cortical origins of motor overflow. Brain Research Reviews, 46, 315327.
Ikeda, A., Luders, H.O., Burgess, R.C., & Shibasaki, H. (1992). Movement related potentials recorded from supplementary motor area and primary motor area. Brain, 115, 10171043.
La Mantia, A.S. & Rakic, P. (1990). Cytological and quantitative characteristics of four cerebral commissures in the rhesus monkey. Journal of Comparative Neurology, 291, 520537.
Liederman, J. & Foley, L.M. (1987). A modified finger lift test reveals an asymmetry of motor overflow in adults. Journal of Clinical and Experimental Neuropsychology, 9, 498510.
Maegaki, Y., Seki, A., Suzaki, I., Sugihara, S., Ogawa, T., Amisaki, T., Fukuda, C., & Koeda, T. (2002). Congenital mirror movement: A study of functional MRI and transcranial magnetic stimulation. Developmental Medicine and Child Neurology, 44, 838843.
Mattay, V.S., Fera, F., Tessitore, A., Hariri, A.R., Das, S., Callicott, J.H., & Weingerger, D.R. (2002). Neurophysiological correlates of age-related changes in human motor function. Neurology, 58, 630635.
Mayston, M.J., Harrison, L.M., & Stephens, J.A. (1999). A neurophysiological study of mirror movements in adults and children. Annals of Neurology, 45, 583594.
Nakada, T., Fujii, Y., Suzuki, K., & Kwee, I.L. (1998). High-field (3.0T) functional MRI sequential epoch analysis: An example for motion control analysis. Neuroscience Research, 32, 355362.
Nass, R. (1985). Mirror movement asymmetries in congenital hemiparesis: The inhibition hypothesis revisited. Neurology, 35, 10591062.
Oldfield, R.C. (1971). The assessment and analysis of handedness: The Edinburgh Inventory. Neuropsychologia, 9, 97113.
Regli, F., Filippa, G., & Wiesendanger, M. (1967). Hereditary mirror movements. Archives of Neurology, 16, 620623.
Reitz, M. & Muller, K. (1998). Differences between “congenital mirror movements” and “associated movements” in normal children: A neurophysiological case study. Neuroscience Letters, 256, 6972.
Sohn, Y.H., Jung, H.Y., & Kaelin-Lang, A. (2003). Excitability of the ipsilateral motor cortex during phasic voluntary hand movement. Experimental Brain Research, 148, 176185.
Somers, A.B., Levin, H.S., & Hannay, H.J. (1976). A neuropsychological study of a family with hereditary mirror movements. Developmental Medicine and Child Neurology, 18, 791798.
Stern, J.A., Gold, S., Hoine, H., & Barocas, V.S. (1976). Towards a more refined analysis of the overflow or associated movement phenomenon. In D.V.S. Sankar (Ed.), Mental Health in Children, Vol. 11 (pp. 113128). New York: PJD Publications Ltd.
Todor, J.I. & Lazarus, J.C. (1986). Exertion level and the intensity of associated movements. Developmental Medicine and Child Neurology, 28, 205212.
Tomasch, J. (1954). Size, distribution and number of fibres in the human corpus callosum. The Anatomical Record, 119, 119135.
van den Berg, C., Beek, P.J., Waganarr, R.C., & van Wieringen, P.C.W. (2000). Coordination disorders in patients with Parkinson's disease: A study of paced rhythmic forearm movements. Experimental Brain Research, 134, 174186.
Witelson, S.F. (1985). The brain connection: The corpus callosum is larger in left-handers. Science, 229, 665668.
Yensen, R. (1965). A factor influencing motor overflow. Perceptual and Motor Skills, 20, 967968.
Ziemann, U., Ishii, K., Borgheresi, A., Yaseen, Z., Battaglia, F., Hallet, M., Cincotta, M., Wassermann, E.M. (1999). Dissociation of the pathways mediating ipsilateral and contralateral motor evoked potentials in human hand and arm muscles. Journal of Physiology (London), 518, 895906.

Keywords

The influence of attention and age on the occurrence of mirror movements

  • YASMIN BALIZ (a1), CHRISTINE ARMATAS (a1), MAREE FARROW (a2), KATE E. HOY (a2) (a3), PAUL B. FITZGERALD (a3), JOHN L. BRADSHAW (a2) and NELLIE GEORGIOU-KARISTIANIS (a2)...

Metrics

Full text views

Total number of HTML views: 0
Total number of PDF views: 0 *
Loading metrics...

Abstract views

Total abstract views: 0 *
Loading metrics...

* Views captured on Cambridge Core between <date>. This data will be updated every 24 hours.

Usage data cannot currently be displayed