Hostname: page-component-76fb5796d-5g6vh Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-04-25T07:10:48.669Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

Evaluation of Specific Executive Functioning Skills and the Processes Underlying Executive Control in Schizophrenia

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  10 November 2010

Gauri N. Savla*
Affiliation:
Department of Psychiatry, University of California, San Diego, La Jolla, California
Elizabeth W. Twamley
Affiliation:
Department of Psychiatry, University of California, San Diego, La Jolla, California Center of Excellence for Stress and Mental Health, Psychiatry Service, VA San Diego Healthcare System, San Diego, California
Wesley K. Thompson
Affiliation:
Department of Psychiatry, University of California, San Diego, La Jolla, California The Sam and Rose Stein Center for Research on Aging, University of California School of Medicine, La Jolla, California
Dean C. Delis
Affiliation:
Department of Psychiatry, University of California, San Diego, La Jolla, California
Dilip V. Jeste
Affiliation:
Department of Psychiatry, University of California, San Diego, La Jolla, California The Sam and Rose Stein Center for Research on Aging, University of California School of Medicine, La Jolla, California
Barton W. Palmer
Affiliation:
Department of Psychiatry, University of California, San Diego, La Jolla, California
*
Correspondence and reprint requests to: Gauri N. Savla, PhD, University of California, San Diego, Department of Psychiatry MC 0603 V, 9500 Gilman Drive, La Jolla, California 92093-0603. E-mail: gsavla@ucsd.edu

Abstract

Schizophrenia is associated with executive dysfunction. Yet, the degree to which executive functions are impaired differentially, or above and beyond underlying basic cognitive processes is less clear. Participants included 145 matched pairs of individuals with schizophrenia (SCs) and normal comparison subjects (NCs). Executive functions were assessed with 10 tasks of the Delis-Kaplan Executive Function System (D-KEFS), in terms of “achievement scores” reflecting overall performance on the task. Five of these tasks (all measuring executive control) were further examined in terms of their basic component (e.g., processing speed) scores and contrast scores (reflecting residual higher order skills adjusted for basic component skills). Group differences were examined via multivariate analysis of variance. SCs had worse performance than NCs on all achievement scores, but the greatest SC-NC difference was that for the Trails Switching task. SCs also had worse performance than NCs on all basic component skills. Of the executive control tasks, only Trails Switching continued to be impaired after accounting for impairments in underlying basic component skills. Much of the impairment in executive functions in schizophrenia may reflect the underlying component skills rather than higher-order functions. However, the results from one task suggest that there might be additional impairment in some aspects of executive control. (JINS, 2010, 17, 000–000)

Type
Research Articles
Copyright
Copyright © The International Neuropsychological Society 2010

Access options

Get access to the full version of this content by using one of the access options below. (Log in options will check for institutional or personal access. Content may require purchase if you do not have access.)

References

Atbasoglu, E.C., Ozguven, H.D., Saka, M.C., Olmez, S. (2005). Relative sparing of executive functions in the early phase of schizophrenia. The Journal of Neuropsychiatry and Clinical Neurosciences, 17, 510516.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Baade, L.E., Schoenberg, M.R. (2004). A proposed method to estimate premorbid intelligence utilizing group achievement measures from school records. Archives of Clinical Neuropsychology, 19, 227243.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Barch, D.M., Braver, T.S., Carter, C.S., Poldrack, R.A., Robbins, T.W. (2009). CNTRICS final task selection: Executive control. Schizophrenia Bulletin, 35, 115135.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Barkley, R.A. (2001). The executive functions and self-regulation: An evolutional neuropsychological perspective. Neuropsychology Review, 11, 128.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Braff, D.L., Heaton, R., Kuck, J., Cullum, M., Moranville, J., Grant, I., Zisook, S. (1991). The generalized pattern of neuropsychological deficits in outpatients with chronic schizophrenia with heterogeneous Wisconsin Card Sorting Test Results. Archives of General Psychiatry, 48, 891898.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Cameron, N. (1939). Schizophrenic thinking in a problem-solving situation. Journal of Mental Science, 85, 10121035.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Chapman, L.J., Chapman, J.P. (1973). Problems in the measurement of cognitive deficits. Psychological Bulletin, 79, 380385.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Cohen, J. (1988). Statistical power analysis for the behavioral sciences. Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates, Inc.Google Scholar
Delis, D.C., Kaplan, E., Kramer, J.H. (2001). Delis-Kaplan Executive Function Scale (D-KEFS): Examiner’s manual. San Antonio, TX: The Psychological Corporation.Google Scholar
Dickinson, D., Ramsey, M.E., Gold, J.M. (2007). Overlooking the obvious: A meta-analytic comparison of Digit Symbol Coding tasks and other cognitive measures in schizophrenia. Archives of General Psychiatry, 64, 532542.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Evans, J.D., Heaton, R.K., Paulsen, J.S., McAdams, L.A., Heaton, S.C., Jeste, D.V. (1999). Schizoaffective disorder: A form of schizophrenia or affective disorder? Journal of Clinical Psychiatry, 60, 874882.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Golden, C.J., Freshwater, S.M. (2002). Stroop color and word test: A manual for clinical and experimental uses (2nd ed.). IL: Stoelting Co.Google Scholar
Goldstein, K. (1959). Concerning the concreteness in schizophrenia. Journal of Abnormal and Social Psychology, 59, 146148.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Green, M.F. (1996). What are the functional consequences of neurocognitive deficits in schizophrenia? American Journal of Psychiatry, 153, 321330.Google ScholarPubMed
Green, M.F., Kern, R.S., Braff, D.L., Mintz, J. (2000). Neurocognitive deficits and functional outcome in schizophrenia: Are we measuring the “right stuff”? Schizophrenia Bulletin, 26, 119136.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Grober, E., Sliwinski, M. (1991). Development and validation of a model for estimating premorbid verbal intelligence in the elderly. Journal of Clinical and Experimental Neuropsychology, 13, 933949.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Hamilton, M. (1967). Development of a rating scale for primary depressive illness. The British Journal of Social and Clinical Psychology, 6, 278296.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Kay, S.R., Opler, L.A., Fiszbein, A. (1987). Positive and Negative Syndrome Scale (PANSS) rating manual. San Rafael, CA: Social and Behavioral Sciences Documents.Google Scholar
Kraepelin, E. (1971). Dementia praecox and paraphrenia [Translated in 1919 from the eighth German edition of the ‘Test-book of Psychiatry,’ vol. iii, part ii, section on Endogenous Dementias published in 1913]. Huntington, NY: Krieger.Google Scholar
Kremen, W.S., Seidman, L.J., Faraone, S.V., Tsuang, M.T. (2001). Intelligence quotient and neuropsychological profiles in patients with schizophrenia and in normal volunteers. Biological Psychiatry, 50, 453462.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Lichter, D.G., Cummings, J.L. (2001). Introduction and overview. In D.G. Lichter & J.L. Cummings (Eds.), Frontal-subcortical circuits in psychiatric and neurological disorders (pp. 143). New York: Guilford Press.Google Scholar
Luria, A.R. (1966). Higher cortical functions in man [B. Haigh Trans]. New York: Basic Books.Google Scholar
Moore, D.J., Savla, G.N., Woods, S.P., Jeste, D.V., Palmer, B.W. (2006). Verbal fluency impairments among middle-aged and older outpatients with schizophrenia are characterized by deficient switching. Schizophrenia Research, 87, 254260.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Painter, J.S. (2010). SPSS propensity matching program description. Retrieved from www.unc.edu/~painterGoogle Scholar
Palmer, B.W., Dawes, S.E., Heaton, R.K. (2009). What do we know about the neuropsychological aspects of schizophrenia? Neuropsychology Review, 19, 365384.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Palmer, B.W., Savla, G.N. (2007). The association of specific neuropsychological deficits on capacity to consent to research or treatment. Journal of the International Neuropsychological Society, 13, 10471059.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Palmer, B.W., Savla, G.N. (2009). Neuropsychological deficits in schizoaffective disorder. In: K.S. Yip (Ed.), Schizoaffective disorders: International perspectives on understanding, intervention and rehabilitation. New York: Nova Science.Google Scholar
Perlstein, W.M., Carter, C.S., Barch, D.M., Baird, J.W. (1998). The Stroop task and attention deficits in schizophrenia: A critical evaluation of card and single-trial Stroop methodologies. Neuropsychology, 12, 414425.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Reitan, R.M., Wolfson, D. (1993). The Halstead-Reitan neuropsychological test battery: Theory and clinical interpretation (2nd ed.). Tucson, AZ: Neuropsychology Press.Google Scholar
Rosenbaum, P.R., Rubin, D.B. (1983). The central role of the propensity score in observational studies for causal effects. Biometrika, 70, 4155.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Royall, D.R., Lauterback, E.C., Cummings, J.L., Reeve, A., Rummans, T.A., Kaufer, D.I., Coffey, C.E. (2002). Executive control function: A review of its promise and challenges for clinical research: A report from the Committee on Research of the American Neuropsychiatric Association. The Journal of Neuropsychiatry and Clinical Neurosciences, 14, 377405.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Saykin, A.J., Gur, R.C., Gur, R.E., Mozley, P.D., Mozley, L.H., Resnick, S.M., Stafiniak, P. (1991). Neuropsychological function in schizophrenia: Selective impairment in memory and learning. Archives of General Psychiatry, 48, 618624.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Sheehan, D.V., Lecrubier, Y., Sheehan, K.H. (1998). The Mini-International Neuropsychiatric Interview (M.I.N.I.): The development of a structured diagnostic psychiatric interview for DSM-IV and ICD-10. The Journal of Clinical Psychiatry, 59, 2233.Google ScholarPubMed
Sullivan, J.R., Riccio, C.A., Castillo, C.L. (2009). Concurrent validity of the tower tasks as measures of executive function in adults: A meta-analysis. Applied Neuropsychology, 16, 6275.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Velligan, D.I., Bow-Thomas, C.C. (1999). Executive function in schizophrenia. Seminars in Clinical Neuropsychiatry, 4, 2433.Google ScholarPubMed
Wechsler, D. (1999). Wechsler Abbreviated Scale of Intelligence (WASI). San Antonio, TX: Psychological Corporation.Google Scholar
Wechsler, D. (2008). Wechsler Adult Intelligence Scale-Fourth Edition (WAIS-IV). San Antonio, TX: Pearson, Inc.Google Scholar
Zec, R.F. (1995). Neuropsychology of schizophrenia according to Kraeplin: Disorders of volition and executive functioning. European Archives of Psychiatry and Clinical Neuroscience, 245, 216223.CrossRefGoogle Scholar